Abstract
Vocal communication is critical for successful social interactions among conspecifics, but little is known about how the brain regulates context-appropriate communication. The neurotransmitter dopamine (DA) is involved in modulating highly motivated, goal-directed behaviors (including sexually motivated singing behavior), and emerging data suggest that the role of DA in vocal communication DA may differ depending on the context in which it occurs. To address this possibility, relationships between immunolabeled tyrosine hydroxylase (TH, the rate-limiting enzyme in catecholamine synthesis) and song produced within versus outside of a breeding context were explored in male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Immunocytochemistry for dopamine beta-hydroxylase (DBH; the enzyme that converts DA to norepinephrine) was also performed to provide insight into whether relationships between song and TH immunoreactivity reflected dopaminergic or noradrenergic neurotransmission. Measures of TH and DBH were quantified in song control regions (HVC, Area X, robust nucleus of the acropallium) and regions implicated in motivation (medial preoptic nucleus (POM), ventral tegmental area (VTA), and midbrain central gray). In Area X, POM, and VTA measures of TH correlated with song produced within, but not outside of a breeding context. DBH in these regions did not correlate with song in either context. Together, these data suggest DA in both song control and motivation brain regions may be more tightly linked to the regulation of highly goal-directed, sexually motivated vocal behavior.
Keywords: tyrosine hydroxylase; dopamine beta-hydroxylase; context; Area X; medial preoptic nucleus; ventral tegmental area; songbird, European starling, Sturnus vulgaris
INTRODUCTION
Across vertebrate species, vocal communication is critically involved in coordinating social interactions among conspecifics. Animals communicate in a variety of contexts and display high levels of vocal behavior in response to many different social, physiological, and environmental cues. Successful social interactions often require an animal to adjust vocal behavior so that it occurs in an appropriate context in response to appropriate stimuli. To date, the neuroendocrine mechanisms that regulate context-appropriate vocal communication are not clear.
Male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) provide a powerful model in which to examine the neural control of context-appropriate vocal communication. Male starlings sing year-round but the function of song and the social and physiological cues that stimulate song production differ depending upon the context in which it occurs [1]. Within a breeding context (i.e., during the spring breeding season) circulating levels of testosterone (T) are elevated [2, 3] and singing behavior is critical for mate attraction. Males increase their rate of song production in response to females [4], and high rates of song may be reinforced through copulation [5–9]. In contrast, within a non-breeding context (i.e. fall and winter) circulating levels of T are basal [2, 3]. Males do not respond to females with an increase in song production [4] and song appears unrelated to mate attraction [1]. At this time, starlings form flocks [10] and song is thought to maintain flock cohesion and dominance hierarchies within the flock [11, 12]. Overall, males within and outside of a breeding context sing at similar rates [4, 13], but on average breeding context song is slightly longer than non-breeding context song [4]. Taken together, breeding context song can be described as highly sexually-motivated and goal-directed, whereas non-breeding context song does not appear to be directed towards a particular individual or immediate goal but to be motivated by affiliation with a social group (i.e. what motivates singing behavior differs ‘context-dependently’).
Little is known about the neurotransmitter systems involved in regulating context-appropriate vocal communication. The neurotransmitter dopamine (DA) has a long history of being associated with reinforcement and reward [14]. DA has also been implicated in highly motivated, goal-directed or anticipatory responses to stimuli associated with reward, including sexual behavior, feeding, and the use of drugs of abuse [15–22]. We have found that within a breeding context, peripheral injections of DA agonists stimulate whereas DA antagonists inhibit song produced by male starlings in response to the introduction of a female [23]. In male zebra finches, DA antagonists also inhibit courtship singing [24] and in vivo microdialysis reveals that levels of DA in the avian basal ganglia (Area X) are elevated during song that is directed towards a conspecific [25]. A role for DA in the regulation of goal-directed vocal behavior extends beyond songbird model systems. For example, DA has also been found to stimulate ultrasonic vocalizations associated with the anticipation of social reward in rats [26, 27].
Overall, it appears that DA may play an important role in modulating vocal behavior associated with social or sexual reinforcement in birds and mammals, but the role of DA in the regulation of vocal communication in other contexts remains unclear. The effects of peripheral DA manipulations on non-breeding context song in male starlings have not been explored; however, dopaminergic drugs have been shown to influence multiple types of vocalizations in domestic chicks (i.e. trills, twitters, and warbles), none of which appear to be directed towards an individual [28]. Furthermore, microdialysis demonstrates that, relative to directed song, DA levels in Area X of zebra finches are significantly lower during undirected song (i.e. the bird is either oriented away from a conspecific or alone) [25]. Together these data suggest that dopaminergic neurotransmission may differentially modulate vocal behavior depending upon the context in which it occurs.
In songbirds, Area X, HVC (used as a proper name), and the robust nucleus of the arcopallium (RA) are components of the song control system, a series of interconnected nuclei that regulate song production, perception, and learning [29–32]. These song control nuclei receive dopaminergic input. DA [33–35] and the DA synthetic enzyme tyrosine hydroxylase [36, 37] are found throughout the song control system and the boundaries of Area X can be defined based on a high density of DA D1 receptors in a manner that is consistent with Nissl-defined boundaries [38]. As indicated above, DA within Area X appears to be involved differentially in modulating directed versus undirected singing behavior in male zebra finches [25]. The role of DA in other song control regions remains unknown; however, several lines of evidence demonstrate that Area X, HVC, and RA regulate song context-dependently [13, 39–42].
Tract-tracing studies have identified the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and midbrain central gray (GCt) as the primary sources of dopaminergic inputs to Area X, HVC, and RA, (Figure 1) [43–45]. VTA and GCt are also reciprocally connected to the medial preoptic nucleus (POM) [46] (Figure 1), a region implicated in sexually motivated behaviors, including song (detailed below). As in mammals, in birds DA-related proteins and receptors are dense within POM, VTA, and GCt [34, 35, 47–49]. These dopaminergic systems are considered central to the neural regulation of motivation (including sexual motivation) and reward [16–18, 20, 50, 51]. Taken together, dopaminergic neural circuits including the POM, VTA, and GCt are ideally positioned to influence the song control system and regulate socially appropriate vocal behavior in songbirds.
Consistent with this idea are data that demonstrate POM and VTA regulate singing behavior context-dependently. In starlings, lesions to the POM suppress sexually motivated song, but stimulate song in other contexts [52]. Additionally in starlings, the numbers of immediate early gene (IEG)-labeled cells in POM and VTA are positively correlated with breeding, but not non-breeding, context song [13]. Similarly, in house sparrows the numbers of IEG-labeled cells in POM correlate with vocalizations produced on, but not away from, a nest site [40]. Electrophysiological data in zebra finches also support a role for VTA in the regulation of female-directed song [53] and research in zebra finches and quail has shown elevated activity of DA neurons in VTA, GCt, and the preoptic area of males displaying sexual behavior, including courtship song [47, 54]. Although these regions are DA-rich, the possibility that DA in POM, VTA and GCt might play a context-dependent role in song has yet to be explored.
Several lines of evidence suggest that DA systems are sensitive to changing levels of circulating steroid hormones. Gonadal steroid hormone receptors and receptor mRNA are present at high concentrations in POM, VTA, GCt and the song control system [55–57]. Furthermore, steroid hormone treatment alters various aspects of dopaminergic neurotransmission in the song system and hypothalamus [35, 58]. The link between steroid hormones and DA further suggests that DA may differentially regulate song within (when T is high) versus outside (when T is low) a breeding context.
To test the hypothesis that DA within motivation and song control brain region regulates singing behavior in male starlings context-dependently, we examined relationships between tyrosine hydroxylase (TH; the rate-limiting enzyme in catecholamine synthesis) immunolabeling and singing behavior in breeding context and non-breeding context male starlings. We also used immunocytochemistry for dopamine β-hydroxylase (DBH; the enzyme that converts DA to norepinephrine (NE)) to provide insight into whether TH immunoreactivity reflects dopaminergic or noradrenergic neurotransmission.
METHODS
Capture and housing
Twenty-seven adult male and eight adult female European starlings were captured from December 2005 through February 2006 on a single farm northwest of Madison, Wisconsin using fly-in traps. After capture, birds were housed indoors in the University of Wisconsin-Madison Department of Zoology animal facilities in single sex cages (91cm × 47cm × 47cm) on photoperiods matching the outdoor light cycle. After three months, birds were shifted from a natural photoperiod to 18L:6D for six weeks and then to 6L:18D for six weeks. This photoperiod manipulation is routinely used in our lab to induce photosensitivity; a condition in which a bird will respond to an increase in photoperiod (≥11L:13D) and/or hormone treatment with sexual behavior. Photosensitive birds were randomly assigned to either breeding context or non-breeding context social groups and photoperiod/hormone manipulated (see below) to induce context-appropriate endocrine states and associated behaviors. Breeding context males (n=17) were given two subcutaneous implants of testosterone (T) and breeding context stimulus females (n=4) were given two subcutaneous implants of estrogen (E2) (see below) and moved to a photoperiod of 11L:13D. Non-breeding context males (n=10) and non-breeding context stimulus females (n=4) were given blank implants and remained on a photoperiod of 6L:18D. Protocols used for bird acquisition, surgery, and behavioral testing were in adherence to guidelines approved by the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (DHEW Publication 80-23, Revised 1985, Office of Science and Health Reports, DRR/ NIH, Bethesda, MD 20205) as well as the University of Wisconsin-Madison Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Hormone implants
Thirteen days prior to the first day of behavioral testing, all birds were lightly anesthetized using isoflurane gas anesthesia and a small incision was made in the skin over the breast muscle. Breeding context males received two, 14mm lengths of silastic tubing (i.d., 1.47mm; o.d. 1.96mm; Dow Corning, Midland, MI) packed for 10mm with crystalline testosterone proprionate (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) and breeding context stimulus females received two, 17mm lengths of silastic tubing packed for 13mm with 17β-estradiol (Sigma-Aldrich). Implants were sealed with silastic glue. Non-breeding context males and non-breeding context stimulus females received empty implants. Implants were placed under the skin, the incision was sutured and birds were allowed to recover on a heated pad.
Behavioral observations
Three days after implant surgery one social group of non-breeding context males (n=10) and two social groups of breeding context males (n=10 and n=7) were introduced into similar indoor observation aviaries. Birds were behaviorally tested in social groups because we were most interested in examining song behavior as it occurs within a dynamic social system (i.e. a flock). A greater number of breeding context males were used because past data from our lab has shown that within a breeding context, nestbox-ownership influences song production (i.e. males with nestboxes sing significantly more than males without nestboxes [4, 13]) and we wanted to ensure that a sufficient number of nestbox-owners were included in the study. Social groups were visually and acoustically isolated from one another. Aviaries contained five nestboxes and branches for perching. Food and water were provided ad libitum. Birds were housed in the observation aviaries throughout the duration of behavioral observations. The breeding context social groups were housed on 11L:13D and the non-breeding context social group was housed on 6L:18D. Behavioral observations began after a ten day habituation period.
On four consecutive days, social groups were observed for forty-five minutes after the introduction of a stimulus female. Breeding context stimulus females were presented to breeding context males to elicit sexually motivated singing behavior. Non-breeding context males do not alter singing behavior in response to the introduction of stimulus females [e.g. 4]. However, to control for the effect of a novel opposite-sex conspecific being introduced to the observation aviary, non-breeding context males were presented with non-breeding context stimulus females. We selected a non-breeding context female in order to maintain ecological validity (i.e. in the wild, a non-breeding context male would never encounter a breeding context female).
On each test day, a novel female was presented to the social groups in a randomized order. To characterize male singing behavior, a point sampling technique [5] was used to estimate the proportion of time every member of the social group spent singing. During the forty-five minute observation period a weak beep was emitted from a timer at one minute intervals and at every beep it was noted whether each member of the social group was or was not singing. The number of time males entered and exited nestboxes was noted throughout the forty-five minute period.
Tissue Processing
Immediately after the last forty-five minute observation period, all males in a social group were sacrificed via rapid decapitation. Brains were removed, fixed in 5% acrolein solution overnight, placed in cryoprotectant, and frozen at −80°C until sectioning. Using a cryostat, brains were sectioned in three, 40µm series and stored in anti-freeze until processing. Series one was used for TH immunocytochemistry and series two was used for DBH immunocytochemistry. Series three was used for an immunocytochemistry not discussed here.
Immunocytochemistry
Immunocytochemistry was run separately for TH and DBH, however all males (n=27) were run in a single batch for each marker. Sections were rinsed in phosphate buffered saline (PBS) for thirty minutes, incubated in 0.5% sodium borohydride solution for fifteen minutes, rinsed in PBS for twenty minutes, incubated in 0.5% hydrogen peroxide solution for ten minutes, rinsed in PBS for twenty minutes, incubated in 5% normal goat serum (NGS (made in PBS with 0.2% triton (PST-T))) solution for one hour, and then incubated in 2% NGS (made in PBS-T) primary solution overnight at room temperature (for TH, mouse anti-TH at 1:10,000 and for DBH, rabbit anti-DBH ( ImmunoStar, Inc, Hudson, WI.; catalog #s 22941 and 22806 respectively) at 1:5,000). Sections were then rinsed in PBS-T for thirty minutes and incubated in 2% NGS (made in PBS-T) biotinylated secondary solution for ninety minutes at room temperature (for TH, goat anti-mouse at 1:500 and for DBH, goat anti-rabbit at 1:250 (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA)). Then, sections were rinsed in PBS-T for thirty minutes, incubated in AB solution (Vector ABC kit) for one hour, rinsed in PBS-T for thirty minutes, and the avidin-biotin complex was visualized using Vector SG (Vector). Sections were float mounted on gel-coated slides, dehydrated, and cover slipped.
Quantification
TH and DBH immunoreactivity were quantified within POM, VTA, GCt, Area X, HVC, and RA using MetaVue software (Fryer Company, Inc., Huntley, IL). A Spot camera connected a Nikon Eclipse E600 microscope to a PC computer and images were acquired at 20X magnification. In MetaVue, a unique threshold for each label in each region was set. Within a region, the same threshold was used for every individual. On each acquired image, the optical density of immunolabeling and the pixel area covered by labeled fibers were quantified using MetaVue Integrated Morphometry Analysis. We used both optical density and pixel area because each measure provides different information about the nature of labeling in a given area. Optical density (the sum of gray value means) provides information about how concentrated label is, whereas pixel area provides information about the relative area covered by labeled fibers. TH and DBH optical density and pixel area were quantified in all areas on three serial sections bilaterally. When present, the numbers of cell bodies immunopositive for TH and DBH were hand-counted. In cases of tissue damage or lost sections, measures were made on a fourth section. If tissue damage was extensive, that individual was dropped from quantification for that brain area. In Area X, HVC, and RA TH and DBH was measured within an area of 0.58mm × 0.44mm; in POM and VTA, 0.37mm × 0.43mm; in GCt, 0.51mm × 0.33mm (see Figure 2 for region placement). The quantification of TH and DBH occurred well within the boundary of each nucleus based on past studies in which the placement and size of these regions were confirmed using alternate Nissl stained sections [13]. For every individual, mean optical density, mean pixel area, and mean number of cell bodies were calculated separately for TH and DBH and used for statistical analysis.
Statistics
Data were analyzed using Statistica 6.0 software (Stat Soft Inc., Tulsa, OK). The mean number of point samples with singing behavior across the four days of behavioral testing was calculated for each male. If a male was observed entering and exiting a nestbox multiple times on more than one day, he was designated a nestbox-owner. Because our goal was to understand dynamic social interactions, males were observed singing within social groups. Thus the behavior of each individual likely influenced other members of its social group. Due to this lack of independence of data points, non-parametric statistics were used. Spearman rank order correlation analyses were used to determine the extent to which TH and DBH optical density, pixel area, and numbers of immunopositive cell bodies in POM, VTA, GCt, Area X, HVC, and RA correlated with singing behaviors in each context. Mann-Whitney U tests were used to compare singing behavior and indices of TH and DBH between breeding context and non-breeding context males as well as between breeding context males with nestboxes and breeding context males without nestboxes.
RESULTS
Individuals within the breeding context social groups displayed behaviors that are only observed in wild birds during the breeding season. They occupied/defended nestboxes, collected nest material, wing-waved (another courtship behavior) and responded to the introduction of an opposite sex conspecific with an increase in singing behavior. No non-breeding context males were observed displaying these behaviors. Taken together this indicates the hormone/photoperiod manipulations were effective in placing birds in context-appropriate behavioral states.
The song behavior and measures of TH and DBH in the two breeding context social groups were compared statistically using Mann-Whitney U tests and did not significantly differ. Furthermore, results of correlations between song and immunolabeling for the two breeding context social group were similar for each brain region. Therefore the two breeding context social groups were combined for analysis.
In Area X, TH optical density was significantly negatively correlated with breeding context, but not non-breeding context, singing behavior (Figure 3A, Figure 4A: non-breeding context: rs=0.50, p=0.21; breeding context: rs=−0.671, p=0.01). In POM, TH pixel area was significantly negatively correlated with only breeding context song (Figure 3B, 4B: non-breeding context: rs=−0.24, p=0.51; breeding context: rs=−0.62, p=0.02). In VTA, TH optical density was significantly positively correlated with only breeding context song (Figure 3C, 4C: non-breeding context: rs=0.38, p=0.28; breeding context: rs=0.60, p=0.02). In VTA, there were two non-breeding context males and two breeding context males whose TH optical density values were noticeably higher than all other individuals (Figure 3C). Removal of these individuals from the analyses did not change the significance of the correlations (non-breeding context: rs=0.67, p=0.07; breeding context: rs=0.59, p=0.03). In RA, there was a trend for TH pixel area to relate positively to breeding context song but the relationship failed to reach significance (non-breeding context: rs=0.04, p=0.91; breeding context: rs=0.49, p=0.07).
No other measures of TH (optical density, pixel area, or numbers of immunopositive cell bodies) were significantly correlated with song in either context (p > 0.16 in all cases). Measures of DBH in Area X, POM and VTA did not significantly correlate with singing behavior in either context (p > 0.39 in all cases). However it was not the case that song did not relate to DBH in any brain region. For example, while beyond the scope of the present paper, DBH pixel area in the ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus significantly correlated with breeding context song and DBH optical density in medial bed nucleus of the stria terminalis significantly correlated with non-breeding context song (Heimovics and Riters, unpublished data).
Mann-Whitney U tests revealed no significant differences between breeding context and non-breeding context males in either the mean proportion of time males in either group spent singing or any index of either TH or DBH (optical density, pixel area, or numbers of immunopositive cell bodies) in any of the brain regions examined. Mann-Whitney U tests also revealed no significant differences between breeding context males with nestboxes and breeding context males without nestboxes in song behavior or measures of TH or DBH.
DISCUSSION
The immunocytochemical data presented here support the hypothesis that DA plays a context-dependent role in the regulation of vocal communication in songbirds. Significant relationships between singing behavior and measures of TH immunoreactivity (THir) in POM, VTA, and Area X were observed for song produced within, but not outside of, a breeding context. No relationship between DBH in these regions and breeding context song was observed suggesting that indices of THir predominantly reflect relationships between song and dopaminergic, rather than noradrenergic, neurotransmission. Although the present data do not preclude a role for DA in non-breeding context song, they do suggest that DA synthesis in POM, VTA, and Area X may be more tightly linked with sexually motivated vocal communication in male starlings.
TH and Breeding context song
VTA
Across vertebrate taxa, DA producing neurons in VTA that project to the forebrain have been shown to mediate reward and/or motivation associated with numerous goal-directed behaviors, including sexual behavior, feeding and the use of drugs of abuse [16, 19, 50, 60, 61]. VTA also projects to the song control system [43–45] and POM [46] in songbirds. In the present study, the relationship between breeding context singing behavior and measures of THir in VTA was positive. Given that VTA is one of the primary sources of dopaminergic input to much of the brain, our findings suggest that VTA synthesizes and sends high levels of DA to terminal fields in males singing high levels of breeding context song. Our data also suggest that VTA sends less DA to terminal fields in males singing low levels of breeding context song. This relationship was not observed within a non-breeding context, suggesting that DA in VTA may regulate singing behavior context-dependently. This pattern is consistent with past data showing immediate early genes in VTA correlate with breeding, but not non-breeding context song in starlings [13].
Area X and POM
Also consistent with past data, the present results highlight context-dependent participation of both POM [13, 52] and Area X [25, 41, 42, 62] in the regulation of singing behavior. However, in contrast to VTA our data revealed a negative relationship between breeding context song and indices of THir in Area X and POM. As mentioned above, VTA sends dopaminergic projections to the song control system, including Area X [43–45]. VTA is also a source of DA inputs to the preoptic area [63]. Microdialysis studies have shown that DA release is elevated in Area X of male zebra finches singing song that is directed towards a conspecific [25]. Similarly, DA levels are elevated in the medial preoptic area of male rodents after exposure to cues associated with mating (i.e. visual and olfactory stimuli) [17, 64]. Past data from our lab show that peripheral DA agonists stimulate breeding context singing behavior in male starlings [23] and preliminary findings from our lab demonstrate that site-specific injections of DA agonists into POM also stimulate starling breeding context song (Pawlisch and Riters, unpublished data). Based on this literature, we believe the negative relationship between measures of THir in Area X and POM and breeding context song probably reflects negative feedback on TH [see 65 for review]. Specifically, within a breeding context DA release may be high in Area X and POM of males singing high levels of sexually-motivated song. This high dopaminergic tone could lead to end-product inhibition lowering TH in these individuals. In contrast, dopaminergic tone may be low in Area X and POM of males singing low levels of breeding context song which could reduce end-product inhibition leading to higher TH. The negative correlation between breeding context song and TH in POM and Area X could also be due to the fact that DA is being synthesized at high rates in the highest singers thus depleting stores of TH.
An alternative interpretation of these findings is that high indices of THir in Area X and POM reflect elevated DA synthesis and release. This would mean that males producing high levels of breeding context song have lower levels of DA in Area X and POM, suggesting that high DA in Area X and POM inhibits sexually motivated singing behavior. Although the present data do not rule out this possibility, given that studies performed to date suggest that sexually motivated behaviors, including courtship song, are associated with elevated DA in Area X and POM (as reviewed above) we believe this interpretation to be unlikely.
It is important to note that changes in density measures of TH do not necessarily indicate changes in DA exocytosis and it is changes in extracellular levels of DA that are most important to the behavioral response (i.e. song) being examined. Thus, we are limited in our ability to interpret the functional relationships between high versus low TH in a given brain area and actual DA release in that region or its terminal field. Future studies that combine immunocytochemistry for TH and microdialysis for extracellular DA would help to further interpret the patterns observed in the present study. However, in spite of these limitations, we find it noteworthy that TH in Area X, POM and VTA significantly correlates with breeding but not non-breeding context singing behavior. Thus, the present findings highlight a potential context-dependent role for DA within Area X, POM, and VTA in the regulation of singing behavior in male European starlings.
Differences in indices of immunolabeling
In the present analysis we quantified both the optical density of TH immunolabeling and the pixel area covered by labeled fibers. Interestingly, in VTA and Area X, TH optical density (but not pixel area) correlated with breeding context song. In contrast, in POM, TH pixel area (but not optical density) correlated with breeding context song. It is possible that an increase in the area of POM that is innervated by DA (as reflected in pixel area) relates to recruitment of new postsynaptic targets; whereas changes in levels of TH activation within a localized area (as reflected in optical density) of Area X and VTA relates to changes in DA levels within an area already targeted. Taken together, the present study highlights the importance of quantifying multiple indices of immunolabeling to provide a more complete picture of the nature and distribution of labeled cells and fibers.
TH and endocrine state
Unlike previous work in songbirds showing that DA systems are modulated by exogenous steroid hormones [34, 35, 58], here we found no significant difference between breeding (T-implanted) and non-breeding (blank-implanted) context males in indices of THir in any of the regions we examined. The effect of androgens on TH in birds and mammals has been shown to vary in a complex time and region specific manner [58, 66–72]. Our results suggest that after seventeen days of T treatment, THir does not differ within the regions examined in this study. Additional studies are needed to elucidate the role androgens on DA and context-dependent song.
Conclusions
Past studies show that peripheral DA agonists stimulate and antagonists inhibit breeding context singing behavior production in starlings [23]. Additional data from pharmacological manipulations in zebra finches support a role for DA in stimulating directed vocal communication [24]. Because these drugs were administered systemically, it is impossible to discern where in the brain altered dopaminergic function influences sexually motivated vocal communication. Past data have highlighted Area X as a site where DA regulates song behavior context-dependently [25]. The present data further support a context-dependent role for Area X and also identify POM and VTA as potential sites where dopaminergic drugs may act to modulate sexually motivated vocal behavior in songbirds.
The present data taken together with past work expand upon a growing body of data implicating brain regions outside of the song control system in the regulation of singing behavior in songbirds [4, 13, 40, 73–78]. Specifically, the present results support the idea that interactions between POM and VTA may be critically involved in adjusting vocal behavior so that it occurs in the appropriate context in response to appropriate stimuli. Furthermore, the data presented here clearly implicate DA as an important modulator of context-appropriate vocal communication in songbirds.
Broader implications
The incentive salience hypothesis of DA function posits that DA may be more critically involved in regulating goal-directed, motivated responses to rewarding stimuli or the “wanting” of a particular stimulus (as opposed to underlying the reward itself) [15]. Other neurochemicals, such as opioid neuropeptides, have been implicated in reward or reinforcement components of behavior, or the “liking” of a particular stimulus [15, 79]. Consistent with this framework, in the present study we found THir to be linked exclusively to song produced within a context in which it is highly sexually motivated, goal-directed and associated with the wanting of a female (i.e. a breeding context) whereas in past work we have found that immunolabeling for the opioid neuropeptide met-enkephalin in POM to be more tightly linked to song produced within a context in which it does not appear to be goal-directed and has no obvious form of immediate external reinforcement (i.e. a non-breeding context [80]). Taken together, these data suggest the intriguing possibility that the context-dependent roles of dopamine and opioids in the regulation of song may relate to their differential involvement in “wanting” versus “liking” associated with song behavior. Importantly, the present study examined one exemplar of goal-directed vocal communication (i.e. sexually motivated, female-directed song) and future studies must examine other forms of goal-directed vocal behavior to verify this hypothesis.
Acknowledgements
The data presented in this paper are based upon work supported by a grant from NIMH (R01MH080225) to LVR and a graduate research fellowship from NSF to SAH. We gratefully acknowledge Kate Skogen, Jeff Alexander, Chris Elliot, and John Irwin for help with starling capture and animal care; Ben Pawlisch, Sarah Alger, Eric Lyngaas, Stephen Bowman, Charles Dulberger, Lara Peschke, Laura Klein, Laura Knepel, and Lea Gardiner for their help with tissue processing; and Bill Feeny for assistance with illustrations.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
REFERENCES
- 1.Eens M. Understanding the Complex Song of the European Starling: An Integrated Ethological Approach. Advances in the Study of Behavior. 1997;26:255–435. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ball GF, Wingfield JC. Changes in Plasma-Levels of Luteinizing-Hormone and Sex Steroid-Hormones in Relation to Multiple-Broodedness and Nest-Site Density in Male Starlings. Physiological Zoology. 1987;60:191–199. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Riters LV, Eens M, Pinxten R, Ball GF. Seasonal changes in the densities of alpha(2)-noradrenergic receptors are inversely related to changes in testosterone and the volumes of song control nuclei in male European starlings. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2002;444:63–74. doi: 10.1002/cne.10131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Riters LV, Eens M, Pinxten R, Duffy DL, Balthazart J, Ball GF. Seasonal changes in courtship song and the medial preoptic area in male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) Horm Behav. 2000;38:250–261. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2000.1623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Eens M, Pinxten R. Extra-Pair Courtship in the Starling Sturnus-Vulgaris. Ibis. 1990;132:618–619. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Eens M, Pinxten R. Intersexual Conflicts over Copulations in the European Starling - Evidence for the Female Mate Guarding Hypothesis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1995;36:71–81. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Eens M, Pinxten R, Verheyen RF. On the Function of Singing and Wing-Waving in the European Starling Sturnus-Vulgaris. Bird Study. 1990;37:48–52. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Eens M, Pinxten R, Verheyen RF. Function of the Song and Song Repertoire in the European Starling (Sturnus-Vulgaris) - an Aviary Experiment. Behaviour. 1993;125:51–66. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Pinxten R, Eens M. Copulation and mate-guarding patterns in polygynous European starlings. Animal Behaviour. 1997;54:45–58. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Feare C. The starling, Oxford [Oxfordshire] New York: Oxford University Press; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hausberger M, Richardyris MA, Henry L, Lepage L, Schmidt I. Song Sharing Reflects the Social-Organization in a Captive Group of European Starlings (Sturnus-Vulgaris) Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1995;109:222–241. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Summers RW, Westlake GE, Feare CJ. Differences in the Ages, Sexes and Physical Condition of Starlings Sturnus-Vulgaris at the Center and Periphery of Roosts. Ibis. 1987;129:96–102. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Heimovics SA, Riters LV. Immediate early gene activity in song control nuclei and brain areas regulating motivation relates positively to singing behavior during, but not outside of, a breeding context. J Neurobiol. 2005;65:207–224. doi: 10.1002/neu.20181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Wise RA. Forebrain substrates of reward and motivation. J Comp Neurol. 2005;493:115–121. doi: 10.1002/cne.20689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Berridge KC, Robinson TE. What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1998;28:309–369. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00019-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Blackburn JR, Pfaus JG, Phillips AG. Dopamine functions in appetitive and defensive behaviours. Prog Neurobiol. 1992;39:247–279. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(92)90018-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hull EM, Du J, Lorrain DS, Matuszewich L. Extracellular dopamine in the medial preoptic area: implications for sexual motivation and hormonal control of copulation. J Neurosci. 1995;15:7465–7471. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-11-07465.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Koob GF. Hedonic valence, dopamine and motivation. Mol Psychiatry. 1996;1:186–189. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Moses J, Loucks JA, Watson HL, Matuszewich L, Hull EM. Dopaminergic drugs in the medial preoptic area and nucleus accumbens: effects on motor activity, sexual motivation, and sexual performance. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1995;51:681–686. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)00437-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Pfaus JG, Phillips AG. Role of dopamine in anticipatory and consummatory aspects of sexual behavior in the male rat. Behav Neurosci. 1991;105:727–743. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.105.5.727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Salamone JD, Correa M, Mingote S, Weber SM. Nucleus accumbens dopamine and the regulation of effort in food-seeking behavior: implications for studies of natural motivation, psychiatry, and drug abuse. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2003;305:1–8. doi: 10.1124/jpet.102.035063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.van Furth WR, Wolterink G, van Ree JM. Regulation of masculine sexual behavior: involvement of brain opioids and dopamine. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1995;21:162–184. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(96)82985-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Schroeder MB, Riters LV. Pharmacological manipulations of dopamine and opioids have differential effects on sexually motivated song in male European starlings. Physiol Behav. 2006;88:575–584. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rauceo S, Harding CF, Maldonado A, Gaysinkaya L, Tulloch I, Rodriguez E. Dopaminergic modulation of reproductive behavior and activity in male zebra finches. Behav Brain Res. 2007 doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Sasaki A, Sotnikova TD, Gainetdinov RR, Jarvis ED. Social context-dependent singing-regulated dopamine. J Neurosci. 2006;26:9010–9014. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1335-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Burgdorf J, Knutson B, Panksepp J, Ikemoto S. Nucleus accumbens amphetamine microinjections unconditionally elicit 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115:940–944. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.115.4.940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wintink AJ, Brudzynski SM. The related roles of dopamine and glutamate in the initiation of 50-kHz ultrasonic calls in adult rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2001;70:317–323. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00615-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.de Lanerolle NC, Youngren OM. Chick vocalization and emotional behavior influenced by apomorphine. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1978;92:416–430. doi: 10.1037/h0077486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Brenowitz EA, Margoliash D, Nordeen KW. An introduction to birdsong and the avian song system. J Neurobiol. 1997;33:495–500. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Margoliash D. Functional organization of forebrain pathways for song production and perception. J Neurobiol. 1997;33:671–693. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4695(19971105)33:5<671::aid-neu12>3.0.co;2-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Mello CV. Mapping vocal communication pathways in birds with inducible gene expression. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol. 2002;188:943–959. doi: 10.1007/s00359-002-0347-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wild JM. Neural pathways for the control of birdsong production. J Neurobiol. 1997;33:653–670. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4695(19971105)33:5<653::aid-neu11>3.0.co;2-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Sakaguchi H, Saito N. The acetylcholine and catecholamine contents in song control nuclei of zebra finch during song ontogeny. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1989;47:313–317. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(89)90189-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Barclay SR, Harding CF. Androstenedione modulation of monoamine levels and turnover in hypothalamic and vocal control nuclei in the male zebra finch: steroid effects on brain monoamines. Brain Res. 1988;459:333–343. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90649-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Barclay SR, Harding CF. Differential modulation of monoamine levels and turnover rates by estrogen and/or androgen in hypothalamic and vocal control nuclei of male zebra finches. Brain Res. 1990;523:251–262. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91494-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bottjer SW. The distribution of tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactivity in the brains of male and female zebra finches. J Neurobiol. 1993;24:51–69. doi: 10.1002/neu.480240105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Soha JA, Shimizu T, Doupe AJ. Development of the catecholaminergic innervation of the song system of the male zebra finch. J Neurobiol. 1996;29:473–489. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4695(199604)29:4<473::AID-NEU5>3.0.CO;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Casto JM, Ball GF. Characterization and localization of D1 dopamine receptors in the sexually dimorphic vocal control nucleus, area X, and the basal ganglia of European starlings. J Neurobiol. 1994;25:767–780. doi: 10.1002/neu.480250703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Liu WC, Nottebohm F. Variable rate of singing and variable song duration are associated with high immediate early gene expression in two anterior forebrain song nuclei. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:10724–10729. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504677102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Riters LV, Teague DP, Schroeder MB. Social Status Interacts with Badge Size and Neuroendocrine Physiology to Influence Sexual Behavior in Male House Sparrows (PASSER DOMESTICUS) Brain Behav Evol. 2004;63:141–150. doi: 10.1159/000076240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Jarvis ED, Scharff C, Grossman MR, Ramos JA, Nottebohm F. For whom the bird sings: context-dependent gene expression. Neuron. 1998;21:775–788. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80594-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hessler NA, Doupe AJ. Social context modulates singing-related neural activity in the songbird forebrain. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:209–211. doi: 10.1038/6306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lewis JW, Ryan SM, Arnold AP, Butcher LL. Evidence for a catecholaminergic projection to area X in the zebra finch. J Comp Neurol. 1981;196:347–354. doi: 10.1002/cne.901960212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Appeltants D, Absil P, Balthazart J, Ball GF. Identification of the origin of catecholaminergic inputs to HVc in canaries by retrograde tract tracing combined with tyrosine hydroxylase immunocytochemistry. J Chem Neuroanat. 2000;18:117–133. doi: 10.1016/s0891-0618(99)00054-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Appeltants D, Ball GF, Balthazart J. The origin of catecholaminergic inputs to the song control nucleus RA in canaries. Neuroreport. 2002;13:649–653. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200204160-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Riters LV, Alger SJ. Neuroanatomical evidence for indirect connections between the medial preoptic nucleus and the song control system: possible neural substrates for sexually motivated song. Cell Tissue Res. 2004;316:35–44. doi: 10.1007/s00441-003-0838-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Bharati IS, Goodson JL. Fos responses of dopamine neurons to sociosexual stimuli in male zebra finches. Neuroscience. 2006;143:661–670. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.08.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ball GF, Casto JM, Balthazart J. Autoradiographic localization of D1-like dopamine receptors in the forebrain of male and female Japanese quail and their relationship with immunoreactive tyrosine hydroxylase. J Chem Neuroanat. 1995;9:121–133. doi: 10.1016/0891-0618(95)00075-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Absil P, Foidart A, Hemmings HC, Jr, Steinbusch HW, Ball GF, Balthazart J. Distribution of DARPP-32 immunoreactive structures in the quail brain: anatomical relationship with dopamine and aromatase. J Chem Neuroanat. 2001;21:23–39. doi: 10.1016/s0891-0618(00)00094-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Fibiger HC, Nomikos GG, Pfaus JG, Damsma G. Sexual behavior, eating and mesolimbic dopamine. Clin Neuropharmacol. 1992;15(Pt A) Suppl 1:566A–567A. doi: 10.1097/00002826-199201001-00294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Neurobehavioural mechanisms of reward and motivation. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1996;6:228–236. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(96)80077-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Alger SJ, Riters LV. Lesions to the medial preoptic nucleus differentially affect singing and nest box-directed behaviors within and outside of the breeding season in European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) Behav Neurosci. 2006;120:1326–1336. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.6.1326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Yanagihara S, Hessler NA. Modulation of singing-related activity in the songbird ventral tegmental area by social context. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;24:3619–3627. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05228.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Charlier TD, Ball GF, Balthazart J. Sexual behavior activates the expression of the immediate early genes c-fos and Zenk (egr-1) in catecholaminergic neurons of male Japanese quail. Neuroscience. 2005;131:13–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.09.068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Ball GF, Riters LV, Balthazart J. Neuroendocrinology of song behavior and avian brain plasticity: multiple sites of action of sex steroid hormones. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2002;23:137–178. doi: 10.1006/frne.2002.0230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Maney DL, Bernard DJ, Ball GF. Gonadal steroid receptor mRNA in catecholaminergic nuclei of the canary brainstem. Neurosci Lett. 2001;311:189–192. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(01)02157-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Bernard DJ, Bentley GE, Balthazart J, Turek FW, Ball GF. Androgen receptor, estrogen receptor alpha, and estrogen receptor beta show distinct patterns of expression in forebrain song control nuclei of European starlings. Endocrinology. 1999;140:4633–4643. doi: 10.1210/endo.140.10.7024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Appeltants D, Ball GF, Balthazart J. Song activation by testosterone is associated with an increased catecholaminergic innervation of the song control system in female canaries. Neuroscience. 2003;121:801–814. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00496-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Lehner PN. Handbook of Ethological Methods. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wenkstern D, Pfaus JG, Fibiger HC. Dopamine transmission increases in the nucleus accumbens of male rats during their first exposure to sexually receptive female rats. Brain Res. 1993;618:41–46. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)90426-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Damsma G, Pfaus JG, Wenkstern D, Phillips AG, Fibiger HC. Sexual behavior increases dopamine transmission in the nucleus accumbens and striatum of male rats: comparison with novelty and locomotion. Behav Neurosci. 1992;106:181–191. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.1.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Riters LV, Teague DP, Schroeder MB, Cummings SE. Vocal production in different social contexts relates to variation in immediate early gene immunoreactivity within and outside of the song control system. Behav Brain Res. 2004;155:307–318. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Balthazart J, Absil P. Identification of catecholaminergic inputs to and outputs from aromatase-containing brain areas of the Japanese quail by tract tracing combined with tyrosine hydroxylase immunocytochemistry. J Comp Neurol. 1997;382:401–428. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Schulz KM, Richardson HN, Romeo RD, Morris JA, Lookingland KJ, Sisk CL. Medial preoptic area dopaminergic responses to female pheromones develop during puberty in the male Syrian hamster. Brain Res. 2003;988:139–145. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(03)03358-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Kilts CD, Anderson CM, Ely TD, Mailman RB. The biochemistry and pharmacology of mesoamygdaloid dopamine neurons. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1988;537:173–187. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1988.tb42105.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.King JA, Kelly TA, Delville Y. Adult levels of testosterone alter catecholamine innervation in an animal model of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychobiology. 2000;42:163–168. doi: 10.1159/000026687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Adler A, Vescovo P, Robinson JK, Kritzer MF. Gonadectomy in adult life increases tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactivity in the prefrontal cortex and decreases open field activity in male rats. Neuroscience. 1999;89:939–954. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(98)00341-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Kritzer MF. Effects of acute and chronic gonadectomy on the catecholamine innervation of the cerebral cortex in adult male rats: insensitivity of axons immunoreactive for dopamine-beta-hydroxylase to gonadal steroids, and differential sensitivity of axons immunoreactive for tyrosine hydroxylase to ovarian and testicular hormones. J Comp Neurol. 2000;427:617–633. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Kritzer MF. Long-term gonadectomy affects the density of tyrosine hydroxylase- but not dopamine-beta-hydroxylase-, choline acetyltransferase- or serotonin-immunoreactive axons in the medial prefrontal cortices of adult male rats. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:282–296. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.3.282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Kritzer MF, Adler A, Marotta J, Smirlis T. Regionally selective effects of gonadectomy on cortical catecholamine innervation in adult male rats are most disruptive to afferents in prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 1999;9:507–518. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.5.507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.King JA, Barkley RA, Delville Y, Ferris CF. Early androgen treatment decreases cognitive function and catecholamine innervation in an animal model of ADHD. Behav Brain Res. 2000;107:35–43. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(99)00113-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Simerly RB, Swanson LW, Handa RJ, Gorski RA. Influence of perinatal androgen on the sexually dimorphic distribution of tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive cells and fibers in the anteroventral periventricular nucleus of the rat. Neuroendocrinology. 1985;40:501–510. doi: 10.1159/000124122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Riters LV, Ball GF. Lesions to the medial preoptic area affect singing in the male European starling (Sturnus vulgaris) Horm Behav. 1999;36:276–286. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1999.1549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Maney DL, Ball GF. Fos-like immunoreactivity in catecholaminergic brain nuclei after territorial behavior in free-living song sparrows. J Neurobiol. 2003;56:163–170. doi: 10.1002/neu.10227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Hara E, Kubikova L, Hessler NA, Jarvis ED. Role of the midbrain dopaminergic system in modulation of vocal brain activation by social context. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:3406–3416. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05600.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Heimovics SA, Riters LV. Breeding-context-dependent relationships between song and cFOS labeling within social behavior brain regions in male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) Horm Behav. 2006;50:726–735. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Heimovics SA, Riters LV. ZENK labeling within social behavior brain regions reveals breeding context-dependent patterns of neural activity associated with song in male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) Behav Brain Res. 2007;176:333–343. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.10.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Goodson JL. Territorial aggression and dawn song are modulated by septal vasotocin and vasoactive intestinal polypeptide in male field sparrows (Spizella pusilla) Horm Behav. 1998;34:67–77. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Kelley AE, Berridge KC. The neuroscience of natural rewards: relevance to addictive drugs. J Neurosci. 2002;22:3306–3311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-09-03306.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Riters LV, Schroeder MB, Auger CJ, Eens M, Pinxten R, Ball GF. Evidence for Opioid Involvement in the Regulation of Song Production in Male European Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) Behav Neurosci. 2005;119:245–255. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]