Abstract
In many animals in which females store sperm, males may detect female mating status and, in order to outcompete rival sperm, increase ejaculate size when copulating with non-virgin females. Although most studies have been restricted to organisms with separate sexes, theoretical models suggest that sperm competition should also be an important selective agent shaping life-history traits in simultaneous hermaphrodites. Nevertheless, the empirical support for ejaculate adjustment in a mating opportunity is scarce in hermaphrodites. In the present study, we performed a double-mating experiment to determine whether earthworms (Eisenia andrei) detect the mating status of their partners and whether they respond by adjusting their ejaculate. We found that earthworms triplicated the donated sperm when mating with a non-virgin mate. Moreover, such increases were greater when the worms were mated with larger (more fecund) partners, indicating that earthworms perform a fine-tune control of ejaculate volume. The results of the present study suggest that, under high intensity of sperm competition, partner evaluation is subject to intense selection in hermaphrodite animals, and donors are selective about to whom they donate how much sperm.
Keywords: sperm competition, hermaphrodites, partner assessment, sensory abilities, earthworms
1. Introduction
In ch. IX of his book ‘The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex’, Charles Darwin (1871) considered sexual selection to be restricted to higher animals and pointed out, for example, that ‘Annelids apparently stand too low in the scale, for the individuals of either sex to exert any choice in selecting a partner, or for the individuals of the same sex to struggle together in rivalry’. Although the cognitive abilities needed to assess mate rivalry may be weak in hermaphroditic invertebrates (e.g. Charnov 1987), the theory predicts that sexual selection and especially sperm competition may also be an important selective force shaping the mating behaviour in hermaphrodites (Charnov 1996; Michiels 1998).
Simultaneous hermaphrodites have both functional female and male reproductive organs and multiple matings are common (Baur 1994; Angeloni et al. 2003; Monroy et al. 2003), and sperm storage organs have evolved in many species (Michiels 1998). It is assumed that hermaphrodites have a limited amount of reproductive resources for both sexual functions (Charnov 1996; Schärer et al. 2005). Given that the costs of sperm production are often non-trivial (Dewsbury 1982), hermaphrodites should optimize the amount of sperm allocated to the current partner while reserving enough sperm for future matings (Wedell et al. 2002). Since increased sperm production may reduce resources that can be allocated to egg production (De Visser et al. 1994; Lorenzi et al. 2007) and decrease somatic growth or maintenance (Van Voorhies 1992; Sella & Lorenzi 2003), hermaphrodites are expected to be prudent with their expensive male reserves (Koene & Ter Maat 2007). However, when males compete with rivals for fertilization, larger ejaculates are predicted, especially to high-quality mates (Cook & Gage 1995; Martin & Hosken 2002; Wedell et al. 2002; Friberg 2006). Thus, opportunistic mating decisions should be selected in hermaphrodites, but empirical support for ejaculate adjustment according to sperm competition intensity is scarce (but see Anthes et al. 2006).
Some recent evidence supports increased sperm production in hermaphrodites when raised in large groups (Trouvé et al. 1999; Schärer & Ladurner 2003; Tan et al. 2004) and also in individuals exposed to enlarged mating groups (Brauer et al. 2007; but see Lorenzi et al. 2005), which indicates that hermaphrodites adjust sex allocation to the number of potential rivals (but see Locher & Baur 2000). The increased investment in male function with group size may be attributed to higher mating rates, or to the ability of individuals to differentiate the mating history of their partners and adjust sperm production accordingly. Nevertheless, there is little evidence that hermaphrodites can evaluate the mating history of their partners to estimate the intensity of sperm competition that their ejaculates will face in a particular mating event (Baur et al. 1998; but see Anthes et al. 2006), as occurs in many insects (Wedell et al. 2002; Uhía & Cordero 2005; Friberg 2006) and vertebrates (del Barco-Trillo & Ferkin 2004).
