Skip to main content
The BMJ logoLink to The BMJ
. 2008 Dec 16;337:a2826. doi: 10.1136/bmj.a2826

Right-left discrimination among medical students: questionnaire and psychometric study

Gerard J Gormley 1,, Martin Dempster 2, Rachael Best 2
PMCID: PMC2604913  PMID: 19088140

Abstract

Objective To determine medical students’ self awareness and ability to discriminate right from left; to identify characteristics associated with this ability; and to identify any techniques used to aid discrimination.

Design Questionnaire and psychometric study.

Setting Undergraduate medical school, Northern Ireland.

Participants 290 first year undergraduate students.

Main outcome measure Medical students’ ability to discriminate right from left using the Bergen right-left discrimination test.

Results Test scores ranged from 31 to 143 on a scale of 0-144 (mean 112 (standard deviation 22.2)). Male students significantly outperformed female students (117.18 (26.96) v 110.80 (28.94)). Students who wanted to be surgeons performed significantly better than those who wanted to be general practitioners or medical doctors (119.87 (25.15) v 110.55 (27.36) v 112.50 (26.88)). The interaction effect for sex and career wishes was not significant (P=0.370). Students who used learnt techniques to help them discriminate scored significantly less than those who did not (P<0.001). Students had greater difficulty in discriminating right from left when looking at the forward view rather than the back view (P<0.001).

Conclusions Male students were better than female students at distinguishing right from left, and aspiring surgeons were better than aspiring general practitioners or medical doctors. Students had more difficulty with the forward view than the back view.


The ability to discriminate right from left calls on several higher functions including memory, visuospatial processing, language, integration of sensory information, and sometimes mental rotation.1 Some people have difficulty in distinguishing right from left in themselves and in others.2 3 Correctly discriminating right from left is important in the practice of medicine. Confusing a patient’s right side from their left can result in surgery, procedures, and investigations being carried out on the wrong side.4 Such mistakes may occur more frequently than is reported,4 and prevention should start at undergraduate level.5

Medical students perform better than psychology or law students in right-left discrimination tests, but data are limited.6 We aimed to assess medical students’ perceived and actual right-left discriminatory ability; to identify characteristics associated with this ability; and to identify any techniques used to aid discrimination.

Methods

We invited all first year students at the School of Medicine, Queen’s University Belfast who attended a clinical skills programme to participate. Consenting students completed an anonymised questionnaire to ascertain their sex, age, and career wishes. We used the Oldfield handedness inventory to determine hand preference.7 Students recorded their perceived discriminatory ability on a five point Likert scale. We also asked them to record any techniques that they used to aid discrimination.

We used the Bergen right-left discrimination test to measure participants’ ability to discriminate right from left.8 In this test, participants are shown a series of line figures, which have a white head when viewed from the front and a black head when viewed from the back. Hands are circles at the end of the figures’ arms. The test has three subsections—all figures viewed from the back, from the front, and alternating views from the back and front. Participants indicated the right or the left hand of the figure by marking the appropriate “hand” circle. Each subsection consists of 48 figures, and participants had 90 seconds a subsection to complete as many items as possible; this gave a maximum potential score of 144 and a minimum score of 0. Subsections were administered in a counterbalanced sequence to account for order effects.

To meet the assumptions of the parametric tests used (that the residual scores are normally distributed), we squared the test scores and used transformed variables in all analyses. We used analysis of variance to examine differences in the test scores between men and women and between career wishes, with post hoc Tukey tests to explore all pairwise comparisons among career wishes, and to determine whether these variables interacted significantly. Handedness and order of presentation of the figures were controlled for. We used an independent t test to compare students who used a discrimination technique and those who did not. We then used analysis of variance to compare the different discriminatory techniques, Pearson’s correlation coefficient to examine the association between test scores and participants’ perceived discriminatory ability, and repeated measures analysis of variance to examine the effect of figure position (front, back, or mixed) on test scores.

Results

Participants

All 290 students participated. Mean age was 19 years (range 18-26); 42% (121/290) were male and 58% (169/290) were female (table 1). About 32% (93/290) of students were considering a career in general practice, 27% (79/290) a career as a medical doctor, 24% (70/290) a career in surgery, and 17% (48/290) other careers. Only 15% (42/290) were left handed and 85% (248/290) were right handed.

Table 1.

