Abstract
Objective
To evaluate the effect of an antismoking advertisement on young people's perceptions of smoking in movies and their intention to smoke.
Subjects/setting
3091 cinema patrons aged 12–24 years in three Australian states; 18.6% of the sample (n = 575) were current smokers.
Design/intervention
Quasi‐experimental study of patrons, surveyed after having viewed a movie. The control group was surveyed in week 1, and the intervention group in weeks 2 and 3. Before seeing the movie in weeks 2 and 3, a 30 s antismoking advertisement was shown, shot in the style of a movie trailer that warned patrons not to be sucked in by the smoking in the movie they were about to see.
Outcomes
Attitude of current smokers and non‐smokers to smoking in the movies; intention of current smokers and non‐smokers to smoke in 12 months.
Results
Among non‐smokers, 47.8% of the intervention subjects thought that the smoking in the viewed movie was not OK compared with 43.8% of the control subjects (p = 0.04). However, there was no significant difference among smokers in the intervention (16.5%) and control (14.5%) groups (p = 0.4). A higher percentage of smokers in the intervention group indicated that they were likely to be smoking in 12 months time (38.6%) than smokers in the control group (25.6%; p<0.001). For non‐smokers, there was no significant difference in smoking intentions between groups, with 1.2% of intervention subjects and 1.6% of controls saying that they would probably be smoking in 12 months time (p = 0.54).
Conclusions
This real‐world study suggests that placing an antismoking advertisement before movies containing smoking scenes can help to immunise non‐smokers against the influences of film stars' smoking. Caution must be exercised in the type of advertisement screened as some types of advertising may reinforce smokers' intentions to smoke.
In the past decade, tobacco companies have devised increasingly innovative and aggressive strategies for attracting consumers.1,2 Product placement in films popular with young people has been the focus of comment and criticism by numerous international health groups.
Depictions of smoking are common in films3 and have decreased in recent decades.4 Sargent et al5 documented an overall increase in the depiction of smoking in films in the 1990s that seemed to coincide with restrictions in advertising.6 Lead characters portrayed as smokers are often likeable, rebellious, attractive and/or successful.7 Role models with such characteristics are often used in tobacco advertising.8 Escamilla et al9 analysed the portrayal of smoking in Hollywood films and found that smoking was highly prevalent in films featuring popular actresses.10 McIntosh et al11 compared Hollywood's depiction of smokers to real‐world demographics on smoking and found that smoking scenes in movies tend to ignore the negative consequences of smoking, a finding confirmed by Dalton et al12 in 2002.
There is mounting evidence linking Hollywood's depiction of smoking in movies and adolescents' attitudes to smoking and their smoking behaviour. Tickle et al13 showed that adolescents whose favourite movie stars use tobacco on screen are significantly more likely to be at a more advanced stage of smoking uptake and to have more favourable attitudes towards smoking than adolescents who choose non‐smoking stars. Studies14,15,16,17,18 provide even stronger evidence that viewing smoking in movies promotes smoking initiation among adolescents. A cohort study by Dalton et al19 in 2003 suggests that viewing smoking in movies strongly predicts whether or not adolescents initiate smoking and the effect increases significantly with greater exposure.
Prominent researchers and public health advocates have called for action to reduce the impact of positive depictions of smoking in the media, including feature films screened in cinemas.20
A Californian study21 suggested that young people can be immunised against the influences of film stars smoking by showing a strong antismoking advertisement before those films that contain smoking scenes. A 2004 Australian study22 supported these findings. The findings of Pechmann and Shih21 and Edwards et al22 support the psychological Theory of Reasoned Action,23,24 which states that the strength of a person's intention to behave in a certain way is a function of attitudes towards the behaviour and the influence of general subjective norms on the behaviour. According to this theory, an antismoking advertisement may alter the positive attitudes towards smoking that are portrayed in movies and elicit more realistic normative perceptions of the practice of smoking. This should theoretically alter the viewer's intention to smoke and subsequently reduce their likelihood of smoking in the future. The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion25 suggests that attitude change can be either via the central route that utilises deliberate information processing to assess an issue or via the peripheral route that takes less effort and may even be subliminal. This model predicts that smoking scenes in movies influence young people via the peripheral route. An antismoking advertisement attempts to change attitudes through the central route that, according to the theory, is more enduring and more likely to lead to long‐term behavioural change.
Cinema attendance and viewing films rated R under the US classification system increases exposure to onscreen depictions of smoking.26 The majority of young people, including those of varied cultural background, attend the cinema on a regular basis.27,28 A significant advantage in using this medium for an antismoking campaign is the potential to reach a large number of young people in a cost‐effective manner.21, 29
This paper evaluates the use of this approach in an intervention conducted in a real‐world cinema setting in Australia. The objective of the study was to evaluate the effect of an antismoking advertisement on young people's perceptions of smoking in the following movie and their intention to smoke. It was hypothesised that when an antismoking advertisement is shown before a movie containing smoking scenes, viewers will be (1) less likely to approve of the smoking and (2) less likely to report an intention to smoke in the future. This study expands on the first real‐world cinema study of the effect of an antismoking advertisement on attitudes to smoking in movies and intention to smoke conducted by Edwards et al in 2004. It samples a larger, more geographically and culturally diverse population of both males and females with a broader age range. It also evaluates a very different type of antismoking advertisement that does not include the health effects of smoking or a quit message.
Method
Study design
This quasi‐experimental controlled study of 12–24‐year‐olds was conducted in cinemas in the Australian states of New South Wales (NSW), South Australia (SA) and Australian Capital Territory (ACT) during a period of 3 weeks in January 2005. Young people were surveyed on leaving the movie. The movies seen by the intervention and control groups were identical. The control group was surveyed during week 1, and the intervention group during weeks 2 and 3. A 30 s antismoking advertisement was shown before the movie in weeks 2 and 3.
Subjects
Of the 3850 young people who were approached and who appeared to be aged between 12 and 24 years, 3100 completed questionnaires and 750 refused, providing a response rate of 80.5%. A total of 9 questionnaires were discarded because the respondents were outside the age range, leaving a sample size of 3091. Most refusals gave “in a hurry” or “couldn't be bothered” as reasons for not wanting to participate. Cinema complexes were chosen according to the attendance numbers of patrons in the targeted age range. Patrons were surveyed on Thursdays, Fridays and Saturdays as new movies begin on Thursdays and the majority of young people attend the cinema on these days.
Materials
Antismoking advertisement
A new advertisement was developed for use in the campaign.
The successful concept was specific to the issue of smoking in movies. It showed action on a film set and was shot in the style of a movie trailer. The advertisement did not contain specific health or cessation messages or images. The voice over said:
In a world where tobacco giants crave fresh blood, a new menace lurks.
Smoking in Movies. Behind all the glamour, behind all the gloss it's
waiting to suck you in. Don't be fooled by the movie you are about to see.
Don't be a sucker.
Preproduction testing with the target age group (a total of 30 respondents) did not identify any problems with the concept or delivery of the message.
Movies
“Screenit”, an internet film review site providing content information about films,30 was used to identify movies due for upcoming release in the Australian school holidays that would appeal to young people and depicted smoking. In the review, the level of smoking was recorded. Relevant movies were selected for the study, based on overseas trends of attendance of young people aged 12–24 years and inclusion in the cast of a star with appeal to young people. Closer31 and Alfie32 were the only two movies that fitted the above criteria during the timeframe allocated for the study. Closer contained mild smoking and Alfie contained heavy smoking.
Questionnaire
The one‐page questionnaire asked respondents what movie they had seen, whether there was any smoking in the movie and, if “yes”, which characters smoked. They were then asked about their opinion of smoking in the movie—“was it OK the character/s were smoking?” A five‐point Likert scale was provided. Respondents could choose from “definitely not OK”, “somewhat not OK”, “no opinion”, “somewhat OK” and “definitely OK”. Responses were subsequently collapsed into “not OK”, “no opinion” and “OK”. This was necessary because the small percentage of smokers in the sample meant that some cells were too small to analyse using all responses. For the binary logistic regression, “not OK” was compared with “OK”.
Respondents were also asked about their current smoking status—“have you smoked cigarettes in the last 4 weeks?” and their intention to smoke—“do you think you will be smoking cigarettes this time next year?” A seven‐point Likert scale was provided that included “certain not to be smoking”, “very unlikely to be smoking”, “unlikely to be smoking”, “can't decide how likely”, “likely to be smoking”, “very likely to be smoking” and “certain to be smoking”. Responses were subsequently collapsed into “likely to be smoking”, “can't decide how likely” and “unlikely to be smoking” because of small cell sizes. For the binary logistic regression, “likely to be smoking” was compared with “unlikely to be smoking”.
Procedure
As patrons were about to enter their chosen movie, they were given a flyer inviting them to complete a short, written questionnaire after viewing the movie. Patrons in NSW and SA were informed that they would receive a free lip gloss if they participated. Provision of an incentive was not permitted in ACT under ACT Health Ethics Guidelines. After the screening of the target movies, all audience members who appeared to be aged 12–24 years were approached as they exited the cinema. After their age was confirmed, they were invited to complete the questionnaire.
Statistical analysis
SPSS for Windows V.10.1.3 was used to analyse the data. To compare the intervention and control groups with respect to subject characteristics, recognition of smoking in the movie, approval of the smoking in the movie and intention to smoke in the future, χ2 tests were used. Binomial logistic regression was used to compare the intervention and control groups with respect to approval of smoking in the movie and intention to smoke, while adjusting for variables that were not evenly distributed across groups.
Results
Characteristics of patrons
Of the total sample of 3091 subjects, 1743 (56.4%) had viewed a movie with the antismoking advertisement (intervention group) and 1348 (43.6%) had viewed a movie without the advertisement (control group). Respondents' ages ranged from 12 to 24 years with a mean (SD) age of 16.5 (2.19) years and a median of 16 years. In all, 19% reported having smoked cigarettes in the past 4 weeks. Table 1 compares the characteristics of intervention and control groups finding differences in relation to gender, age group, movie viewed and language spoken at home, but not for smoking status or percentage of non‐responders.
Table 1 Characteristics of intervention and control groups.
Control, n (%) | Intervention, n (%) | p Value | |
---|---|---|---|
N | 1348 (43.6) | 1743 (56.4) | |
Age (years) | 0.04* | ||
12–17 | 931 (69.1) | 1141 (65.5) | |
18–24 | 417 (30.9) | 602 (34.5) | |
Gender | <0.001* | ||
Male | 204 (15.2) | 366 (21.0) | |
Female | 1141 (84.8) | 1375 (79.0) | |
Current smokers | 0.66 | ||
Yes | 245 (18.2) | 327 (18.8) | |
No | 1100 (81.8) | 1409 (81.2) | |
Movie | <0.001* | ||
Alfie | 1135 (84.2) | 811 (46.5) | |
Closer | 213 (15.8) | 932 (53.5) | |
Language spoken at home | <0.001* | ||
English | 1189 (88.2) | 1623 (93.1) | |
Other | 159 (11.8) | 120 (6.9) | |
Non‐responders | 352 (20.7) | 398 (18.6) | 0.1 |
*Denotes a significant difference in the distribution between control and intervention groups compared to the sample as a whole.
Recall of the antismoking advertisement
More than half the subjects in the intervention group (51.7%) could recall seeing an antismoking advertisement when surveyed immediately after the movie.
Recall of smoking in the movie
In all, 98% of both control and intervention subjects recalled characters smoking in the movie regardless of which movie they saw. All those who recalled the smoking could correctly name (or describe) at least one character who was smoking without being prompted. Two subjects also incorrectly identified characters who were not smoking.
Opinion of smoking in the movie
Univariate analysis showed that opinion of smoking in the movie was significantly related to current smoking status, age group and gender but not to the movie viewed. After adjusting for differences in age group and gender, the antismoking advertisement had an overall effect on opinion of smoking in the movie, with significantly more subjects indicating that the smoking in the movie was “not OK”, if they saw the antismoking advertisement before the movie (Wald χ2 = 5.83, df = 1, p = 0.016). Because there was a significantly lower percentage of smokers than non‐smokers indicating disapproval of the smoking in the movie (χ2 = 308.58, df = 2, p<0.001), the effect of the intervention on approval was analysed separately for smokers and non‐smokers.
As shown in table 2, univariate analysis found that among non‐smokers, those in the intervention group were significantly more likely to say that the smoking was “not OK”, with 47.8% of the intervention group saying that the smoking was “not OK”, compared with 43.8% of the control group (χ2 = 6.39, df = 2, p = 0.041). After adjusting for differences in gender and age group, there were significantly more non‐smoking intervention respondents who said that the smoking in the movie was “not OK”, compared with non‐smoking control respondents (Wald χ2 = 8.05, df = 1, p = 0.005).
Table 2 Approval of smoking in the movie for smokers and non‐smokers by group.
Non‐smokers (%) | Smokers (%) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Control n = 1092 | Intervention n = 1402 | Control n = 242 | Intervention n = 321 | |
Not OK | 43.8 | 47.8 | 14.5 | 16.5 |
No opinion | 28.9 | 29.1 | 24.8 | 20.9 |
OK | 27.3 | 23.1 | 60.7 | 62.6 |
Univariate analysis showed that among smokers, there was no significant difference between groups in relation to the level of approval of smoking in the movie (χ2 = 1.39, df = 2, p = 0.4) although the intervention group showed a higher percentage of disapproval than the control group (table 2). After adjusting for age group and gender differences, there was still no significant difference between smokers' level of approval by group (Wald χ2 = 0.249, df = 1, p = 0.618).
Intention to smoke
Univariate analysis showed that intention to smoke in 12 months was significantly related to current smoking status, age group and gender but not to the movie viewed. After adjusting for differences in age group and gender, there was an overall significant effect of the intervention on intention to smoke (Wald χ2 = 9.386, df = 1, p = 0.002). Surprisingly, a higher percentage of the intervention group said they were likely to be smoking in 12 months (table 3). As a significantly higher percentage of smokers than non‐smokers indicated they would probably be smoking (χ2 = 1154.9, df = 3, p<0.001), a separate analysis for smokers and non‐smokers was undertaken. After adjusting for age and gender, there was a significantly higher percentage of current smokers who said they were likely to be smoking in 12 months in the intervention group than in the control group (Wald χ2 = 9.01, df = 1, p = 0.003; table 3). However, there was no difference between groups in smoking intentions among non‐smokers (Wald χ2 = 0.436, df = 1, p = 0.509).
Table 3 Intention to smoke among smokers and non‐smokers by group.
Non‐smokers (%) | Smokers (%) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Control n = 1031 | Intervention n = 1338 | Control n = 167 | Intervention n = 236 | |
Unlikely to be smoking | 93.9 | 95.5 | 43.4 | 34.3 |
No opinion | 4.9 | 2.9 | 31.0 | 27.2 |
Likely to be smoking | 1.2 | 1.6 | 25.6 | 38.6 |
Discussion
This study supported the hypothesis that when an antismoking advertisement is shown before a movie with smoking, the smoking in the movie is less likely to be perceived as justified; however, this finding was only significant among non‐smokers. Edwards et al showed similar results in her 2004 Australian study. This also supports the findings of Pechmann and Shih21 that 9th grade students who saw an antismoking advertisement tended to elicit negative thoughts about smoking and smokers. There is strong evidence that youth's perceptions of smokers are highly predictive of their smoking behaviours.33,34,35
The present study did not support the second hypothesis that an antismoking advertisement shown before a movie with smoking scenes would result in lower intentions to smoke in the future. The finding that a higher proportion of smokers in the intervention group reported they were likely to be smoking in the future suggests that the antismoking advertisement could have in fact increased stated intentions to smoke. It is unclear what effect this would have on actual future behaviour as some studies have shown that, among adolescents, current smokers stated intention to smoke is not a good predictor of future smoking status.36,37
The unexpected negative effect was investigated with focus group testing of the advertisement, but the cause remains unclear. If this finding is confirmed in future field studies, it could have important ramifications for the type of antismoking advertisements used to target young people. Screening an antismoking advertisement neutralised the effect of the smoking on intention to smoke in the Pechmann 1999 study. The study by Edwards et al22 showed a significant decrease in intention to smoke for the 12–17‐year‐old female smokers who viewed the antismoking advertisement. The studies by Pechmann and Shih21 and Edwards et al22 used advertisements from previous antismoking campaigns that had been extensively evaluated at a state and national level, respectively, and had scored well with young audiences. Both the advertisements contained graphic messages related to the long‐term health effects of smoking with a quit‐smoking message. The present study evaluated a new advertisement that highlighted the deceptive conduct of the tobacco industry in glamourising smoking in movies. There was no health or quit‐smoking message attached to the advertisement. Wakefield et al38 suggest that advertisements that graphically portray the adverse consequences of smoking often rate highly among teens. Highlighting the deceptive and misleading conduct of the tobacco industry typically requires a more sophisticated target audience with additional experience in understanding these messages. A recent study by Pechmann and Reibling39 suggested that “sponsors of tobacco use prevention ad campaigns should consider using ads showing tobacco‐related disease and suffering, not just counter‐industry ads”. Pechmann and Reibling found that counter‐industry and industry advertisements did not significantly lower smoking intention.
Almost all non‐smokers in both groups said they did not intend to smoke in the future, which left little likelihood of detecting a reduction in intention to smoke. Although we did not detect a reduction, there was no evidence that the intervention had the unintended effect of enhancing smoking intentions among non‐smokers. Australian and US research has found that stated intention among non‐smokers to become smokers is a good predictor of smoking uptake.40,41,42,43
The extremely high recall of characters smoking in the movie was unrelated to seeing the antismoking advertisement before the movie or which movie the respondents viewed. The study by Edwards et al22 reported an almost identical finding using five different movies. The antismoking advertisement cannot be accused of drawing attention to the smoking in the movie, because young people recall the smoking in any case. However, this study supports previous evidence that antismoking advertisements can counter young non‐smokers' perceptions of the smoking content as justified. This is a strong argument for using antismoking advertisements to counter the influence of smoking in movies.
The study design had three major limitations. Firstly, because the study was conducted in real movie theatres, it was not possible to randomly allocate cinema‐goers to control or intervention groups. However, variables that significantly differed between control and intervention groups were identified and multivariate analyses were used to adjust for these differences. Furthermore, analyses were stratified by smoking status.
Secondly, we did not collect baseline information on approval of smoking in movies or intention to smoke. In this study, it was judged that collection of these data would prime subjects (especially control group subjects) to smoking in movies. Additionally, matching of baseline and post‐movie questionnaires in this setting would have been extremely difficult.
Thirdly, the opportunistic nature of the sample raises the issue of representativeness. The study attempted to survey every young person leaving the selected movies who appeared to be aged between 12 and 24 years, but there is no way of identifying or describing those cinema‐goers who may have been missed. Australian research24 indicates that 94% of young Australians had attended the cinema in the past 12 months and 56% had attended in the past 4 weeks. An Australian school holiday period was chosen to optimise the likelihood of attaining a large representative sample of young cinema‐goers. The sample is strongly skewed towards females who speak English at home, with 81% of the sample being female and 91% speaking English at home. This may be a result of the types of movies that were chosen.
What this paper adds
This study is only the second study to measure the effect of an antismoking advertisement in the real world of the cinema using a control group.
Results of the first study showed that the Australian National Tobacco Campaign's “Tar/Lung” advertisement, when shown before movies with smoking in them, could alter 12–17‐year‐old female non‐smokers' opinion of the smoking in the movie and significantly alter smokers' intention to smoke. A higher percentage of current smokers in the intervention group indicated that they were unlikely to smoke in 12 months time than smokers in the control group.
This second study evaluates the effect of a very different style of antismoking advertisement using the same methodology and a much broader target group. The non‐randomised control study measured the effect of the New South Wales Cancer Council's “Smoking in Movies” advertisement on 3091, 12–24‐year‐old male and female cinema‐goers across three Australian states. The study evaluated the cinema‐goers' opinion of smoking in the movies they viewed and their subsequent intention to smoke—with very different results.
Although the anti‐tobacco industry advertisement altered non‐smokers' opinion of smoking in the movie, it had no effect on smokers' opinion. Surprisingly, the advertisement seemed to encourage current smokers to continue smoking, with a significantly higher percentage of current smokers in the control group indicating they were unlikely to smoke in 12 months time than smokers in the intervention group.
The findings of this second real‐life study support those of the previous studies that antismoking advertising before movies in which characters are smoking can have a discernible effect on non‐smokers attitudes to smoking in movies. Both the “Tar/Lung” advertisement and the “smoking in movies” advertisement helped to immunise young non‐smokers against the influences of film stars smoking. The boomerang effect on smokers' intention to smoke in this second study suggests that caution must be exercised in the type of advertisement screened as some types of advertising may reinforce smokers intention to smoke.
The major strength of this study lies in the fact that it measured the reactions of the target audience to an antismoking advertisement viewed under naturalistic conditions in real cinemas using a control group. Pechmann and Shih21 acknowledge the limitations of conducting their research in relatively sterile classroom settings. An extensive literature search and consultation with anti‐tobacco experts in Australia and the US has identified the Edwards et al 2004 study as the only previous study of this nature conducted in cinemas.
The findings of this real‐life study confirm those of the Pechmann and Shih21 1999 and Edwards et al22 2004 studies that antismoking advertising before movies in which characters are smoking can have a discernible effect on attitudes to smoking in movies. Placing an antismoking advertisement before movies that contain smoking scenes can help to immunise young non‐smokers against the influences of film stars smoking. Further research needs to be conducted to ascertain what characteristics and messages are needed in antismoking advertisements to immunise young current smokers and alter their intention to smoke.
Acknowledgements
This project was funded jointly by the Central Coast Health Promotion Unit of Northern Sydney Central Coast Health and the Cancer Council NSW. We thank the Cancer Councils in South Australia and the Australian Capital Territory and the staff of Health Promotion Units across New South Wales for their support.
Footnotes
Competing interests: None declared.
References
- 1.Lawson V. Glamour puff. Tob Control 2003123–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Carter S. Worshipping at the Alpine altar: promoting tobacco in a world without advertising. Tob Control 200110391–393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Donovan R J, Weller N J, Clarkson J.Incidental smoking in the media study. Report to Commonwealth Department of Health and Ageing, CBRCC Report no. 030324, Centre for Behavioural Research in Cancer Control. Perth, WA: Curtin University, 2003
- 4.Glantz S A, Kacirk K W, McCulloch C. Back to the future: smoking in movies in 2002 compared with 1950 levels. Am J Public Health 200494(No 2)261–263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Sargent J D, Tickle J J, Beach M L.et al Brand appearances in contemporary cinema films and contribution to global marketing of cigarettes. Lancet 200135729–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gibson B, Maurer J. Cigarette smoking in the movies: the influence of product placement on attitudes toward smoking and smokers. J Appl Soc Psychol 2000301457–1483. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hazan A R, Lipton H L, Glantz S. Popular film do not reflect current tobacco use. Am J Public Health 199484998–1000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Eaton L. Tobacco companies exploit women says WHO. BMJ20013221384. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Escamilla G, Cradock A L, Kawachi I. Women and smoking in Hollywood movies: a content analysis. Am J Public Health 200090412–414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Omidvari K, Lessnau K, Kim J.et al Smoking in contemporary American cinema. Chest 2005128746–754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.McIntosh W D, Bazzini D G, Smith S M.et al Who smokes in Hollywood? Characteristics of smokers in popular films from 1940 to 1989. Addict Behav 199823395–398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dalton M, Tickle J J, Sargent J D.et al The incidence and context of tobacco use in popular movies from 1988 1997. Prev Med 200234516–523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tickle J J, Sargent J D, Dalton M A.et al Favourite movie stars, their tobacco use in contemporary movies, and its association with adolescent smoking. Tob Control 20011016–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sargent J D, Dalton M A, Beach M L.et al Viewing tobacco use in movies: does it shape attitudes that mediate adolescent smoking? Am J Prev Med 200222137–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sargent J D, Beach M, Adachi‐Mejia A.et al Exposure to movie smoking: its relation to smoking initiation among US adolescents. Pediatrics 20051161183–1191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sargent J D, Beach M, Dalton M.et al Effect of seeing tobacco use in films on trying smoking among adolescents: cross sectional study. BMJ 20013236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Distefan J M, Gilpin E A, Sargent J D.et al Do movie stars encourage adolescents to start smoking? Evidence from California. Prev Med 1999281–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Distefan J M, Pierce J P, Gilpin E A. Do favorite movie stars influence adolescent smoking initiation? Am J Public Health 2004941239–1244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dalton M, Sargent J D, Beach M L.et al Effect of viewing smoking in movies on adolescent smoking initiation: a cohort study. Lancet 2003362281–285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Australian Medical Association Cinema without cigarettes. –AMA media release. 9 March 2004
- 21.Pechmann C, Shih C. Smoking scenes in movies and anti‐smoking advertisements before movies: effects on youth. J Mark 1999631–13. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Edwards C A, Harris W C, Cook D R.et al Out of the smokescreen: does an anti‐smoking advertisement affect young women's perception of smoking in movies and their intention to smoke? Tob Control 200413277–282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Fishbein M, Ajzen I.Beliefs, attitudes, intentions, and behavour: an introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison‐Wesley, 1975
- 24.Glanz K, Lewis F M, Rimmer B K. eds. Health behaviour and health education: theory, research and practice San Francisco: Jossey‐Bass Publishers, 199068–69.
- 25.Petty R E, Cacioppo J T. Attitude change. Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Springer‐Verlag, 1986
- 26.Sargent J D, Dalton M A, Heatherton T.et al Modifying exposure to smoking depicted in movies—a novel approach to preventing adolescent smoking. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2003157643–648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Anonymous Cinema in Australia. Roy Morgan Research 1999
- 28.Anonymous Cinema—the multicultural audience. AC Nielsen 1999
- 29.James R, Leivers S, Hannaby L.et al Using cinema advertising to promote health. Health Promot J Aust 1996653–54. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Screenit! Entertainment Review http://www.screenit.com (accessed 30 Mar 2007)
- 31.Nichols M (Producer) Closer [motion picture]. USA: Sony Pictures, 2004
- 32.Pope E (Producer) Alfie [motion picture]. USA: Paramount Pictures, 2004
- 33.Barton J, Chassin L, Presson C.et al Social image factors as motivators of smoking initiation in early and middle adolescence. Child Dev 1982531499–1511. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Chassin L, Presson C, Bensenberg M.et al Predicting adolescents intention to smoke cigarettes. J Health Soc Behav 198122445–455. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Norman N, Tedeschi J. Self presentation, reasoned action, and adolescents' decision to smoke cigarettes. J Appl Soc Psychol 198919543–558. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Stantion W R, Barnett A G, Silva P A. Adolescents' intentions to smoke as a predictor of smoking. Prev Med 20054022–226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Mazanov J, Byrne D G. A comparison of predictors of the adolescent intention to smoke with adolescent current smoking using discriminant function analysis. Br J Health Psychol 20027185–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wakefield M, Flay B, Nichter M.et al Role of media in influencing trajectories of youth smoking. Addiction 1998(Su1) pp 79–1) pp103. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 39.Pechmann C, Reibling E T. Antismoking advertisements for youths: an independent evaluation of health, counter‐industry, and industry approaches. Am J Public Health 200696906–913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Prochaska J, Di Clemente C.The health benefits of smoking cessation. A report of the surgeon general. Rockville, MD: US Department of Health and Human Services Publication Number (CDC) 90‐8416, 1990
- 41.Armstrong B, de Klerk N, Shean R.et al Influence of education and advertising on the uptake of smoking by children. Med J Aust 1990152117–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Shean R, de Klerk N, Armstrong B.et al Seven year follow‐up of a smoking‐prevention program for children. Aust J Public Health 199418205–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Wakefield M, Kloska D D, O'Malley P M.et al The role of smoking in predicting future smoking among youth: findings from monitoring the future data. Addiction 99914–922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]