In the present study, we performed a double-mating experiment to determine whether redworms respond to the mating status of their partners by adjusting their ejaculate. In this simple situation over the sperm competition continuum (no competition and one competitor), an increase in the ejaculate size is predicted (Wedell et al. 2002). The redworm, Eisenia andrei, is a simultaneously hermaphroditic earthworm that lives at high densities where multiple mating is common (Monroy et al. 2003). Consequently, strategies to overcome strong sperm competition are expected. Indeed, redworms display a prolonged courtship that involves short and repeated touches between partners before mating attachment; they also spend a long time in copulation (Grove & Cowley 1926), offering possibilities for partner evaluation. Although sperm digestion is widespread in hermaphrodites, which makes it difficult to distinguish sperm competition and mating investment (Michiels 1998; Greeff & Michiels 1999a), redworms are unable to digest received allosperm (Richards & Fleming 1982). Redworms therefore constitute an excellent model for testing the effect of sperm competition on mating behaviour.
2. Material and methods
(a) Study animal
Eisenia andrei (Bouche 1972) is an epigeic hermaphrodite earthworm (Oligochaeta, Lumbricidae) with a worldwide distribution due to its tolerance to a wide range of temperature and moisture conditions (Domínguez et al. 2005). During mating, both worms are attached in an inverse position by their ventral sides and there is transfer of sperm from the male pores up to the spermathecae of the partner with the help of epidermal organs such as tubercula pubertatis, which together with the mucus secreted, ensure perfect fixation of the earthworms during mating (Grove & Cowley 1926). During copulation, sperm are exchanged simultaneously and reciprocally and stored in the two pairs of spermathecae, located on the dorsal side of ninth and tenth body segments; these are spherical sacs that store the sperm until cocoon laying (figure 1).
Figure 1.
Spermathecae of the redworm, E. andrei. (a) Spermathecae are located on the dorsal side of ninth and tenth body segments. The photography shows one dissected earthworm with the dorsal body wall removed to the right. Spermathecae can be observed as two pairs of brilliant spherical sacs. The remaining white mass corresponds to the seminal vesicles. (b) Freshly dissected spermatheca showing a bundle of sperm stored after two copulations.
(b) Collection and maintenance
Sixty hatchlings of E. andrei were obtained from a laboratory stock cultured at 20±2°C. The laboratory stock was large enough (more than 10 000 individuals) to prevent endogamy. Hatchlings were placed in separate plastic Petri dishes to ensure that they were virgins at the time of experimental matings. Vermicompost and cow manure were supplied ad libitum as breeding medium. The dishes were kept in the darkness in a scientific incubator at 25°C and high humidity. Earthworms were inspected weekly until sexual maturity was attained, detected as the development of tubercula pubertatis and clitellum.
(c) Mating experiments
We manipulated earthworm-mating history by performing a double-mating experiment. Forty-two mature and virgin earthworms were randomly assigned to three experimental groups (first partner, second partner and focal recipient). One week before the start of the experiment, earthworms were marked by a tiny light burn on different segments behind the clitellum, so that they could be recognized after matings. The body mass of each earthworm was recorded prior to the experiment, and there were no significant differences among experimental groups (F2,41=0.62, p=0.55).
First, we placed 14 pairs of mature virgin earthworms (first partner×focal recipient) housed in a Petri dish with vermicompost and cow manure. All the pairs were examined twice daily until the appearance of spermatophores, indicative of copulation (Monroy et al. 2003). When the pairs had completed mating, one earthworm was removed (focal recipient) and placed with another mature virgin earthworm (second partner) for a second mating (n=14) and examined twice daily. In both cases, the earthworms were randomly assigned to the mating pairs. The estimated time elapsed between two matings was 31±6 hours, within the natural range of multiple matings (Monroy et al. 2003). After matings, all earthworms were fixed in formaldehyde 4% : ethanol 96% (1 : 1) and preserved with formaldehyde 4% in plastic tubes until their later dissection.
(d) Estimation of sperm volume
Spermathecae (figure 1) were dissected under a Nikon SMZ1500 stereomicroscope at various magnifications and were compressed to a uniform thickness of 66.3 μm under a supported cover-slip on a slide. The sperm mass was photographed with a Nikon Digital Camera DXM1200F. The sperm area of each spermatheca was measured twice by the use of AnalySIS software and the mean of the two measurements was used to estimate the sperm volume as the area multiplied by the separation between cover-slip and slide (66.3 μm; see Cordero & Miller 1992). Sperm showed homogeneous density in all preparations (measured by colour intensity in the AnalySIS software). Thus, the total volume of sperm received in the matings was estimated as the sum of the volume of the four spermathecae. The earthworms included in the experiment contained sperm in the four spermathecae, except for two earthworms that showed three spermathecae with sperm and an empty one.
(e) Statistical analysis
Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) were used to estimate the correlation in the volume of sperm in spermathecae of the same earthworm. Paired t-tests were used to determine the effect of the mating status of the focal recipient on the volume of donated sperm to the first and second partner.
Homogeneity of the variance of sperm volume was tested by Levene's test for equality of variances. Mean differences in the sperm volume received after a single copulation and two consecutive copulations were analysed using a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with experimental group (first partner and focal recipient) as fixed factor and individual earthworms within mating pairs as repeated measure factor, controlling for heterogeneous treatment variances (PROC MIXED in SAS; Littell et al. 1996). The sperm stored by the first partner was used to estimate the volume of sperm transferred after a single copulation. Since earthworms were randomly assigned to experimental groups, we assumed that the distribution of sperm donated was similar in both groups of partners (first partner and focal recipient) in the first mating. The volume of sperm transferred by the second partner to the focal recipient was estimated by randomization (Monte Carlo; 1000 simulations) as the total volume of sperm in the spermathecae of the focal recipient minus the volume of sperm in the spermathecae of one randomly selected first partner. The results obtained by the randomization procedure are robust to any assumption about the exact amount of sperm that a earthworm (focal earthworm) has received during the first copulation.
The statistical significance of the difference in the volume of sperm in the first and second partners was estimated by means of Monte Carlo analysis (Manly 1997). After 10 000 simulations, the distribution of the t-test statistic in the original data, which measures the discrepancy in the sperm volume donated by the first and second partners (estimated by randomization), was calculated. The observations were then randomly allocated to the two samples and the t-test statistic was recomputed. After 10 000 simulations, the distribution of the t-test statistic under the null hypothesis was calculated. Finally, the estimated distribution of the t-test statistic in the original data was compared with the randomized t-test distribution (null hypothesis). Approximate p-values were calculated as the probability of randomized t-test values exceeding the distribution of t-test statistics based on estimated data. Similar results were achieved when the volume of sperm transferred by the second partner to the focal recipient was estimated by subtracting the amount of sperm present in the first partner from the total amount of sperm present in its focal partner after two matings, and the significance was tested by a paired t-test (data not shown). Data are expressed as mean±s.e.
3. Results
Sperm volume stored in the spermathecae was similar in the first and second partners of the focal redworms (paired t-test, t13=0.10, p=0.91), which indicated that there was no sperm depletion after two consecutive copulations. Sperm was similarly distributed in the four spermathecae. Thus, after a single copulation, sperm volume was correlated among the four spermathecae within individuals (ICC, r=0.86, p<0.001), and the same occurred in the focal redworms after two copulations (ICC, r=0.80, p<0.001). Interestingly, the total sperm volume stored in the focal redworm after two consecutive copulations was 3.9 times greater and more variable than that after a single copulation (figure 2a; Levene's test for homogeneity of variance, p=0.003; GLMM, F1,13=43.96, p<0.0001).
Figure 2.
Comparison of the sperm stored in relation to the number of matings. (a) Total volume of sperm (mm3) received after copulation with one or two donors. In grey, the sperm volume donated by the second donor to non-virgin recipients, as estimated by randomization. (b) Distribution of Monte Carlo simulations of the t-statistic values for the original data, which measures the discrepancy in the volume of sperm donated by the first and second donors (estimated by randomization) and the t-statistic values for randomly allocated observations (null hypothesis).
Since earthworms were randomly assigned to the experimental groups, we estimated the sperm volume transferred by the second partner as the total sperm volume in the spermathecae of the focal recipient minus the randomized sperm volume in the spermathecae of the first partner. Thus, the estimated sperm volume transferred by the second partner was 0.375±0.06 mm3 (n=14), 3.3 times greater than that transferred in the first copulation (Monte Carlo analysis, p=0.0001; figure 2b).
