Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Pain. 2008 Jun 12;9(9):863–871. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.04.008

Pain Catastrophizing and Beliefs Predict Changes in Pain Interference and Psychological Functioning in Persons with Spinal Cord Injury

Marisol A Hanley *, Katherine Raichle †,*, Mark Jensen *, Diana D Cardenas +
PMCID: PMC2600516  NIHMSID: NIHMS69186  PMID: 18550442

Abstract

The current study sought to examine how changes in pain-related beliefs and coping responses are related to changes in pain interference and psychological functioning in individuals with spinal cord injuries (SCI) and pain. To measure longitudinal changes in these variables, respondents completed a survey which included measures of pain intensity, pain interference, and psychological functioning, as well as specific psychosocial variables (pain-related beliefs, coping, and social support), and then completed the same survey 6 months later; analyses included only the individuals who reported pain at both times (N = 40). Demographic and injury-related variables were also assessed, but none were found to be significantly associated with changes in functioning. Changes in catastrophizing and belief in one’s ability to control pain were each significantly associated with changes in the outcome variables: greater pain interference and poorer psychological functioning. Changes in specific coping strategies and social support were not predictors of changes in pain, interference, or psychological functioning . These findings support a biopsychosocial model of pain in persons with SCI. Intervention studies targeting maladaptive pain-related beliefs and catastrophizing may help to identify the causal nature of these relationships and may improve multidisciplinary treatment of pain in SCI.

Perspective

Intervention studies targeting catastrophizing and maladaptive pain-related beliefs may be the next step in determining which variables play a causal role in the pain interference and psychological functioning of individuals with pain and SCI.

Keywords: coping, cognitions, spinal cord injury, pain, mental health, catastrophizing


Pain is a common8,16 and often difficult to treat4,50,51 problem in persons with spinal cord injuries (SCI) which can be associated with increased physical and psychological dysfunction, independent of the effects of the injury.16,27,32,41,43 Given the refractory nature of pain in persons with SCI, the use of adaptive pain coping skills may be especially important for maintaining quality of life.52

Pain-related beliefs and coping methods have been shown to influence levels of psychological and physical dysfunction for individuals with chronic pain in general.2,17,22,39-40,44,46 A growing body of research has sought to extend these findings specifically to SCI and pain, demonstrating that particular beliefs and coping strategies are linked to adjustment to pain in persons with SCI. For example, catastrophizing, characterized as a tendency to exaggerate the negative consequences of a situation, has been associated with reported sensory and affective pain,12 pain intensity,45 psychological distress,45,29 pain-related disability45 and pain interference29 in cross-sectional studies of individuals with SCI. A number of other specific belief/appraisal variables have been associated with functioning. In one study, a belief in one’s ability to have control over pain was associated with greater mental health, and a number of beliefs were associated with levels of pain interference, such as beliefs that pain signifies damage or that a medical cure exists for pain.29 Regarding coping responses linked to pain in SCI, two studies in particular have demonstrated that particular coping responses are associated with better mental health, such as greater use of task persistence,29 coping self-statements, and ignoring pain,45 while other coping responses are associated with higher levels of pain interference, such as greater use of resting, guarding, and asking for assistance.29 In addition, social environmental variables, such as perceived social support29,31 and negative perceived partner responses to pain,38 have been associated with adjustment.

The above cross-sectional findings provide preliminary support for a biopsychosocial model of pain adaptation in SCI, in which one’s beliefs about and coping responses to pain and one’s perceived social support influence pain adaptation, above and beyond pain intensity itself. However, the role of psychosocial variables in predicting changes in pain adaptation over time has not yet been examined. Such research would not only provide a further test of the model, but would help to identify those psychosocial variables which may potentially play a causal role in adjustment, and that should therefore be the target of future experimental research testing causal relationships.

The current study is a continuation of the cross-sectional survey study of SCI and pain, described above,29 which examined the associations between coping, cognitive, and social environmental variables with psychological functioning and pain interference. The primary purpose of the current study was to examine the same variables longitudinally to see if a similar pattern of associations would be found over time. The individuals who completed the original survey were asked to complete the same survey 6 months later. We hypothesized that changes in pain-related beliefs, coping, and social support would be associated with concurrent changes in several key outcomes (pain intensity, pain-related interference with functioning, and psychological functioning) over this same time period. Specifically, we hypothesized that catastrophizing and maladaptive pain beliefs would be associated with decreases in psychological functioning and increases in pain intensity and interference, and that adaptive coping and social support would be associated with the reverse.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Participants in this study were recruited from the pool of individuals with spinal cord injury (SCI) who completed a survey regarding psychosocial functioning and the nature and scope of pain in persons with SCI,16,29 and who agreed to be contacted for future research (n = 142, 90% of survey completers). Detailed information regarding the pool of participants who completed the initial survey has been reported elsewhere.16 In the current study, the same survey was sent to 68 of the original 142 participants 6 months after initial survey completion (Footnote 1). Of the 68 surveys mailed at the 6-month timepoint, 51 useable surveys were returned, yielding a response rate of 77% (51/66, excluding 1 participant who could not be located, and 1 participant who was sent the wrong survey due to a clerical error). Because the majority of variables we wished to examine over time were related to pain (i.e., pain coping, pain cognitions), the current study could only include the participants who reported pain at both time points (n = 40); therefore, all analyses were conducted with these 40 participants, and the study will report on this subset of 40 participants from this point forward. It should be noted that of the initial 51 survey respondents, the overwhelming majority (78%, or 40/51) reported a pain problem lasting at least 6 months.1

The same procedures were employed to assess the study variables at each time point. Each mailed questionnaire included a consent form and a cover letter inviting recipients to participate in the study. Subjects were paid $25 for completing and returning the consent forms and survey at each time point. If surveys were incomplete or any responses were unclear, research staff called participants to clarify their answers. The study questionnaire and protocol were approved by the University of Washington Human Subjects Review Committee.

Measures

Demographic Information

Respondents were asked to provide information concerning their age, sex, ethnic/racial group, education, and marital status. The survey also assessed a number of SCI-related variables, including level, cause, and date of SCI. These demographic and injury-related characteristics are reported in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographic and injury-related information for participants with SCI who reported current pain problems at T1 and T2 (N = 40)

Age range, years 23 –73
Age, mean (SD)* years 49.3 (13.1)
Time since injury, mean (SD) years 17.9 (11.5)
Average age at injury (SD) 31.4 (15.5)
Sex, %
 Men 70
 Women 30
Ethnicity, %
 Caucasian 88
 African-American 0
 Asian-American 8
 Hispanic American 8
 Native American 13
Marital status, %
 Married/living with SO 53
 Divorced 10
 Widowed 8
 Never Married 28
Educational level, %
 Some high school 5
 High school graduate 15
 Vocational/Technical school 8
 Some college 38
 College graduate 20
 Graduate/Professional school 15
Self-reported injury level, %
 C1-C4 10
 C5-C8 38
 T1-T5 15
 T6-T12 28
 L1-S4,5 10
Completeness of injury, %
 Complete 40
 Incomplete 50
 Don’t know 10
Cause of injury, %
 Motor vehicle collision 30
 Fall 18
 Diving 10
 Sports-related accident 8
 Gunshot wound 3
 Other 33
*

SD, standard deviation.

Participants could endorse more than one response, therefore percentages total more than 100%.

Some of the data from the initial survey completion have already been presented. These focus on the prevalence and course of pain problems and functioning in persons with SCI,16 the utility of different pain treatments for persons with SCI,4 the psychometric properties of pain-interference measures in persons with SCI,30 and the utility of a biopsychosocial model of adjustment to pain in SCI.29 The longitudinal data assessed at 6 months following the initial survey have not yet been reported.

Average Pain Intensity

Survey respondents were screened for a recent pain problem using the following question: “Are you currently experiencing, or have you in the past three months experienced, any pain (other than occasional headaches or menstrual cramps)?” Respondents who answered “yes” to this question were then asked to rate the average intensity of this pain during the past week on a 0 – 10 numerical rating scale, with 0 = “No pain” and 10 = “Pain as bad as could be.” The reliability and validity of 0 – 10 scales have been shown by their strong association with other measures of pain intensity over time.21

Pain Interference

Pain interference was assessed using a 10-item scale adapted from the Brief Pain Inventory (BPI).5-6 Using an 11-point rating scale (0 = does not interfere, 10 = completely interferes), the original version of the BPI Pain Interference scale asked participants to rate the degree to which pain interferes with 7 daily activities: general activity, mood, walking ability, normal work, relations with other people, sleep, and enjoyment of life. Our modified version of this scale, which we have used in previous studies of persons with disabilities and pain14,48 has been changed in two ways. First, item 3 pertaining to “Walking ability” was changed to read “Mobility, that is, your ability to get around,” to be more appropriate for the participants in the current and previous studies, many of whom cannot walk. Second, we added three items to assess interference of pain with self-care, recreational activities, and social activities, to obtain a broader-based assessment of areas that could potentially be affected by pain. The original BPI Pain Interference scale has demonstrated validity through its strong association to pain severity across a number of different populations6,14,48 and the modified 10-item version of this scale has demonstrated high levels of internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .89) and validity through its strong association with pain intensity in previous samples of persons with disabilities.48 In the current sample, the internal consistency coefficient of this scale was 0.95.

