Through technological innovations, humans have attained the unique ability to observe and study perceptual phenomena occurring outside the limits of our own sensory capabilities. In the study of acoustic communication, one such realm is the frequency channel above the upper limit of human hearing (ca. ≥20 kHz), which is defined in human terms as “ultrasound”. The ultrasonic frequency boundary is biologically somewhat arbitrary; the ability to hear ultrasound is widespread among terrestrial vertebrates, and is thought to be an ancestral trait of mammals (Masterton et al. 1968; Sales & Pye 1974). However, within the context of acoustic communication, distinguishing between audible (i.e., within the human audible range, ca. 20 Hz – 20 kHz) and ultrasonic communication provides a foundation upon which to explore intriguing questions regarding the adaptive utility of animal communication systems. The ever-expanding selection and quality of ultrasonically-sensitive microphones and recorders have provided the opportunity to address such questions with increasing sophistication.
For effective acoustic communication to occur, an emitted signal must reach a receiver with enough clarity to allow an appropriate behavioral and/or physiological decision to be made. After Endler (1993), we can therefore consider clear reception to be the minimum requirement of a successful communication system. With this in mind, it becomes apparent that factors inherent in the transmission properties of ultrasonic frequencies place some restrictions on the environments and social conditions in which they are useful as information-bearing elements (sensu Suga 1972). Higher-frequency sounds attenuate more rapidly with distance (Morton 1975; Lawrence & Simmons 1982; Surlykke 1988; Römer & Lewald 1992) and are more directional than low-frequencies (Kinsler & Frey 1962). In addition, the short wavelengths of high-frequency sounds make them susceptible to reflection and scattering by relatively small objects, such as twigs, leaves, and blades of grass (Marler 1955; Sales & Pye 1974). Thus, in the interest of signal clarity, we may expect animals engaging in long-distance communication, or communicating in environments with small-scale scattering properties, to focus their vocal efforts in the audible frequency range; in this way, they can ensure that their calls have a higher probability of maintaining fidelity during transmission from emitter to receiver. However, additional environmental and behavioral complexities can place further, and in some cases competing, selective constraints on signal frequency. In addition, certain ecological scenarios may encourage the exploitation of ultrasound’s distinctive transmission properties. Herein, we will discuss some biological and ecological scenarios in which evolution may favor the development of an ultrasonic communication system, and provide examples of terrestrial vertebrates that have apparently been subject to such selection pressures. We focus on terrestrial vertebrates because the study of ultrasonic communication in this group is a rapidly expanding field. By defining biophysical, behavioral, and environmental conditions under which ultrasonic communication is likely to evolve in terrestrial vertebrates, we are well situated to identify additional organisms that may use similar strategies. The discovery of ultrasonic communicators among this group that run the evolutionary spectrum from amphibians to mammals provides an opportunity to gain more complete understanding of the behavioral foundations of high-frequency communication. In addition, comparative studies of ultrasonically-communicating taxa may give insight into the fundamental physiological mechanisms that underlie the ability to both produce and perceive extraordinarily high-frequency sounds.
Exaptation of Non-Communicatory Ultrasounds
In evolutionary theory, the idea of exaptation was first introduced by Gould and Vrba (1982). These authors suggested that an adaptation is “any feature that promotes fitness and was built by selection for its current role”. Other characters, however, “evolved for other usages (or no function at all), and later [were] ‘coopted’ for their current role” (Gould & Vrba 1982, pg. 6). To these latter characters, they assigned the term exaptation. In this context, if an ultrasonic utterance is emitted for a function other than intraspecific communication, or as an incidental byproduct of a physiological process or biomechanical strain, it cannot be said to be adapted for communication. However, if the ultrasounds have a consistent influence on conspecific behavior, then the vocalization may be considered exapted for communication. In these cases, the evolution of the ultrasonic communication system may have been guided by conspecifics’ advantageous use of inadvertently provided sensory information.
Among terrestrial vertebrates, the most conspicuous ultrasonic vocalizers are the bats. Microchiropteran bats [and the Rousettus genus of the megachiroptera (Mohres & Kulzer 1956)] are nocturnally active and have limited vision, relying on a sonar system comprised of ultrasonic pulses (i.e., echolocation) to locate, identify and track moving prey, avoid obstacles, and orient in 3D space (Griffin 1958; Simmons et al. 1975; Grinnell 1995; Neuweiler 2000; Schüller & Moss 2004). The frequencies contained in echolocation pulses must have sufficiently short wavelengths to reflect from small targets as usable echoes. To overcome the rapid attenuation of such high-frequency signals, echolocation calls are often emitted at extraordinary intensities (e.g., ≥ 100 dB SPL). In addition, the auditory systems of many bat species are maximally sensitive to a frequency band corresponding to the dominant frequency of their echolocation pulses, facilitating the perception of faint echoes (e.g., Dalland 1965; Brown 1973; Pollak et al. 1978; Koay et al. 1997). Although echolocation can be seen as a form of self-communication in which bats use their own vocalizations to attain information about their environment, the primary functions of echolocation vocalizations are clearly prey capture and navigation rather than intraspecific communication. However, the ubiquitous use of intense echolocation signals by active individuals, coupled with elevated auditory sensitivity to the frequencies contained in these signals, makes it a system that is ideally situated for eavesdropping and exaptation for communication. Indeed, playback experiments and call-feature analyses demonstrate that echolocation signals can be individually distinct (Jones et al. 1994; Masters et al. 1995; Fenton et al. 2004), and serve important intraspecific communication functions for a number of species (Mohres 1967; Habersetzer 1981; Barclay 1982; Leonard & Fenton 1984; Balcombe & Fenton 1988; Obrist 1995; Pearl & Fenton 1996; Kazial & Masters 2004). Echolocation calls have been implicated in synchronizing activity away from roosts (Fenton et al. 2004), aiding in localization of communal roosts (Barclay 1982), attracting attention to food sources (Griffin 1958; Barclay 1982; Balcombe & Fenton 1988), and communicating between mother and young (Matsumura 1981; Brown et al. 1983). Furthermore, increasing exploration of the intricate, acoustically-mediated social system of bats has revealed that many social calls, while generally longer and more variable than sonar pulses, share spectral and/or structural features with echolocation signals (e.g., Kanwal et al. 1994; Behr & von Helversen 2004; Kanwal 2006; Ma et al. 2006); these data suggest that the two classes of vocalizations share a common evolutionary substrate. Indeed, ontogenetic studies imply that some species’ echolocation signals derive from particular communication calls (Gould 1971, 1977; Matsumura 1981; de Fanis & Jones 1995), although for other species there is no clear precursor to the echolocation pulse (Brown & Grinnell 1980; Moss 1988). Future research in this area is likely to reveal additional ways in which echolocation calls have been exapted for use in intraspecific communication, as well as broaden our understanding of the evolutionary role of echolocation cries, and intentionally inter-individual social signals, in shaping bats’ ultrasonic vocal repertoire.
Rodents provide another interesting example of an ultrasonic communication system comprised of a subset of signals that may have been exapted for their communicatory role. The majority of small rodent species use ultrasound to mediate a wide variety of social interactions (Sales & Pye 1974). The importance of ultrasonic communication by adult rodents has been suggested for an array of interrelated facets of sociality including courtship and mating behavior, aggression, territoriality, and alarm behavior. In addition, pups from all species studied in the families Muridae and Cricetidae emit ultrasonic vocalizations when they are separated from the nest, which reliably induces approach and retrieval behavior by their mothers (Sewell 1970; Brooks & Banks 1973; Ehret & Haack 1981, 1982; Haack et al. 1983; Ehret & Haack 1984). It is generally accepted that most adult rodent ultrasounds are related to expiratory air movements deliberately produced as communication signals, and may even be indicative of affective state in some species (Knutson et al. 1998; Burgdorf & Panksepp 2001; Knutson et al. 2002). The proximate mechanisms inducing pup ultrasounds, however, are less clear. The traditional interpretation of infant rodent vocalizations is that they are distress calls produced by the pups to elicit attention from their mothers. Blumberg et al. (2000) argue, however, that pup ultrasounds are produced as an inadvertent acoustic byproduct of a physiological process called an abdominal compression reaction (ACR). In an ACR, the abdominal muscles are contracted during expiration in order to help propel venous blood back to the heart; this process may help maintain cardiac output during physiologically stressful situations, such as extreme cooling (Youmans et al. 1974). During an ACR by a rodent pup, the larynx is used as a brake during expiration contributing to increased intraabdominal pressure that results in the inadvertent production of ultrasounds (Blumberg et al. 2000). Ultrasounds resulting from ACRs that consistently occur during isolation from the nest, such as in response to cooling, could easily be interpreted as deliberate communication signals rather than incidental side-effects of behaviors linked to external stimuli. It is worth noting that this explanation for pup ultrasounds remains controversial and awaits conclusive correlative data linking ultrasound production with ACRs (Knutson et al. 2002; Shair & Jasper 2003; Ehret 2005; Shair 2007). Regardless of whether rodent pups’ ultrasonic vocalizations represent willful signaling, or are byproducts of biomechanical strain, it is clear that the signals consistently induce a behavior from the mother that is beneficial to the signaler, and so serve a communication function. The implication of the argument of Blumberg et al. is that ultrasonic signals resulting from physiological or biophysical properties associated with the signaler’s behavioral and/or physiological state can be exapted to serve a communicatory function, and thus become fixed within a communication system.
Predator Avoidance
Auditory signaling offers a number of advantages as a sensory mode of communication. Vocal communication can rapidly transfer a substantial amount of information encoded across multiple channels (e.g., frequency, amplitude), and is not limited by light availability or the need for immediate proximity between signaler and receiver (Endler 1973). One of the primary disadvantages of vocal communication, however, is its vulnerability to eavesdropping (Peake 2005). Eavesdroppers may include conspecific competitors, as well as prey or predator species, and can exert strong selection pressure on the design of acoustic signals (Dabelsteen 2005). In the case of rodents, vulnerability to predation may be implicated in the development and elaboration of ultrasonic signal repertoires.
The perceptual abilities of predators can influence signal design on both the evolutionary and proximate time scales (Owings et al. 2002; Snowdon & de la Torre 2002). It is unlikely, however, that rodent ultrasonic communication evolved because of its inaudibility to rodents’ wide array of potential predators. While some of these predators, such as humans, falcons, owls, and eagles are incapable of hearing ultrasonic frequencies, many others, including the domestic dog (Canis familiaris), domestic cat (Felis catus) and coyote (Canis latrans) can detect sounds up to at least 48 kHz (Schwartzkopff 1955; Peterson et al. 1969 ; Sales & Pye 1974; Klump et al. 1986). Nevertheless, the very properties of ultrasonic frequencies that limit their utility for long-distance signaling, namely atmospheric attenuation, directionality, and susceptibility to scattering, can be beneficial for avoiding eavesdropping, even by predators that are ultrasonically sensitive. Many rodent species are highly social and confine the majority of their activity to a limited area, such as a burrow or a nest. In this context, the ultrasonic vocalizations emitted by adult rodents to facilitate interindividual social encounters would benefit from the transmission limitations of ultrasonic frequencies. By emitting ultrasonic signals, individuals can effectively restrict the transmission of acoustic information to only those receiver(s) in close proximity.
In addition to short-range social interactions by rodents, there is evidence that the acoustic properties of ultrasounds can be exploited in a context-specific manner during rodent alarm calling (Wilson & Hare 2004, 2006). Richardson’s ground squirrels (Spermophilus richardsonii) produce two types of alarm vocalizations, audible (fundamental frequency ca. 8 kHz) and ultrasonic (fundamental frequency ca. 50 kHz) (Koeppl et al. 1978; Wilson & Hare 2004). Wilson and Hare (2004) found that the probability of the squirrels producing ultrasonic rather than audible alarm calls increased with increasing distance from the call-inducing-stimulus, thus taking advantage of the rapid attenuation of the ultrasonic frequencies to warn nearby conspecifics without alerting the predator. It has also been demonstrated that laboratory rats living in artificial burrows with conspecifics will emit ultrasonic calls in the presence of a threatening stimulus, and playback of these putative alarm calls promote freezing and avoidance behaviors (Blanchard et al. 1991; Brudzynski & Ociepa 1992; Brudzynski & Chiu 1995). Whether the frequency of rat alarm calls also differs with distance to the predator, or in another context specific manner, is unknown. Additional field experiments like those of Wilson and Hare (2004) that examine the production and function of ultrasonic vocalizations emitted by wild rodents under natural conditions are necessary to fully understand their adaptive significance. Interestingly, Wilson and Hare (2006) make the point that humans are alarm-call-eliciting stimuli for ground squirrels; this observation is presumably true for many, if not all, alarm-calling rodent species. Thus, the increased likelihood that ultrasonic alarm calls will be produced when we are distant from the signaler, coupled with our inability to hear these calls, presents formidable challenges to observing these vocalizations and determining the behavioral contexts under which they are emitted in the field (Wilson & Hare 2006). The increased compactness and portability of ultrasound detection devices and microphone arrays (e.g., Hiryu et al. 2005) should increase the feasibility of such studies with a wide variety of species.
Increased Signal-to-Noise-Ratio
Recalling the minimum requirement for clear reception for the evolution of a communication system, we see that any factor that decreases the signal-to-noise-ratio of a call at its point of reception will negatively impact the active space of the signal (Endler 1973; Narins & Zelick 1988; Klump 1996). One such factor is environmental, or “background”, acoustic noise which is ubiquitous in the habitats of vocally communicating organisms. Noise sources include biotic sounds, such as those produced by arthropods (Bradbury & Vehrencamp 1998), songbirds (Catchpole & Slater 1995) and anurans (Gerhardt & Huber 2002), and abiotic noise from wind, rain, or running water. The degree to which ambient noise interferes with acoustic communication is contingent on the amount of frequency overlap between the communicatory signal and the noise (Dooling 1982; Klump 1996; Hulse 2002). Thus, consistent environmental sound in a particular frequency range may function as a selection pressure guiding the evolution of acoustic signals with non-overlapping spectral components. Accordingly, among terrestrial vertebrates the presence of continuous background noise has been implicated in the evolution of the frequency spectra of some bird, primate, and anuran vocalizations (Morton 1975; Brenowitz 1982; Narins 1982; Wiley & Richards 1982; Ryan & Brenowitz 1985; Waser & Brown 1986; Slabbekoorn & Peet 2003; Narins et al. 2004).
Environmental noise sources differ markedly in their bandwidth. Some, such as rushing water, waterfalls, and anthropogenic traffic noise, can span the entire human audible frequency range. Faced with such noise emitters, acoustic communicators may significantly increase the active space of their signals by shifting the spectral energy of their calls to ultrasonic frequencies; the concomitant gain in signal-to-noise-ratio at the point of the receiver may outweigh the disadvantages to long-distance communication inherent in the transmission limitations of high-frequency sounds. Recently, it was found that the males of two frog species, Odorrana tormota (previously Amolops tormotus) and Huia cavitympanum, that live adjacent to torrents producing broadband, low-frequency-dominated noise, emit advertisement calls with substantial ultrasonic spectral energy and, in the case of H. cavitympanum, exclusively ultrasonic energy (Narins et al. 2004; Arch et al. 2008) . Confirmation that H. cavitympanum communicates ultrasonically awaits further research, but behavioral and neurophysiological experiments demonstrate that the ultrasonic components of O. tormota’s calls serve a communicatory function (Feng et al. 2006). Odorrana tormota’s vocal output and hearing sensitivity significantly exceed previously posited upper limits for anurans (Capranica 1965; Loftus-Hills & Johnstone 1970; Fay 1988; Glaw & Vences 1994), and Narins et al. (2004) suggest that this substantial upward extension of communication frequency is an adaptation to avoid acoustic masking by the nearby torrents. This research suggests that long-term adaptation to habitat-specific noise may be a powerful selective force, encouraging upward shifts of vocalization frequencies among species, or even subspecies or populations (Slabbekoorn & Peet 2003), exposed to persistently high levels of low-frequency background noise. It is important to note that the same selective force may theoretically work in the opposite direction, selecting for a downward shift of vocal frequency, in habitats dominated by high-frequency or ultrasonic noise, such as the sound produced by many arthropod species.
Summary and Conclusion
For many terrestrial vertebrates, acoustic communication is essential for establishing and maintaining territories, attracting mates, evading predators, and locating prey. Successful acoustic communication requires that signals maintain sufficient integrity during transmission to reliably convey the emitted information from signaler to receiver. Thus, the inherent transmission limitations of ultrasonic frequencies resulting from their high rate of atmospheric attenuation, directionality, and vulnerability to scattering, place restrictions on the ecological conditions in which they represent useful communication signals. We have discussed some of the ecological and physiological factors that may select for the use of high-frequency communication channels. These scenarios suggest that additional ultrasonic communicators may be discovered among species that produce non-communicatory ultrasounds correlated with specific behaviors that may be exapted for communication. In addition, social animals living in close quarters, and alarm-calling species, may take advantage of the limited transmission of ultrasounds to selectively “beam” communicatory signals to nearby receivers without alerting competitors or predators. Finally, organisms subject to persistent acoustic interference from broadband, low-frequency environmental noise might gain a significant increase in the signal-to-noise-ratio of their calls by shifting signal frequency up into a comparatively noise-free ultrasonic frequency channel. The communicatory advantage of this shift may be sufficient to offset the transmission disadvantages of high-frequency sounds. It is our hope that recognizing some of the conditions in which ultrasonic communication may be evolutionarily advantageous will enhance our ability to identify additional ultrasonically-communicating species among terrestrial vertebrates. Pursuing the study of ultrasonic communication in the field offers an opportunity to more fully understand the adaptive utility of differential frequency use in communication within complex environments. In addition, comparative studies of ultrasonic communicators offer behaviorists and physiologists an intriguing opportunity to explore the fundamental mechanisms underlying the use of extraordinarily high-frequencies in acoustic communication.
Acknowledgments
Financial support was provided by grants from the Paul S. Veneklasen Research Foundation and NIDCD (no. DC-00222) to P.M.N., and a GAANN Fellowship to V.S.A.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
Victoria S. Arch, Department of Ecology & Evolutionary Biology, University of California, Los Angeles
Dr. Peter M. Narins, Departments of Physiological Science and Ecology & Evolutionary Biology, University of California, Los Angeles
References
- Arch VS, Grafe TU, Narins PM. Ultrasonic signalling by a Bornean frog. Biology Letters. 2008;4:19–22. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balcombe JP, Fenton MB. Eavesdropping by bats: the influence of echolocation call design and foraging strategy. Ethology. 1988;79:158–166. [Google Scholar]
- Barclay RMR. Interindividual use of echolocation calls: eavesdropping by bats. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1982;10:271–275. [Google Scholar]
- Behr O, von Helversen O. Bat serenades - complex courtship calls of the sac-winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata) Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2004;56:106–115. [Google Scholar]
- Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC, Agullana R, Weiss SM. Twenty-two kHz alarm cries to presentation of a predator, by laboratory rats living in visible burrow systems. Physiology and Behavior. 1991;50:967–972. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90423-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg MS, Sokoloff G, Kirby RF, Kent KJ. Distress vocalizations in infant rats: What's all the fuss about? Psychological Science. 2000;11:78–81. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL. Principles of Animal Communication. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Brenowitz EA. The active space of redwinged blackbird song. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1982;147:511–522. [Google Scholar]
- Brooks RJ, Banks EM. Behavioral biology of the collared lemming [Dicrostonyx groenlandicus (traill)]: an analysis of acoustic communication. Animal Behaviour Monographs. 1973;6:3–83. [Google Scholar]
- Brown AM. An investigation of the cochlear microscopic response of two species of echolocation bats: Rousettus aegyptiacus (Geoffroy) and Pipistrellus pipistrellus (Schreber) Journal of Comparative Physiology. 1973;83:407–413. [Google Scholar]
- Brown P, Brown T, Grinnell A. Echolocation, development, and vocal communication in the lesser bulldog bat, Noctilio albiventris. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1983;13:287–298. [Google Scholar]
- Brown PE, Grinnell AD. Echolocation ontogeny in bats. In: Busnel RG, Fish JF, editors. Animal Sonar Systems. New York: Plenum; 1980. pp. 355–377. [Google Scholar]
- Brudzynski SM, Chiu EMC. Behavioural responses of laboratory rats to playback of 22 kHz ultrasonic calls. Physiology and Behavior. 1995;57:1039–1044. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)00003-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brudzynski SM, Ociepa D. Ultrasonic vocalization of laboratory rats in response to handling and touch. Physiology and Behavior. 1992;52:655–660. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(92)90393-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgdorf J, Panksepp J. Tickling induces reward in adolescent rats. Physiology and Behavior. 2001;72:167–173. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(00)00411-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capranica RR. The Evoked Vocal Response of the Bullfrog: A Study of Communication by Sound. Cambridge: MIT Press; 1965. [Google Scholar]
- Catchpole CK, Slater PJB. Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge: University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Dabelsteen T. Public, private or anonymous? Facilitating and countering eavesdropping. In: McGregor PK, editor. Animal Communication Networks. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2005. pp. 38–62. [Google Scholar]
- Dalland J. Hearing sensitivity in bats. Science. 1965;150:1185–1186. doi: 10.1126/science.150.3700.1185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Fanis E, Jones G. Post-natal growth, mother-infant interactions and development of vocalizations in the vespertilionid bat Plecotus auritus. Journal of Zoology. 1995;235:85–97. [Google Scholar]
- Dooling RJ. Auditory perception in birds. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, editors. Acoustic Communication in Birds. New York: Academic Press; 1982. pp. 95–129. [Google Scholar]
- Ehret G. Infant rodent ultrasounds: a gate to the understanding of sound communication. Behavioral Genetics. 2005;35:19–29. doi: 10.1007/s10519-004-0853-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehret G, Haack B. Categorical perception of mouse pup ultrasound by lactating females. Naturwissenschaften. 1981;68:208–209. doi: 10.1007/BF01047208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehret G, Haack B. Ultrasound recognition in house mice: key-stimulus configuration and recognition mechanisms. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1982;148:245–251. [Google Scholar]
- Ehret G, Haack B. Motivation and arousal influence sound-induced maternal pup-retrieving behavior in lactating house mice. Z Tierpsychol. 1984;65:25–39. [Google Scholar]
- Endler JA. Some general comments on the evolution and design of animal communication systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B. 1973;340:215–225. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1993.0060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fay RR. Hearing in Vertebrates: A Psychophysics Databook. Winnetka: Hill-Fay Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Feng AS, Narins PM, Xu CH, Lin WY, Yu ZL, Qiu Q, Xu ZM, Shen JX. Ultrasonic communication in frogs. Nature. 2006;440:333–336. doi: 10.1038/nature04416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenton MB, Jacobs DS, Richardson EJ, Taylor PJ, White W. Individual signatures in the frequency-modulated sweep calls of African large-eared, free-tailed bats Otomops martiensseni (Chiroptera: Molossidae) Journal of Zoology London. 2004;262:11–19. [Google Scholar]
- Gerhardt HC, Huber F. Acoustic Communication in Insects and Anurans. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Glaw F, Vences M. A Fieldguide to the Amphibians and Reptiles of Madagascar. 2 edn. Köln: Verlags; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Gould SJ. Studies of maternal-infant communication and development of vocalizations in the bats Myotis and Eptesicus. Communications in Behavioral Biology. 1971;5:263–313. [Google Scholar]
- Gould SJ. Echolocation and communication. In: Baker RJ, Jones JK, Carter DC, editors. Biology of Bats of the New World Family Phyllostomatidae. Lubbock: Special Publications of the Museum of the Texas Technical University; 1977. pp. 247–279. [Google Scholar]
- Gould SJ, Vrba ES. Exaptation - A missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology. 1982;8:4–15. [Google Scholar]
- Griffin DR. Listening in the Dark: The Acoustic Orientation of Bats and Men. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1958. [Google Scholar]
- Grinnell AD. Hearing in bats: An overview. In: Popper AN, Fay RR, editors. Hearing by Bats. New York: Springer; 1995. pp. 1–36. [Google Scholar]
- Haack B, Markl H, Ehret G. Sound communication between parents and offspring. In: Williott JF, editor. The Auditory Psychobiology of the Mouse. Springfield: Charles C. Thomas; 1983. pp. 57–97. [Google Scholar]
- Habersetzer J. Adaptive echolocation sounds in the bat Rhinopoma hardwickei, a field study. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1981;144:559–566. [Google Scholar]
- Hiryu S, Katsura K, Lin L, Riquimaroux H, Watanabe Y. Doppler-shift compensation in the Taiwanese leaf-nosed bat (Hipposideros terasensis) recorded with a telemetry microphone system during flight. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2005;118:3927–3933. doi: 10.1121/1.2130940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hulse SH. Auditory scene analysis in animal communication. In: Slater PJB, Rosenblatt JS, Snowdon CT, Roper TJ, editors. Advances in the Study of Behavior. San Diego: Academic Press; 2002. pp. 163–200. [Google Scholar]
- Jones G, Sripathi K, Waters DA, Marimuthu G. Individual variation in the echolocation calls of three sympatric Indian hipposiderid bats, and an experimental attempt to jam bat echolocation. Folia Zoologica. 1994;43:347–362. [Google Scholar]
- Kanwal JS. A distributed cortical representation of social communication calls. In: Kanwal JS, Ehret G, editors. Behavior and Neurodynamics for Auditory Communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2006. pp. 156–183. [Google Scholar]
- Kanwal JS, Matsumura S, Ohlemiller K, Suga N. Analysis of acoustic elements and syntax in communication sounds emitted by mustached bats. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1994;96:1229–1254. doi: 10.1121/1.410273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kazial KA, Masters WM. Female big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, recognize sex from a caller's echolocation signals. Animal Behaviour. 2004;67:855–863. [Google Scholar]
- Kinsler LE, Frey AR. Fundamentals of Acoustics. 2 edn. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1962. [Google Scholar]
- Klump GM. Bird communication in the noisy world. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, editors. Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds. Ithaca: Cornell University Press; 1996. pp. 321–338. [Google Scholar]
- Klump GM, Kretzschmar E, Curio E. The hearing of an avian predator and its avian prey. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1986;18:317–323. [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Burgdorf J, Panksepp J. Anticipation of play elicits high-frequency ultrasonic vocalizations in young rats. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1998;112:65–73. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.112.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Burgdorf J, Panksepp J. Ultrasonic vocalizations as indices of affective states in rats. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:961–977. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koay G, Heffner HE, Heffner RS. Audiogram of the big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus) Hearing Research. 1997;105:202–210. doi: 10.1016/s0378-5955(96)00208-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koeppl JW, Hoffmann RS, Nadler CF. Pattern analysis of acoustical behavior in four species of ground squirrels. Journal of Mammalogy. 1978;59:677–696. [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence BD, Simmons JA. Measurement of atmospheric attenuation at ultrasonic frequencies and the significance for echolocation by bats. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1982;71:585–590. doi: 10.1121/1.387529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leonard ML, Fenton MB. Echolocation calls of Euderma maculatum (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae): use in orientation and communication. Journal of Mammalogy. 1984;5:122–126. [Google Scholar]
- Loftus-Hills JJ, Johnstone BM. Auditory function, communication, and the brain-evoked response in anuran amphibians. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1970;47:1131–1138. doi: 10.1121/1.1912015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma J, Kobayasi K, Zhang S, Metzner W. Vocal communication in adult greater horseshoe bats, Rhinolophus ferrumequinum. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2006;192:535–550. doi: 10.1007/s00359-006-0094-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marler P. Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature. 1955;176:6–8. [Google Scholar]
- Masters WM, Raver KAS, Kazial KA. Sonar signals of big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, contain information about individual identity, age and family affiliation. Animal Behaviour. 1995;50:1243–1260. [Google Scholar]
- Masterton B, Heffner H, Ravizza R. The evolution of human hearing. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1968;45:966–985. doi: 10.1121/1.1911574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumura S. Mother-infant communication in a horseshoe bat (Rhinolophus ferrumequinum nippon): vocal communication in three-week old infants. Journal of Mammalogy. 1981;62:20–28. [Google Scholar]
- Mohres FP. Communicative characters of sonar signals in bats. In: Busnel RG, editor. Animal Sonar Systems. Frascati, Italy: NATO Advanced Study Institute; 1967. pp. 939–945. [Google Scholar]
- Mohres FP, Kulzer E. Über die orientierung der flughunde (Chiroptera-Pteropodidae) Zeitschrift Fur Vergleichende Physiologie. 1956;38:1–29. [Google Scholar]
- Morton ES. Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. American Naturalist. 1975;108:17–34. [Google Scholar]
- Moss CF. Ontogeny of vocal signals in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. In: Natchtigall PE, Moore PWB, editors. Animal Sonar Processes and Performances. New York: Plenum; 1988. pp. 115–120. [Google Scholar]
- Narins PM. Effects of masking noise on evoked calling in the Puerto Rican Coqui (Anura: Leptodactylidae) Journal of Comparative Physiology. 1982;147:438–446. [Google Scholar]
- Narins PM, Feng AS, Lin WY, Schnitzler HU, Denzinger A, Suthers RA, Xu CH. Old World frog and bird vocalizations contain prominent ultrasonic harmonics. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2004;115:910–913. doi: 10.1121/1.1636851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narins PM, Zelick R. The effects of noise on auditory processing and behavior in amphibians. In: Fritszch B, Ryan MJ, Wilczynski W, Hetherington T, Walkowiak W, editors. The Evolution of the Amphibian Auditory System. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1988. pp. 511–536. [Google Scholar]
- Neuweiler G. The Biology of Bats. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Obrist MK. Flexible bat echolocation: the influence of individual, habitat and conspecifics on sonar signal design. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1995;36:207–219. [Google Scholar]
- Owings DH, Rowe MP, Rundus AS. The rattling sound of rattlesnakes (Crotalus viridis) as a communicative resource for ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi) and burrowing owls (Athene cunicularia) Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2002;116:197–205. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.116.2.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peake TM. Eavesdropping in communication networks. In: McGregor PK, editor. Animal Communication Networks. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2005. pp. 13–37. [Google Scholar]
- Pearl DL, Fenton MB. Echolocation calls provide information about group identity in the little brown bat, Myotis lucifugus. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1996;74:2184–2192. [Google Scholar]
- Peterson EA, Heaton WC, Wruble SD. Levels of auditory response in fissiped carnivores. Journal of Mammalogy. 1969;50:566–578. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollak GD, Marsh DS, Bodenhammer R, Souter A. A single unit analysis of inferior colliculus in unanaethetized bats: response patterns and spike count functions generated by constant frequency and frequency modulated sounds. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1978;41:677–691. doi: 10.1152/jn.1978.41.3.677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Römer H, Lewald J. High-frequency sound transmission in natural habitats: implications for the evolution of insect acoustic communication. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1992;29:437–444. [Google Scholar]
- Ryan MJ, Brenowitz EA. The role of body size, phylogeny, and ambient noise in the evolution of bird song. American Naturalist. 1985;126:87–100. [Google Scholar]
- Sales G, Pye D. Ultrasonic Communication by Animals. London: Chapman and Hall; 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Schüller G, Moss CF. Vocal control and acoustically guided behavior in bats. In: Thomas JA, Moss CF, Vater M, editors. Echolocation in Bats and Dolphins. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press; 2004. pp. 3–16. [Google Scholar]
- Schwartzkopff J. On the hearing of birds. Auk. 1955;72:340–347. [Google Scholar]
- Sewell GD. Ultrasonic communication in rodents. Nature. 1970;227:410. doi: 10.1038/227410a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shair HN. Acquisition and expression of a socially mediated separation response. Behavioural Brain Research. 2007;182:180–192. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.02.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shair HN, Jasper A. Decreased venous return is neither sufficient nor necessary to elicit ultrasonic vocalizations of infant rat pups. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;117:840–853. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.4.840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmons JA, Howell DJ, Suga N. Information content of bat sonar echoes. American Scientist. 1975;63:204–215. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slabbekoorn H, Peet M. Birds sing at a higher pitch in urban noise. Nature. 2003;424:267. doi: 10.1038/424267a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowdon CT, de la Torre S. Multiple environmental contexts and communication in pygmy marmosets (Cebuella pygmaea) Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2002;116:182–188. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.116.2.182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suga N. Analysis of information-bearing elements in complex sounds by auditory neurons of bats. Audiology. 1972;11:58–72. doi: 10.3109/00206097209072581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Surlykke A. Interaction between echolocating bats and their prey. In: Nachtigall PE, Moore PWB, editors. Animal Sonar. New York: Plenum Publishing Corp; 1988. pp. 551–566. [Google Scholar]
- Waser PM, Brown CH. Habitat acoustics and primate communication. American Journal of Primatology. 1986;10:135–154. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350100205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiley RH, Richards DG. Adaptations for acoustic communication in birds: Sound transmission and signal detection. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, editors. Acoustic Communication in Birds. New York: Academic Press; 1982. pp. 131–181. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson DR, Hare JF. Ground squirrel uses ultrasonic alarms. Nature. 2004;430:523. doi: 10.1038/430523a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson DR, Hare JF. The adaptive utility of Richardson's ground squirrel (Spermophilus richardsonii) short-range ultrasonic alarm signals. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 2006;84:1322–1330. [Google Scholar]
- Youmans WB, Tjioe DT, Tong EY. Control of involuntary activity of the abdominal muscles. American Journal of Physical Medicine. 1974;53:57–74. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]