The total sperm volume stored after two copulations was related to the recipient body mass (figure 3; r14=0.65, p=0.018), but not to the body mass of donors (first partner: r14=0.37, p=0.19; second partner: r14=0.42, p=0.13). This effect was not found in the first copulation, where the total sperm volume stored was related neither to the receiver body mass (figure 3; r14=0.11, p=0.71) nor to the donor mass (r14=0.06, p=0.83).
Figure 3.
Total volume of sperm (mm3) stored in the spermathecae after copulation with one or two donors in relation to the recipient (own) body mass (interaction recipient body mass×number of donors, F1,24=5.925, p=0.02). Dashed line indicates the expected sperm volume stored in the recipient if the sperm volume donated by the second donor be similar to that donated by the first donor.
4. Discussion
In this study, we found that earthworms responded to the mating status of their partners and triplicated the donated sperm when they mate with a non-virgin mate. Moreover, such increases were greater when the worms were mated with larger partners, indicating that earthworms perform a fine-tune control of ejaculate volume. These results indicate that redworms have evolved mechanisms to detect and adjust ejaculate investment in order to maximize fertilization opportunities (Wedell et al. 2002). Our study suggests that sperm competition (Parker 1970) is a powerful evolutionary force that has influenced the mating behaviour in earthworms.
Sperm competition occurs when multiple matings lead to the simultaneous presence of live sperm from two or more individuals within a female's reproductive tract. In this multiple-mating experiment, we found that redworms equally distributed the sperm among the four spermathecae and that the volume of sperm donated by the second partner, although more variable, was probably added to the sperm stored from the preceding copulation. Therefore, when redworms copulate with a mated partner, their ejaculates compete with the rival sperm to fertilize the ova. Under sperm mixing from multiple donors, precedence rules probably approximate to ‘fair raffles’ mixing, and sperm competition will select for an increased amount of transferred sperm (Parker 1998).
Accordingly, we found that redworms are able to perceive partner mating status and triplicate their ejaculates after detecting a risk of sperm competition (a previously mated partner). Interestingly, this finding also reveals that under no sperm competition risk redworms are prudent in their ejaculate expenditure, even when paired with high-quality mates, as that occurs in the first copulation of earthworms reared in isolation. These results indicate that they may regulate the duration of copulation or, alternatively, that they may have mechanisms that prevent all sperm being released in a single mating event. Although we did not find sperm depletion after two consecutive ejaculations, the prudent sperm allocation shown by redworms is in line with the models suggesting non-trivial costs of ejaculate production (Greeff & Michiels 1999b).
Our results indicate that redworms boost their ejaculate when paired with recently mated partners. However, interestingly, the volume of sperm donated to non-virgin partners was more variable than that transferred to virgin recipients. This variability may indicate that under high sperm competition intensity, resource allocation to sperm may become expensive (Greeff & Michiels 1999b), and sperm donors become more selective about to whom they donate sperm (Michiels et al. 2003). To test this prediction, we examined the relationship between sperm volume donated and partner quality, estimated as body mass. As in many hermaphrodites, egg production in earthworms increases with body mass (Domínguez et al. 1997) and larger partners are preferred (Monroy et al. 2005). Thus, if sperm is costly, redworms should boost their ejaculates under high sperm competition intensity, especially when paired with larger partners, since the potential benefits are greater. According to this prediction, we found that the total sperm volume stored after two copulations was related to the recipient body mass and this effect was not found in the first copulation. Thus, the estimated volume transferred to larger non-virgin partners (above 0.85 g) was five times greater than that transferred to virgin partners (figure 3). These results indicate that redworms bias potential sperm competition in their favour, particularly when they copulate with more fecund partners.
Phenotypic plasticity, as we have found in sperm allocation, is expected to be favoured when the environment is unpredictable on a time scale that is short relative to generation time (de Jong 1995). Redworms live in populations in which multiple matings are common, but density and mating rates oscillate frequently and probably unpredictably (Monroy et al. 2006). Opportunistic ejaculate expenditure according to the fluctuations in mating rates should be advantageous, favouring the allocation of resources to egg production under low mating rates, and higher allocation to male reproduction in a scenario of higher sperm competition. As it has been pointed out, this plasticity may explain why hermaphroditism is maintained in fluctuating large and dense populations (Brauer et al. 2007).
We do not know the cues that redworms use to assess partner mating status, but tactile and chemical cues are probable sources of this information (see Schleicherová et al. 2006). Earthworms show a prolonged courtship with short and repeated touches between partners before mating and they spend a long time in copulation with constriction movements between partners, which provides ample opportunity for partner assessment (e.g. Michiels et al. 2001). Earthworms are sensitive to chemicals having large numbers of chemoreceptors all over their bodies, most of them concentrated in the prostomium and anterior segments (Wallwork 1983). In addition, the epithelium in the mouth region accommodates groups of sensory cells that are associated with the detection of mucus secretions from other earthworms (Edwards & Lofty 1972). In his last work, Charles Darwin devoted himself entirely to the investigation of earthworm biology (Darwin 1881). Contrary to his previous thinking (Darwin 1871) but in accordance with his observations, he concluded that earthworms possess more cognitive potential than was generally assumed. The results of the present study are consistent with this view and show that the hermaphroditic redworm recognition system is actually highly efficient in terms of mate evaluation and is important to the reproductive success of these animals.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Kevin Butt, Judith Morales, Roxana Torres, Christine Francis, Constantino Macias and Marta Lores for their comments on the manuscript. We also want to thank Nils Anthes and the other anonymous reviewer for their constructive discussions that clearly improved this manuscript. The study was supported by the Spanish Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia (CGL2006-11928). A.V. was supported by a Ramon y Cajal Fellowship (Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia, Spain).
References
- Angeloni L, Bradbury J.W, Burton R.S. Multiple mating, paternity and body size in a simultaneous hermaphrodite, Aplysia californica. Behav. Ecol. 2003;14:554–560. doi:10.1093/beheco/arg033 [Google Scholar]
- Anthes N, Putz A, Michiels N.K. Hermaphrodite sex role preferences: the role of partner body size, mating history and female fitness in the sea slug Chelidonura sandrana. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2006;60:359–367. doi:10.1007/s00265-006-0173-5 [Google Scholar]
- Baur B. Multiple paternity and individual variation in sperm precedence in the simultaneously hermaphroditic land snail Arianta arbustorum. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1994;35:413–421. doi:10.1007/s002650050114 [Google Scholar]
- Baur B, Locher R, Baur A. Sperm allocation in the simultaneously hermaphroditic land snail Arianta arbustorum. Anim. Behav. 1998;56:839–845. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0855. doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.0855 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brauer V.S, Schärer L, Michiels N.K. Phenotypically flexible sex allocation in a simultaneous hermaphrodite. Evolution. 2007;61:216–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00018.x. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00018.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charnov E.L. Sexuality and hermaphroditism in barnacles: a natural selection approach. In: Southward A.J, editor. Biology of barnacles. Balkema; Rotterdam, The Netherlands: 1987. pp. 89–103. [Google Scholar]
- Charnov E.L. Sperm competition and sex allocation in simultaneous hermaphrodites. Evol. Ecol. 1996;10:457–462. doi:10.1007/BF01237878 [Google Scholar]
- Cook P.A, Gage M.J.G. Effects of risk of sperm competition on the numbers of eupyrene and apyrene sperm ejaculated by the moth Plodia interpunctella (Lepidoptera: Pyralidae) Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1995;36:261–268. [Google Scholar]
- Cordero A, Miller P.L. Sperm transfer, displacement and precedence in Ischnura graellsii (Odonata: Coenagrionidae) Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1992;30:261–267. doi:10.1007/BF00166711 [Google Scholar]
- Darwin C.R. John Murray; London, UK: 1871. The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. [Google Scholar]
- Darwin C.R. John Murray; London, UK: 1881. The formation of vegetable mould, through the action of worms, with observations on their habits. [Google Scholar]
- de Jong G. Phenotypic plasticity as a product of selection in a variable environment. Am. Nat. 1995;145:493–512. doi: 10.1086/285542. doi:10.1086/285752 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- del Barco-Trillo J, Ferkin M.H. Male mammals respond to a risk of sperm competition conveyed by odours of conspecific males. Nature. 2004;431:446–449. doi: 10.1038/nature02845. doi:10.1038/nature02845 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Visser J.A.G.M, Ter Maat A, Zonneveld C. Energy budgets and reproductive allocation in the simultaneous hermaphrodite pond snail, Lymnaea stagnalis (L.): a trade-off between male and female function. Am. Nat. 1994;144:861–867. doi:10.1086/285712 [Google Scholar]
- Dewsbury D.A. Ejaculate cost and male choice. Am. Nat. 1982;119:601–610. doi:10.1086/283938 [Google Scholar]
- Domínguez J, Briones M.J, Mato S. Effect of the diet on growth and reproduction of Eisenia andrei. Pedobiologia. 1997;4:566–576. [Google Scholar]
- Domínguez J, Velando A, Ferreiro A. Are Eisenia fetida (Savigny, 1826) and Eisenia andrei Bouché (1972) (Oligochaeta, Lumbricidae) different biological species? Pedobiologia. 2005;49:81–87. doi:10.1016/j.pedobi.2004.08.005 [Google Scholar]
- Edwards C.A, Lofty J.R. Chapman and Hall; London, UK: 1972. Biology of earthworms. [Google Scholar]
- Friberg U. Male perception of female mating status: its effect on copulation duration, sperm defence and female fitness. Anim. Behav. 2006;72:1259–1268. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.03.021 [Google Scholar]
- Greeff J.M, Michiels N.K. Low potential for sexual selection in simultaneously hermaphroditic animals. Proc. R. Soc. B. 1999a;266:1671–1676. doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0830 [Google Scholar]
- Greeff J.M, Michiels N.K. Sperm digestion and reciprocal sperm transfer can drive hermaphrodite sex allocation to equality. Am. Nat. 1999b;153:421–430. doi: 10.1086/303184. doi:10.1086/303184 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grove A.J, Cowley L.F. On the reproductive processes of the brandling worm, Eisenia foetida (Sav.) Q. J. Microsc. Sci. 1926;70:559–581. [Google Scholar]
- Koene J.M, Ter Maat A. Coolidge effect in pond snails: male motivation in a simultaneous hermaphrodite. BMC Evol. Biol. 2007;7:212. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-7-212. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-7-212 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Littell R.C, Milliken G.A, Stroup W.W, Wolfinger R.D. SAS Institute; Cary, NC: 1996. SAS system for mixed models. [Google Scholar]
- Locher R, Baur B. Sperm delivery and egg production of the simultaneously hermaphroditic land snail Arianta arbustorum exposed to an increased sperm competition risk. Invertebr. Reprod. Dev. 2000;38:53–60. [Google Scholar]
- Lorenzi M.C, Sella G, Schleicherová D, Ramella L. Outcrossing hermaphroditic polychaete worms adjust their sex allocation to social conditions. J. Evol. Biol. 2005;18:1341–1347. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2005.00916.x. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2005.00916.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorenzi M.C, Schleicherová D, Sella G. Sex adjustments are not functionally costly in simultaneous hermaphrodites. Mar. Biol. 2007;153:599–604. doi:10.1007/s00227-007-0833-7 [Google Scholar]
- Manly B.F.J. 2nd edn. Chapman & Hall; London, UK: 1997. Randomization, bootstrap and Monte Carlo methods in biology. [Google Scholar]
- Martin O.Y, Hosken D.J. Strategic ejaculation in the common dung fly Sepsis cynipsea. Anim. Behav. 2002;63:541–546. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1929 [Google Scholar]
- Michiels N.K. Mating conflicts and sperm competition in simultaneous hermaphrodites. In: Birkhead T.R, Møller A.P, editors. Sperm competition and sexual selection. Academic Press; London, UK: 1998. pp. 219–254. [Google Scholar]
- Michiels N.K, Hohner A, Vorndran I.C. Precopulatory mate assessment in relation to body size in the earthworm Lumbricus terrestris: avoidance of dangerous liaisons? Behav. Ecol. 2001;12:612–618. doi:10.1093/beheco/12.5.612 [Google Scholar]
- Michiels N.K, Raven-Yoo-Heufes A, Brockmann K. Sperm trading and sex roles in the hermaphroditic opisthobranch sea slug Navanax inermis: eager females or opportunistic males. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2003;78:105–116. doi:10.1046/j.1095-8312.2003.00135.x [Google Scholar]
- Monroy F, Aira M, Velando A, Domínguez J. Have spermatophores in Eisenia fetida (Oligochaeta, Lumbricidae) any reproductive role? Pedobiologia. 2003;47:526–529. [Google Scholar]
- Monroy F, Aira M, Velando A, Domínguez J. Size-assortative mating in the earthworm Eisenia fetida (Oligochaeta, Lumbricidae) J. Ethol. 2005;23:69–70. doi:10.1007/s10164-004-0127-1 [Google Scholar]
- Monroy F, Aira M, Domínguez J, Velando A. Seasonal population dynamics of Eisenia fetida (Savigny, 1826) (Oligochaeta, Lumbricidae) in the field. C. R. Biol. 2006;329:912–915. doi: 10.1016/j.crvi.2006.08.001. doi:10.1016/j.crvi.2006.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parker G.A. Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Biol. Rev. 1970;45:525–567. [Google Scholar]
- Parker G.A. Sperm competition and the evolution of ejaculates towards a theory base. In: Birkhead T.R, Møller A.P, editors. Sperm competition and sexual selection. Academic Press; London, UK: 1998. pp. 3–54. [Google Scholar]
- Richards K.S, Fleming T.P. Spermatozoal phagocytosis by the spermathecae of Dendrobaena subrubicunda and other lumbricids (Oligochaeta, Annelida) Int. J. Invertebr. Reprod. 1982;5:233–241. [Google Scholar]
- Schärer L, Ladurner P. Phenotypic plastic sex allocation in a simultaneous hermaphrodite. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2003;270:935–941. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2323. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2323 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schärer L, Sandner P, Michiels N.K. Trade-off between male and female allocation in the simultaneously hermaphroditic flatworm Macrostomum sp. J. Evol. Biol. 2005;18:396–404. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00827.x. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00827.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schleicherová D, Lorenzi M.C, Sella G. How outcrossing hermaphrodites sense the presence of conspecifics and suppress female allocation. Behav. Ecol. 2006;17:1–5. doi:10.1093/beheco/ari093 [Google Scholar]
- Sella G, Lorenzi M.C. Increased sperm allocation delays body growth in a protandrous simultaneous hermaphrodite. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2003;78:149–154. doi:10.1046/j.1095-8312.2003.00167.x [Google Scholar]
- Tan G.N, Govedich F.R, Burd M. Social group size, potential sperm competition and reproductive investment in a hermaphroditic leech, Helobdella papillornata (Euhirudinea: glossiphoniidae) J. Evol. Biol. 2004;17:574–580. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00692.x. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00692.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trouvé S, Jourdane J, Renaud F, Durand P, Morand S. Adaptive sex allocation in a simultaneous hermaphrodite. Evolution. 1999;53:1599–1604. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1999.tb05423.x. doi:10.2307/2640905 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uhía E, Cordero R.A. Male damselflies detect female mating status: importance for postcopulatory sexual selection. Anim. Behav. 2005;69:797–804. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.08.005 [Google Scholar]
- Van Voorhies W.A. Production of sperm reduces nematode life-span. Nature. 1992;360:456–458. doi: 10.1038/360456a0. doi:10.1038/360456a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallwork J.A. Camelot Press; Southampton, UK: 1983. Earthworm biology. [Google Scholar]
- Wedell N, Gage M.J.G, Parker G.A. Sperm competition, male prudence and sperm-limited females. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2002;17:313–319. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02533-8 [Google Scholar]