Psychological Funtioning

Psychological functioning served as the final outcome domain in this study, and was assessed with the five-item Mental Health scale from the Medical Outcomes Study 36-Item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36).49 The Mental Health scale has a possible range from 0 to 100, with higher scores indicating better psychological functioning. This commonly used measure has demonstrated reliability, as shown by high internal consistency coefficients (Cronbach’s alpha ranging from 0.81 – 0.95) and test-retest stability coefficients (0.75 – 0.80).49 Its validity as a measure of psychological functioning is supported by its association with other measures that assess this same construct.49

Pain Cognitions

Pain-related cognitions pertain to an individual’s ideas about the nature of his or her pain, as well as to beliefs regarding his or her own and others’ roles in dealing with pain. Pain-related cognitions were assessed using the Survey of Pain Attitudes (SOPA).22 The SOPA includes the following 7 multi-item subscales which measure the extent to which an individual holds certain beliefs about pain: Control (belief in one’s own control over pain), Disability (belief that one is unable to function because of pain), Harm (belief that pain is an indication of damage and that activities should be avoided), Emotion (belief that emotions influence pain), Medication, (belief that medications are suitable for treating chronic pain), Solicitude (belief that others should offer assistance in response to pain behaviors), and Medical Cure (belief that there exists a medical cure for one’s pain). Items are rated on a scale from 0 - 4 (“this is very untrue for me” to “this is very true for me.”); item scores are averaged to create the subscale scores, and higher scores indicate a stronger belief in that idea about pain. The SOPA has demonstrated good test-retest stability, internal consistency, and criterion validity.22,37

Catastrophizing

The 6-item Catastrophizing scale of the Coping Strategies Questionnaire (CSQ)33 was used to assess pain catastrophizing cognitions (e.g., “It is terrible and I feel it is never going to get any better,” “I worry all the time about whether it will end). Respondents rate the extent, in general, to which they engage in that activity or thought when experiencing pain, on a scale from 0 – 6 (“never do that” to “always do that”). The subscale score is the mean of all 6 items, and higher scores indicate more frequent pain catastrophizing. The Catastrophizing scale of the CSQ has demonstrated excellent internal consistency reliability and validity in numerous studies.e.g.,13,15,33 Its validity has been demonstrated by associations with measures of psychosocial dysfunction and depression.11,18,25,39

Coping

Coping with pain was measured using the Chronic Pain Coping Inventory (CPCI),23 which was developed to quantify the specific coping strategies that tend to be encouraged in multidisciplinary pain programs, as well as those strategies that tend to be discouraged.23 The original CPCI consists of eight subscales that each assess a different coping response to pain: resting (7 items), task persistence (6 items), guarding (9 items), asking for assistance (4 items), relaxation (7 items), coping self-statements (11 items), exercise/stretch (12 items), and seeking social support (12 items). An additional scale (6 items) to assess the coping strategy of pacing was added later to the CPCI,28 and is included in the current study. The frequency of these coping strategies is measured by the total number of days that the strategy was used in the past week (0-7). The subscale score is the mean of all of the subscale items. These scales have demonstrated good internal consistency and test-retest reliability, and have shown validity through significant correlations in the expected directions with measures of patient functioning, and through significant correlations between patient and significant other versions of the scales.23,28

Social Environmental Factors

Perceived social support was assessed with the 12-item Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS),53 which was scored to measure global perceived support (e.g., “There is a special person around when I am in need.”). The items are rated on a scale from 1- 7 (“very strongly disagree” to “very strongly agree”). Internal consistencies of the subscales and total scale are all excellent (Cronbach’s alphas = .85 to .91), and the scales have demonstrated strong test-retest stability over a two- to three-month interval (r = .72 to .85). Validity of the total MSPSS scale has been demonstrated through its significant (negative) association with depression.53

Data Analyses

First, the distributions of all study variables were examined and none were significantly skewed, thus meeting the assumptions necessary for the subsequent statistical analyses. Next, change scores (time 2 – time 1) were calculated for all of the hypothesized predictor and criterion variables. These change scores were used to compute bivariate correlational (for continuous variables) and t-test (for categorical variables) analyses to examine the extent to which demographic and SCI-related variables were associated with the criterion variables of interest; that is, change in pain intensity, pain interference, and mental health. Demographic variables included age, gender, marital status (married/living with significant other or living alone), educational level (college graduate/advanced degree or less than college graduate); and SCI variables included time since injury, age at injury, and level of injury (tetraplegia or paraplegia). None of the demographic or SCI variables was significantly related with changes in mental health or pain interference, therefore, these variables were not included as control variables in the subsequent regression analyses. To further reduce the number of belief and coping measures in the planned regression analyses, we computed univariate correlations between the criterion variable change scores (change in pain intensity, pain interference, and mental health) and the predictor variable change scores, and limited the measures used in each of the regression analyses to include only predictor change scores that were correlated at a significance level of p < .10 with the criterion measure at hand. For change in pain intensity, the predictor change scores were entered in a single step. For change in pain interference and change in mental health, the pain intensity change score was entered in the first step of the equation to control for its effects (if any), and the predictor change scores were entered as a block in the second step.

RESULTS

Average pain intensity, pain interference, and psychological distress

On average, these individuals with chronic pain and SCI reported a moderate level of pain intensity (M = 5.0 at both T1 and T2) and a mild-to-moderate level of pain interference (M = 3.8 at T1, M = 3.6 at T2). The mean SF-36 Mental Health score was 69.6 at T1 and 69.2 at T2.

Regarding changes in pain intensity, 28% (n = 11) of the sample reported a 30% or greater (a standard for clinically meaningful change in pain)10 increase in pain intensity over the 6-month study period, and 20% (n = 8) reported a 30% or greater decrease in pain intensity. No significant systemic change was found for any of the outcome variables over the 6-month study period; that is, similar proportions of individuals reported increases or decreases in these variables. Regarding the predictor variables, only two measures, the SOPA control scale and the CPCI Relaxation scale, demonstrated significant differences from T1 to T2. Sample-wide changes in all other predictor variables were not significant. Using a standard for clinical importance of change of a 0.50 effect size (ie, a change in one-half the standard deviation of the initial score)7 we found that the majority of variables demonstrated meaningful change for at least half the participants, with the exceptions of the SOPA Harm, Exercise, and Medication scales (48%, 40%, and 43% of the participants evidenced meaningful change, respectively), the CPCI Exercise, Coping Self-statements, and Seeking Social Support scales (45%, 45%, and 38%, respectively), the Catastrophizing scale (43%) and the MSPSS (social support) scale (40%).

Univariate correlations were performed to determine which variables were correlated with pain intensity, pain interference or psychological functioning at a significance level of at least 0.10 and would therefore be included in the regression equations. Catastrophizing was associated with greater pain interference and lower mental health (rs = .42 and -.41, respectively, ps < .01). SOPA control evidenced strong correlations with pain interference and psychological functioning (r = -.53 and .55, ps < .001), and was moderately correlated with pain intensity (r = -.38, p < .01). In addition, SOPA Disability (the belief that one is disabled) was moderately correlated with pain interference and mental health (rs = .33 and -.32, respectively, ps < .05) and SOPA Harm (the belief that pain equals harm) was moderately correlated with mental health (r = -.38, p < .05). Among the coping scales, CPCI Exercise was moderately associated with pain intensity and mental health (rs = -.31, and -.32, respectively, ps < .05) and more strongly associated with pain interference (r = -.44, p < .01). The only other significant coping scale, CPCI Relaxation, was moderately associated with Mental Health (r = .33, p < .05).

Predicting Change in Pain Intensity

Change in pain intensity was not significantly associated with any of the demographic or SCI-related variables (age, gender, marital status, education level, age at injury, time since injury, or level of injury (paraplegia/tetraplegia). Age demonstrated a non-significant trend to be correlated with change in pain intensity (r = 0.28, p < .10); greater age was associated with greater pain intensity. Regarding relationships with the other criterion variables, change in pain intensity was significantly correlated with change in pain interference (r = .51, p < .001), but not with change in psychological functioning (r = -.12, n.s.).

In the regression model predicting change in pain intensity, the predictor variable change scores as a whole accounted for a significant (p < .05) 17% of the variance in pain intensity change score (see Table 2). Change in perceived control over pain (SOPA control) demonstrated a non-significant trend toward making an independent contribution to the variance in change in pain intensity (β = -.30, p < .10), suggesting that greater perceived control may be associated with decreased pain intensity.

Table 2.

Relationship between change in pain interference with changes in pain and psychosocial variables (N = 40).

Variables Total R2 R2 change F change β
Change in pain intensity
1. Changes in Cognition and Coping Variables 0.17 0.17 3.78*
 SOPA Control -.30
 CPCI Exercise -.17
Change in pain interference (BPI interference)
1. Change in Pain Intensity 0.26 0.26 12.44*** .32*
2. Changes in Cognition and Coping Variables 0.50 0.25 5.48**
 SOPA Control -.34*
 CPCI Exercise -.04
 Castastrophizing .33*
Change in psychological functioning (SF-36 MH)
1. Change in Pain Intensity 0.01 0.01 0.54 .12
2. Changes in Cognition and Coping Variables 0.50 0.49 4.35**
 SOPA Control .42*
 SOPA Harm -.12
 SOPA Disability -.18
 CPCI Exercise -.07
 CPCI Relaxation .05
 CPCI Pacing .15
 Catastrophizing -.35*

.05 < † < .10;

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001.

Predicting Change in Pain Interference

In the regression model predicting change in pain interference, change in pain intensity explained a large and significant (p < .001) 26% of the variance in the BPI pain interference change score (see Table 2). The predictor variable change scores as a whole accounted for an additional, and significant (p < .01), 25% of the variance in BPI pain interference change score. In addition, two predictor variables, change in perceived control over pain and change in catastrophizing (β = -.34 and .33, respectively, ps < .05), made significant and independent contributions to the variance in the pain interference change score. Increased perceived control, and decreased catastrophizing, were each associated with decreased pain interference.

Predicting Change in Psychological Functioning

In the regression model predicting change in psychological functioning, change in pain intensity did not make a significant contribution. However, the predictor variable change scores as a whole accounted for a very large and significant (p < .01) proportion, 49% of the variance in the SF-36 Mental Health scale change score. In addition, two predictor variables, change in perceived control over pain and change in catastrophizing (β = .42 and -.35, respectively, ps < .05), made significant, independent contributions to the prediction of the criterion variable. Specifically, increased perceived control, and decreased catastrophizing, were each associated with improved psychological functioning.

DISCUSSION

The primary finding of this study is that changes in adjustment to pain could not be fully explained by changes in pain intensity. Changes in pain intensity, pain interference, and psychological functioning were associated with changes in pain-related coping and cognitive variables, with changes in catastrophizing and perceived control over pain making significant independent contributions to changes in pain interference and psychological functioning. These findings provide preliminary support for the potentially important role played by coping and cognitive variables in short-term longitudinal outcomes for individuals with SCI and chronic pain.

Except for age, none of the demographic or SCI-related variables were significantly related to changes in pain intensity in the multivariate analyses. The two predictor variable change scores included in the regression analyses (change in exercise coping and perceived control over pain) were associated with a significant 17% of the variance in change in pain intensity, with a trend for greater perceived control over pain to be associated with decreased pain intensity. Change in pain was significantly associated with one of the other outcome variables -- change in pain interference -- but was not associated with change in psychological functioning.

Change in catastrophizing was a significant independent predictor of changes in adjustment; individuals with decreased catastrophizing tended to report decreased pain interference and improved psychological functioning. These longitudinal findings are consistent with previous cross-sectional studies of pain in SCI, which also found significant associations between catastrophizing and outcome variables (e.g., pain intensity, pain-related disability,12,45 pain interference,29 and psychological distress29,45). Given that most of the individuals in our sample had lived with SCI for several years or more, changes in catastrophizing cannot be explained by initial reactions to injury. The current study suggests the possibility that catastrophizing could potentially have a causal influence on both pain interference and psychological functioning, supporting the need for trials to test this hypnosis (via assignment to conditions that do, and that do not, alter catastrophizing see 9). Although it is possible that increasing pain causes increased catastrophizing or that the relationship is bi-directional, cognitive-behavioral interventions that target catastrophizing have been shown to reduce depression and disability in samples of individuals with chronic pain,34-35 and the maladaptive effects of catastrophizing are strongly supported by the general chronic pain literature. 2,17,19-20,39-40,44,46 Therefore, testing a de-catastrophizing intervention in the SCI population would be a logical next step.

Change in perceived control over pain was also a significant predictor of adjustment. This pain belief has been consistently associated with pain interference, disability, and depression in research with other chronic pain populations.17,19,20,25,37,42,44,47 Our previous study based on the initial survey completion provided the first evidence, to our knowledge, of the relationship between control appraisals and pain in SCI; greater perceived control over pain was associated with better mental health and lower levels of pain interference.29 In the current study, we provided additional support for this relationship by demonstrating that changes in perceived control over time were associated with changes in these outcomes. Further research is needed on a potential causal role for control beliefs, and, if a causal influence is found, to develop interventions that can enhance such beliefs.

Overall, pain cognitions appeared to have the strongest relationships with longitudinal changes in functioning, especially if catastrophizing is characterized as a set of maladaptive pain cognitions or a cognitive style. In contrast, change in pain coping variables and change in perceived social support did not evidence significant relationships with change in psychological functioning or pain interference after controlling for change in pain intensity and all other change variables. Therefore, our hypothesis that changes in pain beliefs would be associated with changes in pain interference and psychological functioning was supported, but changes in specific coping and social environmental variables were not associated with outcomes as we hypothesized. The latter results differ somewhat from the cross-sectional study results, which found that passive pain coping and perceived social support demonstrated significant relationships with adjustment to pain.29 It is possible that relationships found at one point in time may not hold true for changes over time. It is also possible that the smaller sample size of the current study did not provide adequate power for some associations to be identified.

Regarding the coping variables, at least one other cross-sectional study of SCI and pain found significant associations between coping and functioning;45 however, the coping measure used in that study (the Coping Strategies Questionnaire;CSQ)33 focuses mostly on cognitive coping strategies, in contrast to the measure used in this study, the CPCI, which places more emphasis on behavioral strategies, such as resting, exercise, and relaxation. In addition, the scales of the CPCI favor the use of a greater number of strategies rather than more frequent use of fewer strategies,20 which may be especially relevant for the exercise scale, given that individuals with SCI may have fewer options for exercise and exercise patterns may not change greatly over time. In addition, consistently weak support has been found for some behavioral coping strategies in the general chronic pain literature.20 In the study employing the CSQ,45 mainly cognitive coping strategies (and catastrophizing) were associated with criterion variables. In our previous cross-sectional study,29 passive coping was associated with a negative outcome (greater pain interference), but proactive coping was not associated significantly with any outcome. Taken together, these results suggest that changes in psychological functioning and pain interference in SCI are more strongly related to changes in cognitive coping than to changes in active behavioral strategies. Cognitive coping strategies may be especially important for this population given the physical limitations of SCI.

An important next step may involve controlled intervention studies or daily process studies of perceived control over pain and catastrophizing, in order to shed light on the causal nature of these relationships. Widerstrom-Noga has suggested that for a subgroup of SCI individuals with high affective distress and dysfunctional coping styles, interdisciplinary approaches that include psychological interventions, such as cognitive-behavioral therapy, may be most appropriate.51-52 Preliminary support for potential efficacy multidisciplinary management of SCI-related pain can be found in Budh and colleagues’ treatment study of SCI-related neuropathic pain, which utilized educational, cognitive, and behavioral interventions, resulting in decreased levels of anxiety and depressioncompared to a control group.3

Significant findings emerged in our study despite the fact that we found (and expected to find) few major systematic changes in pain or adjustment. The individuals in this study had been living with SCI and pain for years, as opposed to adjusting to a new injury, and research has shown that pain in SCI remains relatively stable over time.16,36 Our results may be relevant mainly for individuals at a later stage of adjustment to SCI, given that we do not know how our results may have differed in a sample demonstrating more dramatic change. Given the different challenges posed by initial adjustment to SCI,26 longitudinal research at earlier phases of adjustment to SCI may be very important.

A similar number of individuals reported an increase compared to a decrease in pain, suggesting that there is no general trend for individuals with SCI to have worsening pain intensity over the short-term. Similarly, the other variables did not demonstrate any systematic changes, with the exception of perceived control over pain and relaxation. Although it is possible that a lack of significant associations between certain variables could be due to a lack of change over time (no variation) in these variables rather than a real absence of association, most of the variables demonstrated a meaningful change (increase or decrease of at least ½ of a standard deviation) for half or close to half of the participants, suggesting that these variables did not remain static over the 6 month period.

As stated above, a larger sample size would have provided more power to detect associations, and some of the non-significant associations might have become significant. Therefore, our findings should be viewed as preliminary, and the coping and social support variables should not be disregarded even though their importance was not supported in this study. These results require replication in future studies. In addition, it is not possible to know how results would have differed if additional or different psychosocial factors had been included; however, we sought to measure the psychosocial variables supported by previous pain research. More diversity in education level and ethnicity would be desirable to strengthen the generalizability of the results. We do not know what was responsible for changes over time, and we did not assess if there were changes in other medical issues associated with SCI or in medical interventions.

Despite the limitations of this study, the findings demonstrated that changes in specific variables (perceived control over pain and catastrophizing) were associated with changes in pain interference and psychological functioning over a 6-month period, consistent with hypotheses derived from a biopsychosocial model of pain in persons with SCI. Identification of causal relationships may be furthered by intervention studies targeting pain cognitions and catastrophizing.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant “Management of Chronic Pain in Rehabilitation” PO1 HD/NS33988, from the National Institutes of Health and National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (National Center for Medical Rehabilitation Research). The authors gratefully acknowledge the contributions of Amy Hoffman, Noel Pereyra-Johnston, Laura Nishimura, Kevin Gurtz, Silvia Amtmann, Kerry Madrone, Kristin McCarthur, and Joe Skala, University of Washington Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, in data collection and database management.

Footnotes

1

Only 68 of 142 possible surveys were sent because of an administrative error in the study procedures.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Arnow BA, Hunkeler EM, Blasey CM, Lee J, Constantino MJ, Fireman B, Kraemer HC, Dea R, Robinson R, Hayward C. Comorbid depression, chronic pain, and disability in primary care. Psychosom Med. 2006;68:262–268. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000204851.15499.fc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Boothby JL, Thorn BE, Stroud MW, Jensen MP. Coping with pain. In: Gatchel RJ, Turk DC, editors. Psychosocial Factors in Pain. New York, NY: 1999. pp. 343–359. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Budh CN, Kowalski J, Lundeberg T. A comprehensive pain management programme comprising educational, cognitive and behavioural interventions for neuropathic pain following spinal cord injury. J Rehabil Med. 2006;38:172–180. doi: 10.1080/16501970500476258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Cardenas D, Jensen MP. Treatments for chronic pain in persons with spinal cord injury: a survey study. J Spinal Cord Med. 2006;29:109–117. doi: 10.1080/10790268.2006.11753864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cleeland CS, Ryan KM. Pain assessment: global use of the brief pain inventory. Ann Acad Med. 1994;23:129–138. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Daut RL, Cleeland CS, Flanery RC. Development of the Wisconsin Brief Pain Questionnaire to assess pain in cancer and other diseases. Pain. 1983;17:197–210. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(83)90143-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Dworkin RH, Turk DC, Wyrwich KW, Beaton D, Cleeland CS, Farrar JT, Haythornthwaite JA, Jensen MP, Kerns RD, Ader DN, Brandenburg N, Burke LB, Cella D, Chandler J, Cowan P, Dimitrova R, Dionne R, Hertz S, Jadad AR, Katz NP, Kehlet K, Kramer LD, Manning DC, McCormick C, McDermott MP, McQuay HJ, Patel S, Porter L, Quessy S, Rappaport BA, Rauschkolb C, Revicki DA, Rothman M, Schmader KE, Stacey BR, Stauffer JW, von Stein T, White RE, Witter J, Zavisic S. Interpreting the clinical importance of treatment outcomes in chronic pain clinical trials: IMMPACT recommendations. J Pain. 2008;9:105–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2007.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ehde DM, Jensen MP, Engel JM, Turner JA, Hoffman AJ, Cardenas DD. Chronic pain secondary to disability: a review. Clin J Pain. 2003;19:3–17. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200301000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ehde DM, Jensen MP. Feasibility of a cognitive restructuring intervention for treatment of chronic pain in persons with disabilities. Rehabil Psychol. 2004;49:254–258. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Farrar JT, Portenoy RK, Berlin JA, Kinman JL, Strom BL. Defining the clinically important difference in pain outcome measures. Pain. 2000;88:287–294. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00339-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Geisser ME, Robinson ME, Henson CD. The coping strategies questionnaire and chronic pain adjustment: a conceptual and empirical reanalysis. Clin J Pain. 1994;10:98–106. doi: 10.1097/00002508-199406000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Giardino ND, Jensen MP, Turner JA, Ehde DM, Cardenas DD. Social environment moderates the association between catastrophizing and pain among persons with a spinal cord injury. Pain. 2003;106:19–25. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(03)00226-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Gil KM, Abrams MR, Phillips G, Keefe FJ. Sickle cell disease pain: relation of coping strategies to adjustment. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1989;57:725–731. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.57.6.725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hanley MA, Jensen MP, Ehde DM, Hoffman AJ, Patterson DR, Robinson LR. Psychosocial predictors of long-term adjustment to lower-limb amputation and phantom limb pain. Disabil Rehabil. 2004;26:882–893. doi: 10.1080/09638280410001708896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hill A, Niven CA, Knussen C. The role of coping in adjustment to phantom limb pain. Pain. 1995;62:79–86. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(94)00253-B. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Jensen MP, Hoffman AJ, Cardenas DD. Chronic pain in individuals with spinal cord injury: a survey and longitudinal study. Spinal Cord. 2005;43:704–712. doi: 10.1038/sj.sc.3101777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Jensen MP, Karoly P. Control beliefs, coping efforts, and adjustment to chronic pain. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1991;59:431–438. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.59.3.431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Jensen MP, Karoly P. Self-report scales and procedures for assessing pain in adults. In: Turk DC, Melzack R, editors. Handbook of Pain Assessment. New York, NY: 1992. pp. 135–151. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Jensen MP, Turner JA, Romano JM. Changes in beliefs, catastrophizing, and coping are associated with improvement in multidisciplinary pain treatment. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2001;69:655–62. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.69.4.655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Jensen MP, Turner JA, Romano JM. Changes after multidisciplinary pain treatment in patient pain beliefs and coping are associated with concurrent changes in patient functioning. Pain. 2007;131:38–47. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Jensen MP, Turner JA, Romano JM, Fisher LD. Comparative reliability and validity of chronic pain intensity measures. Pain. 1999;83:157–162. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(99)00101-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Jensen MP, Turner JA, Romano JM, Lawler BK. Relationship of pain-specific beliefs to chronic pain adjustment. Pain. 1994;57:301–309. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(94)90005-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Jensen MP, Turner JA, Romano JM, Strom SE. The Chronic Pain Coping Inventory: development and preliminary validation. Pain. 1995;60:203–216. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(94)00118-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Keefe FJ, Affleck G, Lefebvre JC, Starr K, Caldwell DS, Tennen H. Pain coping strategies and coping efficacy in rheumatoid arthritis: a daily process analysis. Pain. 1997;69:35–42. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(96)03246-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Keefe FJ, Williams DA. A comparison of coping strategies in chronic pain patients in different age groups. J Gerontol. 1990;45:161–165. doi: 10.1093/geronj/45.4.p161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Livneh H, Martz E. Psychosocial adaptation to spinal cord injury as a function of time since injury. Int J Rehabil Res. 2003;26:191–200. doi: 10.1097/00004356-200309000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mariano MJ. Chronic pain and spinal cord injury. Clin J Pain. 1992;8:87–92. doi: 10.1097/00002508-199206000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Nielson WR, Jensen MP, Hill ML. An activity pacing subscale for the chronic pain coping inventory: development in a sample of patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Pain. 2001;89:111–115. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(00)00351-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Raichle KA, Hanley MA, Jensen MP, Cardenas D. Cognitions, coping, and social environment predict adjustment to pain in spinal cord injury. J Pain. 2007;8:718–729. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2007.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Raichle KA, Osborne TL, Jensen MP, Cardenas D. The reliability and validity of pain interference measures in persons with spinal cord injury. J Pain. 2006;7:179–186. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Rintala DH, Hart KA, Priebe MM. Predicting consistency of pain over a 10-year period in persons with spinal cord injury. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2004;41:75–88. doi: 10.1682/jrrd.2004.01.0075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Rintala DH, Loubser PG, Castro J, Hart KA, Fuhrer MJ. Chronic pain in a community-based sample of men with spinal cord injury: prevalence, severity, and relationship with impairment, disability, handicap, and subjective well-being. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1998;79:604–614. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(98)90032-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rosenstiel AK, Keefe FJ. The use of coping strategies in chronic low back pain patients: relationship to patient characteristics and current adjustment. Pain. 1983;17:33–44. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(83)90125-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Smeets RJ, Vlaeyen JW, Kester AD, Knottnerus JA. Reduction of pain catastrophizing mediates the outcome of both physical and cognitive-behavioral treatment in chronic low back pain. J Pain. 2006;7:261–271. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Spinhoven P, Ter Kuile M, Kole-Snijders AM, Hutten Mansfeld M, Den Ouden DJ, Vlaeyen JW. Catastrophizing and internal pain control as mediators of outcome in the multidisciplinary treatment of chronic low back pain. Eur J Pain. 2004;8:211–219. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2003.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Störmer S, Gerner HJ, Gruninger W, Metzmacher K, Follinger S, Wienke C, Aldinger W, Walker N, Zimmermann M, Paeslack V. Chronic pain/dysaesthesiae in spinal cord injury patients: results of a multicentre study. Spinal Cord. 1997;35:446–455. doi: 10.1038/sj.sc.3100411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Strong J, Ashton R, Chant D. The measurement of attitudes towards and beliefs about pain. Pain. 1992;48:227–236. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(92)90062-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Stroud MW, Turner JA, Jensen MP, Cardenas DD. Partner responses to pain behaviors are associated with depression and activity interference among persons with chronic pain and spinal cord injury. J Pain. 2006;7:91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Sullivan MJ, D’Eon JL. Relation between catastrophizing and depression in chronic pain patients. J Abnorm Psychol. 1990;99:260–263. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.99.3.260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Sullivan MJ, Thorn B, Haythornthwaite JA, Keefe F, Martin M, Bradley LA, Lefebvre JC. Theoretical perspectives on the relation between catastrophizing and pain. Clin J Pain. 2001;17:52–64. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200103000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Summers JD, Rapoff MA, Varghese G, Porter K, Palmer RE. Psychosocial factors in chronic spinal cord injury pain. Pain. 1991;47:183–189. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(91)90203-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Tait RC, Chibnall JT. Development of a brief version of the survey of pain attitudes. Pain. 1997;70:229–235. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(97)03330-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Turner JA, Cardenas DD, Warms CA, McClellan CB. Chronic pain associated with spinal cord injuries: a community survey. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2001;82:501–509. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2001.21855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Turner JA, Jensen MP, Romano JM. Do beliefs, coping, and catastrophizing independently predict functioning in patients with chronic pain? Pain. 2000;85:115–125. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(99)00259-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Turner JA, Jensen MP, Warms CA, Cardenas DD. Catastrophizing is associated with pain intensity, psychological distress, and pain-related disability among individuals with chronic pain after spinal cord injury. Pain. 2002;98:127–134. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(02)00045-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Turner JA, Mancl L, Aaron LA. Pain-related catastrophizing: a daily process study. Pain. 2004;110:103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Turner JA, Whitney C, Dworkin SF, Massoth D, Wilson L. Do changes in patient beliefs and coping strategies predict temporomandibular disorder treatment outcomes. Clin J Pain. 1995;11:177–188. doi: 10.1097/00002508-199509000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Tyler EJ, Jensen MP, Engel JM, Schwartz L. The reliability and validity of pain interference measures in persons with cerebral palsy. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2002;83:236–239. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2002.27466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Ware JE, Snow KK, Kosinski M, Gandek B. SF-36 Health Survey: Manual and Interpretation Guide. Lincoln, RI: QualityMetric Incorporated; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Warms CA, Turner JA, Marshall HM, Cardenas DD. Treatments for chronic pain associated with spinal cord injuries: many are tried, few are helpful. Clin J Pain. 2002;18:154–163. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200205000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Widerstrom-Noga EG, Turk DC. Types and effectiveness of treatments used by people with chronic pain associated with spinal cord injuries: influence of pain and psychosocial characteristics. Spinal Cord. 2003;41:600–609. doi: 10.1038/sj.sc.3101511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Widerstrom-Noga EG, Duncan R, Turk DC. Psychosocial profiles of people with pain associated with spinal cord injury. Clin J Pain. 2004;20:261–271. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200407000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Zimet GD, Dahlem NW, Zimet SG, Farley GK. The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. J Pers Assess. 1988;52:30–41. doi: 10.1080/00223891.1990.9674095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES