Abstract
A study by Rowe et al. reported a winning bias for judo athletes wearing a blue outfit relative to those wearing a white one during the 2004 Olympics. It was suggested that blue is associated with a higher likelihood of winning through differential effects of colour on opponent visibility and/or an intimidating effect on the opponent. However, we argue that there is no colour effect on winning in judo. We show that alternative factors, namely allocation biases, asymmetries in prior experience and differences in recovery time are possible confounding factors in the analysis of Rowe et al. After controlling for these factors, we found no difference in blue and white wins. We further analysed contest outcomes of 71 other major judo tournaments and also found no winning bias. Our findings have implications for sports policy makers: they suggest that a white–blue outfit pairing ensures an equal level of play.
Keywords: sexual selection, aggression, status signalling, judo, competition outcome, human performance
1. Introduction
Certain colours, like red and orange, are sexually selected traits signalling male quality, aggression and dominance in a wide variety of organisms (e.g. Rowland 1989; Pryke et al. 2001; Maan et al. 2004; reviewed in Andersson 1994; Hill & McGraw 2006). In aggressive interactions, these colours may increase the likelihood of winning (Evan & Norris 1996; Baube 1997; Cuthill et al. 1997; Dijkstra et al. 2005; Pryke & Griffith 2006). This winning bias has traditionally been attributed to the inherently intimidating effect of red coloration on opponents. Interestingly, Hill & Barton (2005) reported a similar effect in human combat sports; they found that wearing of a red outfit was associated with a higher winning probability relative to wearing a blue outfit in the men's division of Greco-Roman wrestling, freestyle wrestling, boxing and tae kwon do. Recently, Rowe et al. (2005) proposed the idea that this effect is not unique for red coloration. They found in another human combat sport, judo, a significant winning bias for athletes wearing a blue outfit (judogi) compared with those wearing a white judogi during the 2004 men's Olympics. Here we argue that there exists no winning bias for blue and that their findings were confounded.
Two, not mutually exclusive, mechanisms have been proposed to underlie the winning bias for blue. First, Rowe et al. (2005) attribute the effect to blue and white colour having differential effects on opponent visibility. The white judogi may be more visible than the blue judogi, allowing the athlete in blue to better anticipate the movements of his (white) opponent. A second explanation was given by Barton & Hill (2005). They suggested that blue coloration is brighter than white, and may therefore have an intimidating effect on opponents, as was originally proposed for red coloration.
Both explanations are unlikely. Considering the first explanation, we believe that a differential perception effect is unlikely to occur in a situation where athletes fight at close quarters in brightly lit areas, as in judo (see Barton & Hill 2005).
Considering the second explanation, inherently intimidating effects of colour on opponents have been suggested for red coloration in a wide range of animal species (e.g. Dijkstra et al. 2005; Pryke & Griffith 2006) but never for blue coloration. For example, in a non-human primate, the intensity of red coloration on the nose correlated with dominance status (Setchell & Wickings 2005). In humans, anger may trigger increases in facial blood flow (Drummond 1999). In people with a fair skin, this facial reddening may alter the behaviour of others in social encounters (Drummond & Quah 2001). Therefore, the traditional intimidating nature of red coloration may influence dominance in humans (Hill & Barton 2005). Such an effect cannot be logically attributed to blue coloration.
We argue that athletes wearing a blue judogi are not more likely to win than those wearing a white judogi, and suggest that the results obtained by Rowe et al. (2005) were compromised by three confounding factors: (i) seeding system, (ii) inclusion of repechage rounds, and (iii) differences in recovery time.
First, wearing of judogi colour was not completely determined at random during the 2004 Olympics, even though athletes switch blue and white judogi depending on their position in the tournament bracket (electronic supplementary material). Top-ranked athletes were ‘seeded’ so as to avoid matching them against one another before the semi-finals; seeded athletes were given the blue judogi by default in the first round, causing an athlete quality–colour association and a winning bias for blue. In their corrigendum (Rowe et al. 2006), this allocation bias was controlled for by excluding first-round matches. However, we show that this correction was insufficient, because seeded athletes have a higher chance to wear blue in the second and third rounds too, causing the athlete quality–colour association to persist (electronic supplementary material).
A second fundamental methodological problem concerns the fact that Rowe et al. (2005) included the loser's pool, or repechage in their analysis. In international tournaments, the modified double elimination system is used (www.ijf.org); athletes who win move to the subsequent round up to the gold-medal final. Under certain conditions, those who lose get another chance in the loser's pool where they can maximally win the bronze medal (figure 1). Many repechage matches contain systematic asymmetries in prior experience (win–lose and/or number of previous matches fought) and judogi colour. First, in approximately 50% of matches, an asymmetry in prior win–lose experience exists because the athletes in blue had won their previous match, while the athletes in white had lost theirs. In the other 50% of the matches, this asymmetry in win–lose experience never occurs conversely with respect to judogi colour. Evidence indicates that prior defeat or victory can have a dramatic influence on the outcome of future aggressive encounters (Rutte et al. 2006). Second, an asymmetry can occur in the number of previous matches fought, with blue being more likely to have experienced one match less than his opponent in white (explained in figure 1). Possible asymmetries in prior experience associated with judogi colour require exclusion of the repechage matches.
Third, athletes wearing a blue judogi have on average in all international tournaments a slightly longer intercontest time interval than those who wear white, resulting in a longer recovery time for athletes in blue compared with those in white (N. van Dijk & K. Kienhuis 2007, personal communication). How this difference in intercontest interval comes about is explained in figure 1. In brief, each weight category is divided into four pools. Per pool matches take place sequentially from top to bottom, with little time in between matches, so that in the subsequent round the athlete in blue has had a longer recovery time than the one in white. Only in the gold-medal final are recovery times equal for the athletes in white and blue because the semi-finals are scheduled simultaneously.
In our study, we test whether there is a winning bias for blue in judo in two ways. First, we re-analysed the data of the 2004 Olympics of Rowe et al. (2005). If there is no effect of colour on winning, the winning bias for blue as reported by Rowe et al. should disappear when controlling for the confounding factors. In addition, if there is no effect of colour, we predict that the winning bias for blue declines over the tournament rounds when we include seeded athletes. This is because only the strongest competitors are expected to progress in the tournament, thereby reducing differences in athlete ability.
Second, we analysed in addition to the 2004 Olympics a total of 71 other international judo tournaments, thereby avoiding possible effects specific to a single tournament. We compared the number of blue and white winners in 501 gold-medal finals to avoid a biased sample due to the seeding system, asymmetries in prior experience and recovery time. We predict no colour-associated winning bias in the gold-medal finals.
2. Material and methods
(a) Tournaments analysed
For our re-analyses of Rowe et al. (2005)'s data, we will (obviously) use the results of the 2004 Olympics.
For the tournaments in our second additional analyses, we obtained as many contest outcomes as possible of major international tournaments held between 1996 and 2005. (The blue–white judogi pairings have been used in international competition since 1996 instead of white–white pairings to improve differentiating the two contestants.) Our dataset included 72 international tournaments (Olympic Championships, n=2; World Championships, n=7 (including three Junior World Championships, less than 20 years); European Championships, n=9; Super A Tournaments, n=15 and A Tournaments, n=39).
The results are from the International Judo Federation and the European Judo Federation. In all analyses, we focus on the men's division, following Hill & Barton (2005) and Rowe et al. (2005), and on the winner's pool (thus excluding repechage matches). In some tournaments, there is an open weight category; in this non-Olympic category, athletes from all weight categories can compete. We excluded this category in our analysis, because we expect substantial ability asymmetries between athletes due to large weight differences.
(b) Procedure
(i) Analysis 1: re-analysis of the 2004 Olympics
We compared the number of blue and white winners after excluding matches involving a seeded athlete. We excluded contests involving a seeded athlete, because seeded athletes had a bigger chance of wearing blue in the first three tournament rounds. We found that 11% of contestants were seeded. They started in a blue judogi in their first match with a winning chance of 84%. In Rowe et al. (2006)'s corrigendum, this association between the blue judogi and winning was removed by excluding first-round matches. However, this is insufficient, because the association also affected other rounds; seeded athletes had a 66.7% chance of wearing blue in the second and third rounds, with winning probabilities of 85.7 and 55.6%, respectively (see electronic supplementary material for an example of a tournament programme and explanation of how the seeding system can affect the probability of wearing blue).
In our analysis, we did not control for differences in recovery time. In the gold-medal finals, recovery times between athletes are equal (see below). However, the sample of the gold-medal finals is too small (n=7) to make valuable comparisons. We also tested whether the difference in athlete quality as well as the association between winning probability and judogi colour declined over the tournament rounds when seeded athletes are included in the analysis.
(ii) Analysis 2: comparing the number of white and blue winners in 72 tournaments
We compared blue and white winners in 501 gold-medal finals of 72 tournaments.
We only focus on the finals because (as explained in §1) (i) the seeding system can cause non-random allocation of judogi colour as in the 2004 Olympics and (ii) intercontest intervals (and thus recovery times) are not the same for athletes in blue and white. To check whether the seeding system indeed can cause the non-random allocation, we performed an analysis on the eight most important tournaments held between 2003 and 2005 (2003 and 2005 World Championships; 2004 Olympic Championships; 2003, 2004 and 2005 European Championships; 2005 Hamburg Super A Tournament and 2005 Paris Super A Tournament). Regulations concerning the seeding system were obtained from the International Judo Federation, the European Judo Union, the Dutch Judo Federation and the Swiss Judo and Jiu-Jitsu Federation. We then verified regulations governing the allocation of seedings by examining the contest sheets of the tournaments (downloaded from www.ippon.org). To this end, we checked whether athletes ranking numbers 1–7 in the most recent World and Olympic or European Championships, were consistently separated over the pools; we then checked whether they started in the blue judogi by default. We found that in all tournaments except in the 2004 European Championships such ‘high-quality’ athletes were seeded. The seeded athletes were given the blue judogi by default in their first match of all tournaments except in the 2003 European Championships (table 1). The starting position on the contest scheme determines the chance to wear blue when an athlete progresses through the tournament. We found that the starting position of seeded athletes is such that the association between winning and blue judogi may affect all rounds up to the pool finals. This is fully explained in the electronic supplementary material. Therefore, correcting for seeds should be done by excluding those affected matches.
Table 1.
tournament | percentage of seeds starting in blue |
---|---|
Olympics 2004 | 100 |
World Championships 2003 | 100 |
World Championships 2005 | 100 |
European Championships 2003 | 74 |
European Championships 2004 | no seeds |
European Championships 2005 | 100 |
Super World Cup Paris 2005 | 100 |
Super World Cup Hamburg 2005 | 100 |
(c) Statistical analysis
We used Χ2-tests to compare the number of blue and white winners. Logistic regression was used to test whether the proportion of blue winners declined over the tournament rounds in the 2004 Olympics. Tests of significance were two-tailed, unless we have a one-directional alternative hypothesis. Analyses were done using SPSS v. 12.0.1.
3. Results
(a) Analysis 1: re-analysis of the 2004 Olympics
When excluding the matches involving a seeded athlete, the winning bias for blue disappeared as expected (proportion of blue wins=0.539, Χ2-test: Χ12=0.858, p=0.4, n=141 contests).
If matches with a seeded athlete are included, we expected under the null hypothesis of no effect of colour that the difference in athlete ability as well as the association between winning probability and judogi colour would decline over the tournament rounds. This was indeed the case (logistic regression with covariate ‘round’ Wald=3.293, d.f.=1, p=0.0348, one-tailed).
(b) Analysis 2: comparing the number of white and blue winners in 72 tournaments
In the gold-medal finals, there was no winning bias for either colour, as expected (figure 2; proportion of blue wins=0.507, Χ2-test: Χ12=0.098, p=0.8, n=501 finals).
(c) Effect of seeding system
The 72 tournaments allow us to further analyse the effect of seedings and colour on winning. In 2004, the European Judo Federation doubled the number of seedings, each starting in blue by default (R. Abächerli & N. van Dijk 2007, personal communication). More colour–quality associations predict a stronger winning bias for blue. Consistently, the proportion of tournaments with a significant winning bias for blue was much higher after 2003 (1996–2003: 4 out of 52 tournaments, 2004–2005: 10 out of 20 tournaments, Χ2-test: Χ2=9.83, p=0.0009, one-tailed).
4. Discussion
Our results show that in judo blue–white outfit pairings do not affect contest outcomes. This is consistent with our expectation and contrasts the findings of Rowe et al. (2005). They corroborate the suggestion that a differential perception effect due to colour differences is unlikely in close combat sports, such as judo (Barton & Hill 2005). The findings also contrast the suggestion of Barton & Hill (2005) that the brightness of blue intimidates the opponent.
Rowe et al. (2005) attributed their findings in judo to the higher visibility of the white opponents, conferring an advantage to blue contestants in being better able to perceive and anticipate their white opponent's moves. This hypothesis is not convincing for judo, because, if anything, the light grey or yellow background of the judo floor would make white judogis less visible than blue judogis (see www.judophotos.com).
We argue that in international judo tournaments four factors can confound a comparison between blue and white winners. First, many matches in the repechage are asymmetric with respect to prior win–lose experience and judogi colour. Second, in the repechage, athletes in blue are more likely to have experienced one match less than the athlete in white. Third, in many tournaments the seeding system results in the allocation of blue judogis to top-ranked athletes, creating an athlete quality–colour association even beyond the first round. Regulations on the draw and seeding system are subject to change and may vary by tournament. In the same tournament, they may even differ between the men's and women's divisions. For example, during the 2004 Olympics, all seeded athletes in the men's division started in blue, while in the women's division 19 out of 25 athletes started in blue, but we found no official document describing this. It is therefore critical for investigators to carefully examine the seeding system of a particular tournament before testing a hypothesis. Finally, athletes in blue have on average a longer recovery time than those in white. Although the difference in recovery time is small (7.17 min; Sikorski et al. 1987), it can nevertheless (slightly) increase the likelihood of winning for athletes wearing a blue judogi, because judo is a physiologically very demanding sport that often leaves athletes exhausted after a match (Degoutte et al. 2003).
We suspect that the factors described in the previous paragraph may also compromise analyses on colour-associated winning biases in other sports. One could therefore also ask for a more specified analysis of the findings of Hill & Barton (2005) in which they describe a winning bias for red in four Olympic combat sports. A critical re-evaluation is also necessary for other recent analyses of colour-associated winning biases in sports (tae kwon do: Ioan et al. 2007; judo: Matsumoto et al. in press). Matsumoto et al. (in press) reported a highly significant winning bias for blue in the men's division of four major international judo tournaments (2001, 2003, 2005 World Championships; 2004 Olympics), but overlooked the same factors as Rowe et al. (2005). We re-analysed the same data and excluded the repechage and matches with seeds. Despite the fact that the asymmetry in recovery time was not controlled for, the winning bias for blue disappeared (proportion of blue wins=0.525, Χ2-test: Χ12=2.698, p=0.1, n=1041 contests). In general, we call for more careful analyses to determine whether colour-associated winning biases exist in human combat sport in the first place.
Despite our efforts to control for possible confounding factors, we believe that our analysis is still somewhat limited, in that it had no experimental approach. For example, athletes in blue and white always have fixed left and right positions relative to the scoring board. Experimental work is needed to rule out any possible confounding variable.
The general intimidating nature of red may explain the red-associated winning bias in human combat sport (Hill & Barton 2005). Given this explanation, critical investigations on how colour can impact on psychological functioning and performance in combat sports are necessary. Viewing colour can impact on mood, behaviour, cognition tasks and brain activity (Kwallek et al. 1997; Vrij 1997; Elliot & Maier 2007; Ioan et al. 2007). For example, Elliot & Maier (2007) described a series of intriguing experiments showing that red evokes avoidance motivation, thereby undermining intellectual performance. Such effects of colour may arise from cultural associations, which themselves may have an evolutionary basis (Elliot & Maier 2007). Black is associated with evil and death in virtually all cultures (Adams & Osgood 1973). Frank & Gilovich (1988) found that teams of the National Football League and the National Hockey League with black uniforms were penalized more often (but see Mills & French 1996; Tiryaki 2005). Additional experiments revealed that this might be due to referees being more prone to penalize athletes wearing black and an increased aggressiveness in players wearing black (Frank & Gilovich 1988). Although more empirical investigations are needed, the existing research supports psychological effects of red and black, but not of blue, on performance in sports.
In summary, we found no effects of winning in athletes wearing blue outfits versus white outfits. These results may have important implications for sports policy makers in judo, the most widely practised competitive martial art; we show evidence that the blue–white judo outfit does ensure an equal level of play. If the findings of Hill & Barton (2005) are reliable, it may also affect the four Olympic combat sports for which they showed a winning bias for athletes wearing red outfits; our results suggest that a blue–white pairing may be a suitable and fair alternative.
Acknowledgments
We are very grateful to Brent Cooper and Hans van Essen for their stimulating discussions and providing their datasets on contest outcomes of international judo tournaments. We thank Karin Kienhuis and the officials of the European Judo Union (Niko van Dijk), the International Judo Federation (Julie Lee), the Dutch Judo Federation (Wouter Koeman) and the Swiss Judo and Jiu-Jitsu Federation (Reto Abächerli) for valuable information on the seeding system and tournament guidelines. Irene De Pater, Ton Groothuis, Roelof Hut, Neil Metcalfe, Bernd Riedstra, Kristin Schubert, Annelies Van Vianen and Nicolaus Von Engelhardt, the editor Elizabeth Adkins-Regan and two reviewers gave insightful comments on the earlier versions of the manuscript. P.D.D. was financed by a Rubicon grant 825.07.001 of The Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO).
Supplementary Material
References
- Adams F.M, Osgood C.E. A cross-cultural study of the affective meaning of color. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 1973;4:135–156. doi:10.1177/002202217300400201 [Google Scholar]
- Andersson M. Princeton University Press; Princeton, NJ: 1994. Sexual selection. [Google Scholar]
- Barton R.A, Hill R.A. Hill & Barton reply. Nature. 2005;437:E10–E11. doi:10.1038/nature04307 [Google Scholar]
- Baube C.L. Manipulations of signaling environment affect male competitive success in three-spine sticklebacks. Anim. Behav. 1997;53:819–833. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0347 [Google Scholar]
- Cuthill I.C, Hunt S, Cleary C, Clark C. Colour bands, dominance, and body mass regulation in male zebra finches. Proc. R. Soc. B. 1997;264:1093–1099. doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0151 [Google Scholar]
- Degoutte F, Jouanel P, Filaire E. Energy demands during a judo match and recovery. Br. J. Sports Med. 2003;37:245–249. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.37.3.245. doi:10.1136/bjsm.37.3.245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dijkstra P.D, Seehausen O, Groothuis T.G.G. Direct male–male competition can facilitate invasion of new color types in Lake Victoria cichlids. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2005;58:136–143. doi:10.1007/s00265-005-0919-5 [Google Scholar]
- Drummond P.D. Facial flushing during provocation in women. Psychophysiology. 1999;36:325–332. doi: 10.1017/s0048577299980344. doi:10.1017/S0048577299980344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drummond P.D, Quah S.H. The effect of expressing anger on cardiovascular reactivity and facial blood flow in Chinese and Caucasians. Psychophysiology. 2001;38:190–196. doi:10.1017/S004857720199095X [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliot A.J, Maier M.A. Color and psychological functioning. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007;16:250–254. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00514.x [Google Scholar]
- Evan M.R, Norris K. The importance of carotenoids in signaling during aggressive interactions between male firemouth cichlids (Cichlasoma meeki) Behav. Ecol. 1996;7:1–6. doi:10.1093/beheco/7.1.1 [Google Scholar]
- Frank M.G, Gilovich T. The dark side of self- and social perception: black uniforms and aggression in professional sports. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1988;54:74–85. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.1.74. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.74 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill R.A, Barton R.A. Red enhances human performance in contests. Nature. 2005;435:293. doi: 10.1038/435293a. doi:10.1038/435293a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill G.E, McGraw K.J. Bird coloration function and evolution. vol. 2. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Ioan S, Sandulache M, Avramescu S, Ilie A, Neascu A, Zagrean L, Moldovan M. Red is a distractor for men in competition. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2007;28:285–293. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.03.001 [Google Scholar]
- Kwallek N, Woodson H, Lewis C.M, Sales C. Impact of three interior color schemes on worker mood and performance relative to individual environmental sensitivity. Colour Res. Appl. 1997;22:121–132. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6378(199704)22:2<121::AID-COL7>3.0.CO;2-V [Google Scholar]
- Maan M.E, Seehausen O, Soderberg L, Johnson L, Ripmeester A.P, Mrosso H.D.J, Taylor M.I, van Dooren T.J.M, van Alphen J.J.M. Intraspecific sexual selection on a speciation trait, male colouration, in the Lake Victoria cichlid Pundamilia nyererei. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2004;271:2445–2452. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2911. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2911 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto, D., Konno, J., Hata, S. & Takeuchi, M. In press. Blue judogis may bias competition outcomes. Res. J. Budo
- Mills B.D, French L.M. Assertive behavior and jersey color in the national hockey and football leagues: the black uniform myth and reality. J. Hum. Mov. Stud. 1996;31:47–60. [Google Scholar]
- Pryke S.R, Griffith S.C. Red dominates black: agonistic signaling among head morphs in the colour polymorphic Gouldian finch. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2006;273:949–957. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3362. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3362 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pryke S.R, Lawes M.J, Andersson S. Agonistic carotenoid signalling in male red-collared widowbirds: aggression related to the colour signal of both the territory owner and model intruder. Anim. Behav. 2001;62:695–704. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1804 [Google Scholar]
- Rowe C, Harris J.M, Roberts S.C. Seeing red? Putting sportswear in context. Nature. 2005;437:E10. doi: 10.1038/nature04306. doi:10.1038/nature04306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowe C, Harris J.M, Roberts S.C. Corrigendum: sporting contests: seeing red? Putting sportwear into context. Nature. 2006;441:E3. doi: 10.1038/nature04306. doi:10.1038/nature04848 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowland W.J. The effects of body size, aggression and nuptial coloration on competition for territories in male threespine sticklebacks, Gasterosteus aculeatus. Anim. Behav. 1989;37:282–289. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(89)90117-6 [Google Scholar]
- Rutte C, Taborsky M, Brinkhof M.W.G. What sets the odds of winning and losing? Trends Ecol. Evol. 2006;21:16–21. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.10.014. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2005.10.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Setchell J.M, Wickings E.J. Dominance, status signals and coloration in male Mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx) Ethology. 2005;111:25–50. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.01054.x [Google Scholar]
- Sikorski W, Mickiewicz B, Maole C, Laska C. Polish Judo Association, Institute of Sports; Warsaw, Poland: 1987. Structure of the contest and work capacity of the judoist. [Google Scholar]
- Tiryaki M.E. Assessing whether black uniforms affect the decisions of Turkish soccer referees: is finding of Frank and Gilovich's study valid for Turkish culture? Percept. Mot. Skills. 2005;100:51–57. doi: 10.2466/pms.100.1.51-57. doi:10.2466/PMS.100.1.51-57 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vrij A. Wearing black clothes: the impact of offenders' and suspects' clothing on impression formation. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 1997;11:47–53. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199702)11:1<47::AID-ACP421>3.0.CO;2-H [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.