 Characteristics of participants

Characteristic Proportion of participants (n)
Sex:
 Male 42 (121/290)
 Female 58 (169/290)
Career aspiration:
 General practitioner 32 (93/290)
 Medical doctor 27 (79/290)
 Surgeon 24 (70/290)
 Other 17 (48/290)
Hand preference:
 Left 15 (42/290)
 Right 85 (248/290)
Perceived ability to discriminate right from left:
 Very good 26 (76/290)
 Good 43 (126/290)
 Average 20 (57/290)
 Poor 9 (25/290)
 Very poor 2 (6/290)

Techniques used to aid right-left discrimination

Many students (32%; 94/290) used techniques to help them discriminate right from left (table 2).

Table 2.

 Proportion of participants who reported the use of different categories of right-left discriminatory techniques

Discriminatory technique category Proportion of participants (n)
Relates to a physical activity 49 (46/94)
Relates to a unilateral body feature 27 (25/94)
Relates to a unilateral dress or accessory feature 7 (7/94)
Use of word association 10 (9/94)
Other 7 (7/94)

Bergen right-left discrimination test scores

Test scores ranged from 31 to 143 (mean 112 (standard deviation 22.2); figure). Table 3 shows the test scores according to subgroup. The order of presentation of the front, back, and mixed views did not affect test scores (F2,233=1.325, P=0.268), and neither did the participants’ handedness (F1,233=0.137, P=0.711).

graphic file with name gorg608067.f1.jpg

Participants’ scores on the Bergen right-left discrimination test. *Scale of 0-144

Table 3.

  Bergen right-left discrimination test summary scores according to subgroup

Mean scores Standard deviation
Presentation order:*
 Back, front, mixed 113.95 27.10
 Front, mixed, back 112.05 26.14
 Mixed, back, front 116.92 25.37
Dominant hand:*
 Left 114.69 22.16
 Right 113.92 22.29
Sex:*
 Male 117.81 26.96
 Female 110.80 28.94
Career wishes:*
 General practitioner 110.55 27.36
 Medical doctor 112.50 26.88
 Surgeon 119.87 25.15
Used discrimination technique:*
 No 117.77 19.00
 Yes 102.14 24.40
Figure orientation:†
 Back view 41.55 7.98
 Front view 37.36 9.72
 Mixed view 33.69 9.00

*Scale of 0-144.

†Scale of 0-48 for each subsection.

Male students performed significantly better than female students (F1,233=5.859, P=0.016). We also saw a significant association between test scores and career wishes (F2,233=4.157, P=0.017); post hoc Tukey tests indicated that students who wanted to be surgeons performed significantly better than those who wanted to be general practitioners (P=0.003) or medical doctors (P=0.047). The interaction effect for sex and career wishes was not significant (F(2,233)=1.000, P=0.370).

Perceived discriminatory ability was significantly associated with test scores (r=0.387, P<0.001). Students who used techniques to help them discriminate had significantly lower test scores than those who did not (t=5.342, P<0.001). We found no significant difference between the technique used and the score (F4,75=0.442, P=0.778).

The orientation of the figures significantly affected the scores (F2,482=99.059, P<0.001). Scores were significantly higher for the back view than for the front view (t=6.843, P<0.001) or the mixed view (t=14.683, P<0.001). Scores were significantly higher for the front view than for the mixed view (t=6.995, P<0.001).

Discussion

Male students were better than female students at discriminating left from right. Aspiring surgeons performed better than aspiring general practitioners or medical doctors. Students had more difficulty with the forward view than the back view.

The differences between men and women are in keeping with other studies3 9 10 and may result from brain lateralisation, whereby males perform better in largely right hemisphere tasks, such as spatial ability, and females perform better in left hemisphere dominant tasks, such as verbal ability.11 Imaging studies indicate that right-left discrimination is mostly a right hemisphere task.12 However, the test used depended on participants answering as many questions as possible in a given time. Males are often better at such tests, and this may account for some of the differences seen in our study.13

We found no differences between right handed and left handed students. Some studies have shown right handers to be better at discriminating between right and left,2 14 15 but others report no difference.3 16

Even at this early stage of training, students’ career aspirations were associated with their test score. Students aspiring to be surgeons had significantly higher scores than those who wished to enter general practice. Perhaps students who think they are better at right-left discrimination are more attracted to specialties that place greater demands on spatial ability, such as surgery. Further research is needed to determine if right-left discrimination ability, and other spatial tasks, are associated with career aspiration and attainment. It would also be important to determine if students who were less able to discriminate improved after medical training.

Around a third of students reported using techniques to help them discriminate right from left—these students scored lower on the test. Perhaps students who do not use such techniques have a greater natural ability to discriminate than students who do.

Medical practitioners usually face patients in the front position. Our results indicate that medical students have greater difficulty in right-left discrimination in this position, perhaps because mental rotation is required.17

Wrong sided patient events can have serious consequences.4 18 19 Our study cannot determine whether medical students’ right-left discriminatory ability directly affects patient safety. However, as well as learning relative anatomical directions such as superior and inferior, students should be told the importance of correctly differentiating right from left and that this can pose problems for some people. Measuring the discriminatory ability of students who report such problems may make them more vigilant and help them develop strategies to prevent errors in clinical practice. Further research would help clarify any associations between right-left discriminatory ability and patient safety, and whether this ability is influenced by fatigue or distraction.

What is already known on this topic

  • In clinical practice confusing right and left can have serious consequences for patient care and safety

  • Some people have difficulty in discriminating right from left in themselves and others

What this study adds

  • Medical students vary in their ability to discriminate right from left

  • Medical educators need to emphasise the importance of differentiating right from left and for students to develop checks when making right-left decisions

Thanks to all the students who participated in this study and the staff of the Clinical Skills Education Centre, Queen’s University Belfast. Thanks also to Sonja Helgesen Ofte (Statped West, Speech and Language Department, Bergen, Norway) for permission to use the Bergen right-left discrimination test.

Contributors: All authors helped conceive, design, and supervise this project. GJG wrote the first draft, which was revised by MD and RB, with GJG coordinating rewriting and agreement of the final article. MD provided statistical expertise. GJG is guarantor.

Funding: Association for the Study of Medical Education (small project grant).

Competing interests: None declared, although on occasion the authors have been known to confuse their right from their left.

Ethical approval: School of Psychology, Research Ethics Committee, Queen’s University Belfast.

Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

Cite this as: BMJ 2008;337:a2826

References

  • 1.Benton AL. Body schema disturbances: finger agnosia and right-left disorientation. In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E, eds. Clinical neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press, 1979.
  • 2.Hannay HJ, Ciaccia PJ, Kerr JW, Barrett D. Self-report of right-left confusion in college men and women. Percept Mot Skills 1990;47:451-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Snyder TJ. Self rated right-left confusability and objectively measured right-left discrimination. Dev Neuropsychol 1991;7:219-30. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Seiden SC, Barach P. Wrong-side/wrong-site, wrong-procedure, and wrong patients adverse events. Arch Surg 2006;141:931-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.General Medical Council. Tomorrow’s doctors: recommendations for change in undergraduate medical education. London: GMC, 1993.
  • 6.Ofte SH. Right-left discrimination: effects of handedness and educational background. Scand J Psychol 2002;43:213-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Oldfield OD. The assessment of handedness and educational background: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971;9:97-113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ofte SH, Hugdahl K. Right-left discrimination in male and female, young and old subjects. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2002;24:82-92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Linn MC, Peterson AC. Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. Child Dev 1985;56:1479-98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Martino G, Winner E. Talents and disorders: relationships among handedness, sex, and college major. Brain Cogn 1995;29:66-84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Johnson BW, McKenzie KJ, Hamm JP. Cerebral symmetry for mental rotation: effects of response hand, handedness and gender. Cogn Neurosci Neuropsychol 2002;13:1929-32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Auer T, Schwarcz A, Aradi M, Kalmár Z, Pendleton C, Janszky I, et al. Right-left discrimination is related to the right hemisphere. Laterality 2008;13:427-38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Goldstein D, Haldane D, Mitchell C. Sex differences in visual-spatial ability: the role of performance factors. Mem Cogn 1990;18:546-50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.McKeever WF. The influences of handedness, sex, familial sinistrality, and androgyny on language laterality and spatial ability. Cortex 1986;22:521-37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Silverman A, Adevai G, McGough W. Some relationships between handedness and perception. J Psychosom Res 1966;10:151-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Gibson JB. Intelligence and handedness. Nature 1973;243:482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Shepard R, Metzler J. Mental rotation of three dimensional objects. Science 1971;171:701-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gibbs VC. Patient safety practices in the operating room: correct-site surgery and nothing left behind. Surg Clin North Am 2005;85:1307-19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kohn LT, Corrigan JM, Donaldson MS. To err is human: building a safer health system. Washington, DC: Institute of Medicine, National Academy Press, 2000. [PubMed]

Articles from The BMJ are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES