Skip to main content
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience logoLink to Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
. 2008 Aug 7;3(4):353–366. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn022

Brain mechanisms of persuasion: how ‘expert power’ modulates memory and attitudes

Vasily Klucharev 1,2,, Ale Smidts 1, Guillén Fernández 2,3
PMCID: PMC2607059  PMID: 19015077

Abstract

Human behaviour is affected by various forms of persuasion. The general persuasive effect of high expertise of the communicator, often referred to as ’expert power’, is well documented. We found that a single exposure to a combination of an expert and an object leads to a long-lasting positive effect on memory for and attitude towards the object. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we probed the neural processes predicting these behavioural effects. Expert context was associated with distributed left-lateralized brain activity in prefrontal and temporal cortices related to active semantic elaboration. Furthermore, experts enhanced subsequent memory effects in the medial temporal lobe (i.e. in hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus) involved in memory formation. Experts also affected subsequent attitude effects in the caudate nucleus involved in trustful behaviour, reward processing and learning. These results may suggest that the persuasive effect of experts is mediated by modulation of caudate activity resulting in a re-evaluation of the object in terms of its perceived value. Results extend our view of the functional role of the dorsal striatum in social interaction and enable us to make the first steps toward a neuroscientific model of persuasion.

Keywords: persuasion, expertise, memory encoding, attitude, social influence, celebrities

INTRODUCTION

Persuasion is a fundamental form of social influence on human decision making. G.R. Miller defined persuasive communication as any message that is intended to shape, reinforce or change the responses of others (Miller, 1980). People are exposed to hundreds of persuasive messages per day in one form or another: from TV commercials to political statements and to scientific publications. Persuasion has been a focus of extensive psychological research, but it has been nearly ignored by cognitive neuroscientists. The main purpose of this study is to explore the neuronal mechanisms underlying effective persuasion. The recently emerging field of social cognitive neuroscience has predominantly studied neural mechanisms of established attitudes: prejudice, implicit and explicit attitudes (Cunningham and Zelazo, 2007; Lieberman, 2007). The current study investigates the brain mechanisms of the formation of attitudes, the primary target of persuasion. The vast diversity and popularity of advertising makes it an excellent vehicle by which persuasive communication can be studied (McClure et al., 2004). In advertising, a presenter (for example, a celebrity endorsing a product or brand) is frequently used as a source of information. The experiment described in this article simulates advertising and shows how and under which circumstances celebrities effectively persuade via the modulation of attitude-related neuronal activity.

Persuasion has been studied extensively in social psychology (Petty and Wegener, 1998; O’Keefe, 2002). Much research has focused on the persuasive impact of so-called source variables, which refers to aspects of the person presenting the persuasive appeal. One powerful source variable is high expertise (Rhine and Severance, 1970; Eagly and Chaiken, 1993; Cialdini and Goldstein, 2004): persuasiveness generally increases with communicator expertise. The persuasive effect of experts is based on the idea that people will believe the opinions of someone who is assumed to have a lot of relevant knowledge (French and Raven, 1960). Expertise is a major component of a persuader's credibility (Priester and Petty, 2003), next to trustworthiness (the source's reputation to tell the truth and be honest). High credibility sources have typically been found to be more persuasive than low credibility sources (Petty and Wegener, 1998), although in certain circumstances high credibility can backfire (Tormala et al., 2006). Modern psychological models of persuasion have discovered different mechanisms explaining the persuasive power of high credibility sources. Consistent with predictions of the elaboration likelihood (Petty and Cacioppo, 1986) and heuristic–systematic (Chaiken et al., 1989) models of persuasion, and depending on the level of elaboration, source credibility has been found to operate as peripheral cue or an heuristic (as in ‘experts are usually correct’; Petty et al., 1981), to bias thoughts (Chaiken and Maheswaran, 1994), to act as a relevant piece of information for an issue (Kruglanski and Thompson, 1999) or affecting the amount of processing that occurs (Heesacker et al., 1983).

Here, we focus on the expertise dimension of source credibility, and we take celebrities to effectively manipulate the level of expertise for a particular product. In advertising, immediately perceived expertise for the type of product the celebrity is hired to endorse, appears to be an important condition for increasing sales of a product. If the celebrity has no apparent expertise, the endorsement is likely to have no effect (Rossiter and Bellman, 2005, p. 177). For example, Bill Cosby played in a sitcom Dr Huxtable, a father caring perfectly for his children. This popular role earned him the nickname ‘America's Dad’. Thus, he was regarded as an expert for children oriented food and was very successful for endorsing a brand of gelatin deserts; at the same time, he was unsuccessful for a brokerage firm because of apparent lack of perceived expertise. Although much behavioural research has shown that our attitudes and decisions are successfully modulated by experts’ opinions, the neural underpinnings of this fundamental social phenomenon are hardly studied.

Furthermore, the current study aims to disentangle brain mechanisms of persuasive expertise effects on attitudes and memory. It has been suggested that to have a lasting persuasive effect, attitude change has to be accompanied by successful memory formation (Sawyer, 1981; Bless et al., 2001). The relationship of attitudes and declarative memory has been the subject of intense research (Eagly et al., 2001) and recent studies suggest that memory is relatively independent of attitudes (Cacioppo and Petty, 1989; Eagly et al., 2001). Furthermore, amnesic patients show intact attitude change but impaired declarative memory (Lieberman et al., 2001). Hence, different neurophysiological mechanisms appear to support declarative memory and attitudes. Therefore, we investigated the persuasive effects of perceived expertise both on memory and attitudes.

We hypothesized that effects of expertise can be modelled as a contextual modulation of memory- and attitude-related brain activity. We know that objects in our environment tend to appear in typical contexts. These highly predictable properties of our environment explain why the recognition of an object appearing in a typical context is facilitated (Bar, 2004). Similarly, we propose that a persuasive communicator creates a context for the information effectively modulating attitudes and memory. We hypothesized that the presentation of a photo of an expert celebrity before an object should alter memory- and attitude-related brain activity evoked by the object. Thus, we combined event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with memory and attitude evaluation to probe neural responses to pictures of everyday objects that are modulated by perceived expertise of celebrities.

In short, while scanning, we presented young female subjects interested in celebrities and in shopping with photographs of famous persons followed by photos of everyday objects (Figure 1). Therefore, in the current design the face of the celebrity communicates the context for the object and triggers the retrieval of information linking the object to the celebrity. Overall, this design imitates commercials where celebrities present (communicate) certain products or information. The expert association between celebrities and objects was counterbalanced across subjects, so that each celebrity served equally often as an expert and a non-expert. Attitude and memory for the objects were tested 1 day later. In that test, photos of objects were presented alone with no celebrity context. Finally, at the end of the behavioural session, celebrities’ familiarity, physical attractiveness and expertise were evaluated. This design allowed us to study whether memory and attitude-related brain activity was different for objects following experts as compared to those following non-experts.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Trial structure. During each trial of the encoding session (day 1), subjects were presented with the photo of a celebrity followed by the photo of an object (product). All stimuli were separated by varying ISI. Subjects were instructed to indicate whether or not they see a link between the celebrity and the object. The gradient bar represents the time when BOLD signal was modelled for each trials. On day 2, recognition memory and attitude towards the object presented in two separate sessions were tested. Finally, familiarity, physical attractiveness and perceived expertise of celebrities were measured (this step is not depicted here). A sketch of a celebrity and not a real photo as used in the study is presented in the figure due to potential copyright restrictions.

We used the ‘subsequent memory effect’ (SME) to study the neural correlates of memory formation for ‘advertised’ objects. The SME consistently demonstrates (Paller and Wagner, 2002) that during memory encoding, activity in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) and diverse prefrontal cortical areas is greater in response to later successfully remembered items (hits) as compared to forgotten items (misses: old items misclassified as new). Previous fMRI studies demonstrated that the context of stimulus encoding modulates SME effects in the MTL (Maratos et al., 2001; Erk et al., 2003; Smith et al., 2004). We hypothesized that brain activity in the MTL (i.e. in the hippocampus or the parahippocampal gyrus) shows an interaction between the SME for the object and the expertise of the celebrity (celebrity expertise: high vs low) presenting the object. Such interaction would indicate the mechanism of memory modulation by expert power that is similar to previously reported effects of context on memory encoding (Maratos et al., 2001; Erk et al., 2003, 2005).

To study attitude formation evoked by experts, we introduced a ‘subsequent attitude effect’ (SAE): we compared brain responses to later favoured objects (high purchase intention) with responses to later not favoured objects (low purchase intention). Thus, an interaction of the SAE with celebrity expertise should reveal the neural correlates of persuasion or attitude modulation by the perceived expertise of celebrities, i.e. by ‘expert power’. We hypothesized that a neuronal mechanism of such attitude formation could be a modulation of caudate nuclei activity by experts. This region has been previously associated not only with learning (Elliott et al., 1998; Poldrack et al., 2001; Cromwell and Schultz, 2003; Delgado et al., 2003; Shohamy et al.; 2004; Zink et al., 2004), but also with social cooperation (Rilling et al., 2002) and trust to social partners (Delgado et al., 2003; King-Casas et al., 2005). Thus, caudate nuclei activity showing the interaction of the SAE with celebrity expertise would indicate a possible mechanism of effective persuasion based on trust and a re-evaluation of the object in terms of its perceived value.

METHODS

Participants

Twenty-four healthy young right-handed females (students, mean age 21.8 years) participated in two experimental sessions: an fMRI session and a behavioural session separated by 24–30 h. None of the subjects reported a history of drug abuse, head trauma or neurological or psychiatric illness. Written informed consent was obtained according to the local medical ethics committee. Subjects’ familiarity with celebrities was a critical pre-requirement for the study; subjects were therefore selected using a specially designed questionnaire screening their interests and shopping behaviour (for additional information, see, Experimental Procedures in Supplementary Materials).

Stimuli

One hundred and eighty digital photos of celebrities (music-, TV-, sport- and movie-stars) were collected. Colour portraits of most familiar celebrities (e.g. Julia Roberts, Brad Pitt and Andre Agassi) with gaze contact and moderate smile were selected from a larger set of stimuli based on familiarity ratings of 14 young females not used as subjects in the further study. The photos were projected onto a screen with a visual angle of 12.6° vertically and 8.1° horizontally. In addition, 360 digital colour photos of both hedonic and functional everyday products (objects on white background: clothes, cosmetics, packaged food, etc.) were obtained from publicly available internet resources. Objects with no brand labels or logos were used in order to avoid a subjective bias toward preferred brands. The size of objects was approximately 8.0° × 3.0°. Photographs were similar in terms of overall visual complexity and brightness. Pictures were used to create two sets of pairs counterbalanced across subjects (Figure 1): (i) celebrities followed by a congruent object (a product that is relevant to the celebrity expertise, e.g. photo of Andre Agassi followed by a photo of a sports shoe)—high expertise condition, (ii) celebrities followed by an incongruent object (a product with no obvious link to the celebrity expertise, e.g. Andre Agassi followed by an alcohol drink)—low expertise condition. The expert association between celebrities and objects was counterbalanced across subjects, so that each celebrity served equally often as an expert and a non-expert. Only gender-relevant pairs were presented (i.e. female products or unisex products since all subjects were female) to avoid a semantic conflict. Well known celebrities were used to make the level of expertise more evident and vivid. This set-up also fits the current trend in advertising, where many advertisements these days are created with little or no explicit message (examples are Nicole Kidman's print and billboard ads for perfume and Brad Pitt's for watches, both of which simply show the star and the product). This type of print ad is usually looked at for about 1.3 s in naturalistic exposure conditions (Kroeber-Riel and Esch, 2004) and so if the ad is going to be effective, consumers have to bring to mind the relevant expertise very quickly. In the current study, celebrities and objects were presented sequentially. The celebrity first, product second sequence is in fact the usual one in broadcast ads (TV and radio), where the product or brand is presented often alone at the end of the clip to focus consumers’ attention. Even in print ads, where celebrity and products are presented simultaneously, sequential separation is the intended one because these ads are usually designed such to draw attention first to the celebrity by showing the celebrity at the top and the product lower down or at the bottom. Furthermore, for print ads, analysis of eye movement data has shown that subjects will first focus on the face and eyes of the person in the ad and subsequently on the brand name or product (Pieters and Wedel, 2004).

Procedure

While being scanned, subjects attended to sequentially presented face–object pairs and indicated by appropriate button press whether or not they perceived a link between each given celebrity and the object presented thereafter. This orienting task aimed to focus subjects’ attention and to link pairs of stimuli (a celebrity with an object) in a stream of rapidly presented 360 pictures that would be virtually impossible to achieve in a passive paradigm. The task was similar in all experimental conditions and imitated the typical advertising strategy: focusing consumers on celebrities who use certain products. In our study, pictures of celebrities and objects were presented separately in time to isolate the brain activity related to objects. Mean inter-stimulus interval (ISI) was 7.5 s (range 4.5–9.5 s) and stimuli duration was 1 s. Prior to scanning, subjects practiced the task with stimuli not used in the actual experiment.

Behavioural measures

One day later during the behavioural session, subjects’ attitudes and memory were evaluated (subjects were not informed about the purpose of the second session, therefore memory and attitude tests were unexpected by participants). Recognition memory and attitude towards the objects were measured (objects were presented now without the associated celebrity). During the recognition memory test, subjects were exposed to a sequence of pictures containing a random mixture of all objects processed the day before inside the scanner and 180 new previously unseen objects (stimuli duration was 1 s). Subjects were offered three response options: (i) picture seen before with high confidence, (ii) picture not certain to be seen before and (iii) picture not seen before with high confidence.

The subject's attitude towards the object was measured by asking participants to make an estimate of purchase incidence and indicate it on a percentage answer scale marked at intervals of 10% points, that is, ‘0, 10, 20, …, 80, 90, 100%’ (Aaker et al., 2005). Behavioural intention towards the attitude object refers to the conative component in the tripartite theory of attitude and this response may serve as an indicator of attitude (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975; Eagly and Chaiken, 1993). Therefore, we assume that during the fMRI session, celebrities modulated the attitude towards the object that was measured using the purchase incidence scale.

At the end of the behavioural session, familiarity and physical attractiveness of celebrities (presented alone) were evaluated. Finally, the level of celebrities’ expertise (i.e. how knowledgeable the celebrity is about the product) for each given type of object (product) was measured. All trials were presented in a self-paced manner. We included a measure of physical attractiveness because it is a source variable that can be of influence in persuasion (McGuire, 1969) and might also vary for the selected celebrities. Therefore, we controlled the interaction of attitudes and memory with both perceived expertise and attractiveness of celebrities. The celebrity characteristics ratings were made on 11-point unipolar scales; a rating of 0 was anchored by the description ‘not at all …’ and 10 as ‘very’ anchored the positive description at the end of the answer scale. In the analysis of the brain data, all behavioural ratings were grouped for further analysis of variance (ANOVA) in two sub-levels of studied factors: low (below the mid-point of the 11-point scale) and high levels (above the mid-point of the 11-point scale). We excluded all mid-point responses from the data processing. Behavioural results were analysed using two-tailed paired t-tests.

Expert classification

It is important to note that in analysing the brain data, the classification of the celebrities as experts or non-experts (and also attractive or non-attractive) was based on the subject's own responses in the post-scanning behavioural session and not on the pre-classification of stimuli. The celebrity–object expertise association was assessed by an independent group of age-matched female students (n = 14) prior to the actual study while developing the stimulus set. As expected, the ratings of (post-scan) perceived expertise significantly correlated with pre-selected expertise conditions (mean r = 0.64, s.d. = 0.13). However, since perceived expertise is based on the unique, individual knowledge of celebrities’ characteristics it varies across individuals, especially if one considers the large number of celebrity–object pairings (n = 180). On average 22% of celebrity–object associations received a different classification (expert vs non-expert) in the post-scan assessment as compared to the pre-scan assessment obtained in an independent sample of subjects. Thus, a classification of trials that would have been based on the pre-experimentally defined categories would contain a substantial number of false associations and thus noise. Furthermore, we conducted a statistical analysis to test the persuasive behavioural effects for pre-experimentally defined expertise. As expected, we found that the behavioural effects were strongly attenuated when the pre-experimental categorization was used as input for analysis (Supplementary Table S3). This pattern of results clearly shows that perceived (not predefined) expertise is driving the persuasive behavioural effects. We therefore based the classification of the celebrities as experts and non-experts on individual ratings obtained in the behavioural session after scanning. We consider the use of individual ratings vital for the correct manipulation of perceived expertise, because it mimics the subjective nature of this effect. We also conducted additional analyses to test whether the conditions based on post-scanning categorization still counterbalanced each other in our study. The perfect counterbalancing of celebrity–object pairs would be indicated by the probability of 0.5 of categorization of a celebrity as an expert. In fact, the average probability was 0.47 (s.d. = 0.11) based on the post-scan assessments and did not differ significantly from the expected probability of 0.5 (one sample t-test, P < 0.3). Thus, celebrity–object pairings were correctly counterbalanced across subjects.

fMRI data acquisition

fMRI was performed with ascending slice acquisition a T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging sequence [Sonata 1.5 T, Siemens, Munich, Germany; 33 axial slices; volume repetition time (TR), 2.29 s; echo time (TE), 30 ms; 90° flip angle; slice matrix, 64 × 64; slice thickness, 3.0 mm; slice gap, 0.5 mm; field of view, 224 mm]. For structural MRI, we acquired a T1-weighted MP-RAGE sequence (176 sagittal slices; volume TR, 2.25 s; TE, 3.93 ms; 15° flip angle; slice matrix, 256 × 256; slice thickness, 1.0 mm; no gap; field of view, 256 mm).

MRI data analysis

Image pre-processing and statistical analyses were performed using the Brainvoyager QX, v. 1.6 software (www.brainvoyager.com). Functional images were corrected for motion and slice scan time acquisition. Because of movement artefacts (absolute maximum 1 voxel motion), two of the 24 participants had to be excluded from data analysis. Functional data were pre-processed with linear trend removal and underwent high-pass temporal frequency filtering to remove frequencies below three cycles per run (∼0.001 Hz cut-off frequency). Functional images were co-registered with the anatomical scan and transformed into Talairach coordinate space (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988) using the nine-parameter landmark Brainvoyager method. Images were spatially smoothed with a full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel of 8 mm. The fMRI data were analysed statistically by using the general linear model. For the statistical analysis, relevant contrast parameter images were subjected to a random effects analysis. In the whole-brain search, the results from the random effects analyses were initially threshold at P < 0.001 (uncorrected), the cluster size statistics were used subsequently as the test statistic. Only clusters significant at P < 0.05 (corrected for multiple comparisons using minimum cluster-size statistics) are reported. Given that the MTL and caudate nucleus are regions of interest for SME and SAE, the MTL and caudate nucleus were additionally investigated within a spherical region of interest, thresholded at P < 0.05 [small volume corrected, radius, 10 mm; centred at (x/y/z) = ± 30/−15/−10 and ± 14/8/14] similar to previous studies (Voermans et al., 2004; Piekema et al., 2006). All local maxima are reported as Talairach coordinates (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988).

Neuropsychological studies suggest that attitudes and declarative memory can be selectively impaired and probably are represented separately in the brain (Johnson et al., 1985; Lieberman et al., 2001), and thus we investigated separately effects of perceived celebrity expertise on attitude and memory encoding. We used the SME approach as a conventional method to delineate the neural correlates of declarative memory formation (Paller et al., 1987; Wagner et al., 1998; Fernandez et al., 1999) in which later memory performance is used to back-sort neural encoding signals into events later remembered—hits and those later forgotten—misses (mean number of hits trials per experimental condition was 55.3, mean number of misses was 27.6). To study attitude formation, we introduced the SAE comparing brain responses to later favoured objects (high estimates of purchase incidence) with responses to later not favoured objects (low estimates of purchase incidence) (mean number of low purchase incidence trials per experimental condition was 34.3, mean number of high purchase incidence trials was 28.7). Due to an insufficiently small number of trials (less than 16) in one of the experimental conditions, four subjects were excluded from the analysis of SME (less than 20 misses per experimental SME condition); six subjects were removed from the analysis of SAE since these subjects demonstrated a strong bias towards preferring or rejecting most of the objects (the high estimate of purchase incidence condition contained less than 20 trials). Remaining subjects showed the same behavioural effects as the entire group (two-tailed t-test, corrected for multiple comparisons): the significant effect of the celebrity expertise on attitude [t(15, 1) = 2.2, P < 0.05], the significant effect of celebrity attractiveness on attitude [t(15, 1) = 2.1, P < 0.05], and the better memory for objects presented by experts than by non-experts [t(17, 1) = 2.1, P < 0.05].

The following 2 × 2 factorial designs were used for the analysis of brain activity:

  1. SAE (favoured vs not favoured objects) and celebrity expertise (low vs high);

  2. SAE (favoured vs not favoured objects) and celebrity attractiveness (low vs high);

  3. SME (hits vs misses) and celebrity expertise (low vs high);

  4. SME (hits vs misses) and celebrity attractiveness (low vs high).

Correlation of the recognition memory performance with the attitude towards the object was extremely weak (r = 0.053, tested across all items and subjects), which additionally ensured us that SME and SAE are of different, largely independent nature. Again, the aforementioned stimuli classification was based on subject's own responses in the post-scanning behavioural session and not on the experimenter's classification of stimuli.

RESULTS

Behavioural results: experts affect memory and attitude

We found strong persuasive behavioural effects of experts (Table 1). The effect of celebrity expertise on the attitude towards the object was significant [t(22, 1) = 3.8, P = 0.001], due to a higher purchase intention for an object that followed an expert-celebrity during encoding (44.3%, s.d. = 12.5) than objects that followed non-experts (39.6%, s.d. = 9.7). Therefore, the high level of celebrity expertise made the attitude more favourable by 4.7% (that is equivalent to 12% relative difference of the attitude for objects that followed experts as compared to those followed non-experts). Celebrity attractiveness also showed a significant effect on attitude [t(22,1) = 2.3, P = 0.03]: Subjects showed a higher preference for objects associated with physically attractive celebrities (44.0%, s.d. = 12.1) as compared to objects associated with less-attractive celebrities (41.7%, s.d. = 10.1).

Table 1.

Attitude towards and memory for the objects that followed non-experts vs objects that followed experts (means; s.d. in brackets)

Context Attitude (purchase intention, 0–100% scale) Memory (PhitsPfalse alarms)
Non-experts 39.61 (9.7) 0.644 (0.16)
Experts 44.28 (12.5) 0.706 (0.17)
P 0.001 0.006

N = 23.

P, the observed significance level.

Recognition memory performance remained clearly above chance level: t(22,1) = 12.7, P = 0.0001 (one sample t-test), the mean hit rate corrected by the rate of false alarms and excluding uncertainty responses was 68%. Subjects demonstrated better memory [t(22, 1) = 2.7, P = 0.006] for objects presented by experts (probability hits corrected by probability false alarm: 70.6%, s.d. = 17) than by non-experts (64.4%, s.d. = 16), whereas celebrity attractiveness showed no effect on subjects’ memory. Overall, our results show that experts increased the probability of object recognition memory by 6.2% (or 10% relative improvement for high as compared to low expertise trials, see Supplementary Tables S1 and 2 for further details).

In addition, we checked the effects of the orienting task in the scanner (the task of perceiving a link or not between the celebrity and the object: perceived link) on memory and attitudes. First of all, the linking task was not equal to the expertise rating obtained after scanning: only 66.6% of the perceived links where perceived as expertise links. Moreover, a two-way ANOVA with the factors link and expertise revealed a significant main effect of expertise on attitudes towards the objects [F(15,1) = 12.7, P < 0.005], but no effect of the factor link [F(15,1) = 2.8, P = 0.11]. In addition, no interaction between the factors was found. Therefore, we can assume that the linking task had no significant effect on differences in attitudes. A two-way ANOVA with the same factors on memory performance revealed a significant interaction between both factors [F(17,1)=13.97, P = 0.002]. The interaction was based on a significant memory effect of expertise only in cases when subjects detected a link between celebrities and objects. However, it is not surprising that a link identified during scanning was a prerequisite for the memory effect of expertise, given that the ‘no link’ bin contained a very low number (range 1–11 trials) of high expertise associations. Memory was not affected by the link factor itself.

Taking into account a slight correlation (r = 0.26, s.d. = 0.09) between the link and expertise factors and the small number of trials in the ‘no link&expert’ bin, we additionally conducted one-way ANOVA analyses (with three levels: ‘link&expert’, ‘link&non-expert’, ‘no link’). We found a statistically significant main effect on attitudes [F(15,1) = 8.1, P < 0.01] and memory [F(17,1) = 6.1, P < 0.01]. Planned comparisons revealed a more positive attitude and better memory toward objects that followed experts with perceived link (‘link&expert’) than toward objects in other bins (‘link&non-expert’ and ‘no link trials’), t(15,1) = 4.6, P < 0.001 and t(17,1) = 3.4, P < 0.001, respectively. Furthermore, we found no significant difference of attitudes and memory for objects in the ‘link&non-expert’ and the ‘no link’ bins. Therefore, these additional analyses revealed that the persuasive effect of expertise on memory and attitudes is driven by the celebrities’ expertise and not the linking task done during scanning.

In sum, the behavioural results show that exposure to 180 celebrity–object s pairs, results 1 day later in better memory for and a more favourable attitude towards those objects that were perceived to be presented by an expert.

fMRI results

Tables 2–4 list regions of activity increases associated with studied effects. Figures 2 and 3 represent selected statistical maps and time courses of averaged brain activity. It is important to note that our event-related fMRI analysis was time-locked to the onset of object presentation and thus independent of direct effects of celebrities preceding the presentation of objects by 4.5–9.5 s.

Table 2.

Significant activation clusters for SAE (favoured vs not favoured objects)

Brain region HEM x y z Nr of Voxels Z (max)
SAE
    Middle frontal gyrus, BA10 R 33 61 8 168 −4.4
    Anterior cingulate gyrus, BA 24 L/R 3 0 35 1098 −6.3
    Caudate nucleus (Body) R 10 −12 21 1066 −7.5
    Insula, BA 13/precentral gyrus, BA 44 R 41 7 9 926 −5.1
    Amygdala/parahippocampal gyrus, BA 34,37 R 19 0 −12 300 −4.5
    Ventral posterior thalamus L −16 −19 8 807 −5.0
    Parahippocampal gyrus, BA 35 R 24 −24 −23 688 −7.3
    Parahippocampal gyrus, BA 28 R 22 −22 −5 829 −6.5
    Parahippocampal gyrus, BA36 L −36 −23 −13 699 −5.4
    Superior temporal gyrus, BA 13 L −51 −39 17 830 −5.8
    Middle occipital gyrus, BA19 R 37 −66 3 1331 −9.3
    Lingual gyrus, BA 18 L −11 −67 −2 1120 −7.4
    Cuneus/posterior cingulate gyrus, BA30 R 10 −64 9 1313 −7.2

Favoured objects are objects with high subsequent estimates of purchase incidence, not favoured objects are objects with low estimates of purchase incidence.

Cluster threshold at a significance level of P<0.05 were corrected for multiple comparisons using minimum cluster-size statistics the family-wise error rate or using small volume corrections (see Methods section). Local maxima within these clusters are reported together with the number of voxels (Nr of Voxels). x, y, z are coordinates of the cluster centre.

Table 3.

Significant activation clusters for memory effects (hits vs misses), celebrity expertise (experts vs non-experts) and celebrity attractiveness (attractive vs unattractive)

Brain region HEM x y z Nr Of Voxels Z (max)
SME
    Inferior frontal gyrus, BA 45, 46 L −47 26 13 1550 7.2
    Middle frontal gyrus, BA 46 R 39 29 18 738 8.0
    Caudate nucleus (Body) L −9 11 9 1022 7.7
    Medial globus pallidus L −8 −4 1 1088 6.8
    Parahippocampal gyrus L −27 −9 −10 752 7.2
    Posterior parahippocampal gyrus, BA 36,37 L −27 −38 −11 1230 6.6
    Parahippocampal gyrus, BA 36 R 24 −34 −17 1307 7.3
    Parahippocampal gyrus/hippocampus L −29 −9 −12 548 6.6
    Fusiform gyrus, BA 37 L −42 −54 −10 1221 10.2
    Middle temporal gyrus, middle occipital gyrus, BA 19 L −30 −68 28 873 6.1
Celebrity expertise
    Precuneus BA 19 L −34 −67 36 1108 5.8
    Medial frontal gyrus, BA 6, cingulate gyrus, BA 24/31 L −5 34 36 962 6.0
    Anterior cingulate gyrus, BA 24 L −4 13 48 642 7.2
    Superior frontal gyrus, BA 10 L −6 62 21 522 6.9
    Inferior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus BA 6/9 L −40 6 30 690 5.5
    Medial dorsal thalamus L −8 −14 6 660 6.2
    Superior temporal gyrus, BA22/39 L −41 −51 23 645 5.2
Celebrity attractiveness no

Table 4.

Significant activation clusters for persuasive effects

Brain region HEM x y z Nr of Voxels Z (max)
Celebrity expertise x SAE
    Caudate nucleus (Body) L −12 11 8 702 5.6
    Caudate nucleus (Body) R 12 12 7 355 4.1
    Superior frontal gyrus, BA 9 L −14 50 25 688 4.5
    Superior frontal gyrus, BA 10 R 9 66 18 553 3.9
Celebrity attractiveness x SAE no
Celebrity expertise x SME
    Parahippocampal gyrus/hippocampus L −30 −10 −8 603 3.8
    Parahippocampal gyrus/hippocampus R 38 −17 −7 332 4.7
    Lingual gyrus, BA 18–19/fusiform gyrus L −17 −68 −5 797 5.12
    Anterior cingulate gyrus, BA 24 L/R 4 −3 37 849 4.6

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Main effects of attitudes (SAE), memory (SME) and celebrity expertise (A) SAE on neural activation—the contrast of subsequently favoured versus not favoured objects (high vs low estimates of purchase incidence); n = 16. (B) SME on neural activation (parahippocampal/fusiform gyrus)—the contrast between brain activity to later successfully remembered objects (hits) vs forgotten objects (misses); n = 18. (C) Effect of perceived celebrity expertise on neural activation—the contrast of brain activity related to objects that followed experts vs objects that followed non-experts during the period following object encoding. Amg, amygdala; CG, cingular gyrus; DPF, dorsal prefrontal cortex; PHG, parahippocampal/fusiform gyrus region; STG, superior temporal gyrus region; Th, thalamus; R, right hemisphere.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Persuasive expertise effects on attitudes and memory (A) The interaction of perceived celebrity expertise with SAE. The left panel depicts the interaction in the caudate nucleus. The right panel depicts the averaged fMRI signal for the left caudate nucleus cluster. The averaged fMRI signals for not favoured objects (with low estimates of purchase incidence) that followed non-experts (dark blue), favoured objects (with high estimates of purchase incidence) that followed non-experts (pink), not favoured objects that followed experts (yellow) and favoured objects that followed experts (light blue) are displayed. The averaged fMRI signals were calculated for all significant voxels within the cluster. The error bars depict standard errors of the mean. n = 16. (B) The interaction of perceived celebrity expertise with subsequent recognition memory (SME). The left panel depicts the interaction in the left hippocampus/parahippocampal cortex. The right panel depicts the fMRI signal for the left parahippocampal cluster. The averaged fMRI signals for subsequent misses (objects) that followed non-experts (dark blue), subsequent hits that followed non-experts (pink), subsequent misses that followed experts (yellow) and subsequent hits that followed experts (light blue) are displayed. PHG, parahippocampal cortex. n = 18.

Subsequent attitude effect

While the main effect of the factor SAE did not reveal any activity increase for favoured objects as compared to not favoured objects, activity in a distributed cortical and sub-cortical network was stronger for not favoured objects as compared to favoured ones (Table 2). Anteriorly, it comprised the superior and middle frontal gyrus, cingulate gyrus and insular cortex. In the MTL, the amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus were activated (Figure 2A). Posteriorly, the set of activations included the middle occipital gyrus, lingual gyrus, the superior temporal gyrus, the cuneus and the posterior cingulate. Sub-cortically, the caudate nucleus and the ventral posterior medial thalamus were also activated by not favoured objects. Previous studies have associated each of these areas with processing of negative, aversive information and negative attitudes (LeDoux, 2000; Cunningham et al., 2003; O’Doherty et al., 2003; Cunningham et al., 2004a, b; Coricelli et al., 2005).

Subsequent memory effect

The inferior and middle frontal gyri, anterior cingulate gyrus, the caudate nucleus, the globus pallidus, the parahippocampal gyrus, the hippocampus, the fusiform gyrus, the middle temporal gyrus and the middle occipital gyrus all yielded greater activity for subsequently remembered than forgotten objects (Table 3, Figure 2B). This activation of brain regions, known to be involved in declarative memory encoding (Brewer et al., 1998; Wagner et al., 1998; Fernandez et al., 1999), confirmed the sensitivity of the SME paradigm applied here [for additional discussion, see SME in Supplementary Materials].

Celebrity expertise affects processing of objects

The analysis of the main effect of the factor celebrity expertise clarified how experts modulate neuronal processing of subsequently presented objects independently of behavioural effects. We found that objects following celebrities with high expertise elicited stronger activation than objects presented by non-experts in brain regions (Figure 2C and Table 3) associated with semantic processing [left dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (PFC), anterior cingulate and superior temporal sulcus] (Leveroni et al., 2000; Kraut et al., 2002), retrieval of episodic and autobiographical memories (Maguire, 2001; Moscovitch et al., 2005) and in mentalizing about thoughts, intentions or beliefs of others referred to in the ‘theory of mind’ (Gallagher and Frith, 2003; Singer, 2006). Thus, our neuroimaging results indicate that experts induced a semantic or social context for the objects, which can be used for conceptual and associative processing. Moreover, activity in the posterior superior temporal sulcus and adjacent regions was previously observed when subjects made trustworthiness judgments of faces (Winston et al., 2002). Therefore, superior temporal sulcus activity in our study could reflect active processing of celebrity's personality, desires and intentions. Overall, in our study, photos of objects following those of experts induced distributed left-lateralized brain activity indicating active semantic elaboration and theory of mind judgments.

Effects of attractiveness

We found no activations demonstrating the significant main effects of the factor celebrity attractiveness. Moreover, we did not find any interaction between the factors celebrity attractiveness and SAE. The behavioural effect of attractiveness in our study might be too small to be detected at the neural level. Additionally, the variability of celebrity attractiveness in the current study might have been too small to effectively manipulate the factor of attractiveness in fMRI data. Overall, subjects found the celebrities moderately attractive (mean attractiveness 4.8, s.d. = 2.4 using an 11-point unipolar scale of attractiveness, ranging from 0 to 10). On average, only three celebrities out of 180 were perceived as very attractive and only nine as absolutely non-attractive. Such relatively small variability of attractiveness might also explain why no main effect was observed for the factor ‘celebrity attractiveness’. In contrast, the factor of celebrity expertise was very successfully manipulated in our study: celebrities classified as experts had much higher perceived expertise (mean expertise = 7.7 on an 11-point unipolar scale of expertise ranging from 0 to 10, s.d. = 1.3) than non-experts (mean expertise = 2.2, s.d. = 1.4).

Persuasive expertise effects on attitude- and memory-related neural activity

Probing the interaction between the factor of celebrity expertise on the one hand and the SAE and SME factors on the other hand aims at revealing the neural underpinnings of our behavioural finding of a more favourable attitude and better memory for objects presented by experts as compared to objects presented by non-experts. We found an interaction between the factor celebrity expertise and SAE in a set of brain structures including the superior frontal gyrus, left and right caudate nuclei (Figure 3A and Table 4). Celebrities with high expertise evoked particularly enhanced caudate activity to objects that were later evaluated as attractive. This result suggests that modulation of caudate activity is involved in triggering the persuasive behavioural effect of experts.

In addition, we found an interaction of celebrity expertise and SAE in the PFC. This brain area has been associated with subjective intensity of emotions and interaction of emotional evaluation with attention (Elliott et al., 1998; Gusnard et al., 2001; Cromwell and Schultz, 2003; Anders et al., 2004; Dolcos et al., 2004; Northoff et al., 2004; Zink et al., 2004; Grimm et al., 2006). Taking into account that both the caudate nucleus and the dorsomedial PFC are generally involved in processing emotional stimuli, monitoring of action-outcome contingency (Yin and Knowlton, 2006), we can assume that the persuasive effect of experts at the final stage is pre-dominantly based on an emotional reaction, that modulates the subject's attitude towards any given object. Observed modulation of caudate and dorsomedial PFC activity may thus explain our behavioural results, showing the striking effect of celebrity expertise on the attitude towards objects. Observed differences of attitudes can only be explained by changes of attitudes, because our celebrity–object pairings were counterbalanced across subjects. Thus, we can assume that the only reason why attitudes for objects presented with an expert differed from attitudes for objects presented together with a non-expert is a modulation of attitudes by perceived expertise.

Effective persuasion not only affects attitudes but also memory. To reveal the neural underpinnings of this behavioural effect we analysed the interaction between the factors celebrity expertise and SME. The behavioural effect was paralleled at the brain system level: we found an interaction of the perceived expertise and activity associated to successful memory encoding, first of all in the MTL and related regions (Figure 3B and Table 4): hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, lingual gyrus, fusiform gyrus and cingulate gyrus were activated by later successfully recognized objects presented in the context of an expert celebrity. All aforementioned structures are known to be involved in successful declarative memory encoding (Paller and Wagner, 2002). Our finding suggests that the facial context does directly modulate (enhance) encoding activity and therefore optimizes memory formation.

DISCUSSION

In the present study, we found that experts made the attitude toward objects more favourable by 12% and increased the probability of object recognition by 10%. In everyday life, the apparent expertise of the communicator has a striking impact on persuasion. Earlier studies (Page et al., 1987; Jordan, 1993) showed that a single expert's publication in The New York Times newspaper, or broadcasting the expert's opinion on national TV, can change public opinion on policy issues by up to 4%. Similarly, in advertising, expert celebrity-product pairings can be very successful, such as Tiger Woods for golf equipment or celebrity chef Jamie Oliver as the presenter for a UK food retailer, with his addition to the brand estimated to have resulted in about $400 million in incremental profit over 5 years (Pringle, 2004). Our behavioural results for attitude formation confirm the typical finding in the psychological literature that high expertise sources are generally more persuasive (Petty and Wegener, 1998; O’Keefe, 2002; Rossiter and Bellman, 2005), whereas the neuroimaging results revealed the neural underpinnings of such persuasive behavioural effects.

As we expected, the interaction of celebrities’ expertise and attitudes toward objects was found in a set of brain structures including the left and right caudate nuclei. Feedback processing and learning have been previously associated with neuronal activity in the caudate nucleus (Elliott et al., 1998; Poldrack et al., 2001; Cromwell and Schultz, 2003; Delgado et al., 2003; Shohamy et al., 2004; Zink et al., 2004). Substantial evidence implicates the caudate in reward-related tasks, including responses linked to positive affect, expectations or receipt of reward (Apicella et al., 1991; Kawagoe et al., 1998; Lauwereyns et al., 2002). Activity of the caudate was also connected to social cooperation (Rilling et al., 2002) and social conflict (Berns et al., 2005). Recently, the role of the caudate in processing such social information as perceived fairness of social partners, was demonstrated (Delgado et al., 2003; King-Casas et al., 2005). It has been shown that caudate activity correlates with the ‘intention to trust’ on the next play of a trust game and with player reputation development (King-Casas et al., 2005). Moreover, the perceived trustworthiness of the playing partner modulated caudate activity to the outcome (feedback) of the game (Delgado et al., 2005). The trustworthy partner reduced the difference of responses to positive and negative outcomes. Even punishment or violation of trust increased caudate activity (de Quervain et al., 2004). Consequently, in the current study, we propose to interpret the persuasive effect of a celebrity with high expertise for the object, in terms of inducing ‘trust’ to the object (inducing trust to the product's quality) and in such a way modulating the attitude towards the object. It is important to stress here that high expertise induces trust to the object (product), as in everyday life where we state that ‘we trust the opinion of the expert for this product or topic’. This effect should be clearly distinguished from trust in the person as a general characteristic (i.e. celebrity trustworthiness: having a reputation to be honest). Whereas expertise can only be established in connection to an object or message (i.e. an expert source possesses the requisite knowledge for this object), trustworthiness exists independent of an object or message. Since we counterbalanced celebrities in our study and presented the same celebrity both in the role of expert and non-expert, celebrity trustworthiness can not explain the current results. Alternatively, the level of ambiguity in choices has been shown to be negatively correlated with activity in the caudate nucleus (Hsu et al., 2005). Therefore, persuasive information can also compensate a shortage of relevant information about new objects and modulate the degree of uncertainty. Overall, an expert might decrease the perceived risk of purchasing an unknown object. Thus, our results demonstrate that experts effectively modulate activity in neural structures (i.e. caudate nucleus) involved in trustful behaviour and risk evaluation. We suggest that the persuasive effect of experts is mediated by the modulation of caudate activity resulting in a re-evaluation of the object in terms of its perceived value, related attitudes or risk–reward tradeoffs.

A large body of literature shows that recent exposure to a target makes the target more readily accessible in memory; as a result, this increased accessibility enhances the fluency of target recognition, which is referred to as ‘processing fluency’ (Jacoby and Dallas, 1981). Similarly, ‘conceptual fluency’ reflects the ease with which the target comes to mind and activates meanings (Hamann, 1990). There is a considerable evidence that processing and conceptual fluency are affectively positive (Reber et al., 1998; Lee and Labroo, 2004). Thus, in the current study, celebrities could prime the congruent objects and facilitate processing and conceptual fluency resulting in positive attitudes. On the other hand, our behavioural results showed that the perceived link between celebrities and objects, indicating the strength of a general conceptual association within celebrity–object pairs, did not affect attitudes. Therefore, our results suggest that fluency per se does not explain the entire behavioural effect of expertise that is probably based on an emotional re-evaluation of objects.

Importantly, the fMRI method is correlational in nature, which creates a causal ambiguity (Cacioppo et al., 2003). The alternative account predicts a random classification of celebrities as experts or non-experts due to neural ‘noise’. On the contrary, the expertise ratings significantly correlated with pre-selected high expertise condition (on average, r = 0.64, s.d. = 1.3). It means that classification was not random which makes the alternative interpretation of the results implausible. In the future, it would be important to study other aspects of expertise, e.g. professional expertise (such as a doctor in a white coat endorsing a medication) that allows unambiguous pre-experimental manipulation of expertise and resolves causal ambiguity.

In accordance with our hypothesis, persuasive effects on memory could be explained in the framework of contextual memory studies. Previous fMRI studies reliably demonstrated that emotional context of stimulus encoding modulates SME effects in the MTL, fusiform and lingual gyri (Maratos et al., 2001; Erk et al., 2003; Smith et al., 2004). Context was often manipulated by overlaying emotionally neutral stimuli on various emotional stimuli: emotionally neutral words overlaid on emotionally positive, negative or neutral pictures (Erk et al., 2003); neutral words included in affective sentences (Maratos et al., 2001); emotionally neutral objects superimposed on negative, positive or neutral backgrounds (Smith et al., 2004). One recent study (Adcock et al., 2006) showed that MTL activity preceding the stimulus to be memorized is modulated by the emotional context (high or low level of motivation) and reliably predicts memory encoding. Previously mentioned paradigms are quite similar to the presentation of products in advertising where neutral objects often overlay on or follow the emotional context. Therefore, the modulation of MTL activity can be an important target of effective persuasion. Enhanced MTL activity strengthens the memory of an object (i.e. the increase of ‘brand awareness’), which is an important marketing objective aiming to narrow the consumer's selection of a product to a list of familiar and well-known brands (these familiar brands comprise the so-called awareness set). Consumers more probably consider and choose a product from the awareness set (Peter and Olsen, 2005). Alternatively, observed persuasive memory effects could be based on semantic priming mechanisms: the facilitated processing of an object following prior experience with a semantically related stimulus (a celebrity) (Meyer and Schvaneveldt, 1971). However, our results contradict with the repetition suppression usually observed in semantic priming experiments—repeated stimuli (similar or semantically related) are typically associated with decreased brain responses (Henson, 2003). Another alternative explanation for the persuasive effect could be that an expert, e.g. a tennis player, activates a schema of tennis, facilitating thereby schema-driven processing of semantically related objects. In fact, such schema-driven processing could be an intrinsic part of the persuasive effect of expertise. Additional studies are needed to further explore the relationship between these processes (Lieberman et al., 2004; Tse et al., 2007).

The main effect of attitude (SAE) revealed activity in a distributed cortical and sub-cortical network that was stronger for unfavourable as compared to favourable attitudes: prefrontal, cingular, occipital and insular cortices, amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus. Previous studies have associated each of these areas with processing of negative, aversive information and negative attitudes (LeDoux, 2000; Cunningham et al., 2003; O’Doherty et al., 2003; Cunningham et al., 2004a,b; Coricelli et al., 2005). Our results support the recent hypothesis that the amygdala and bilateral parahippocampal regions are implicated in the emotional evaluation of attitude (Wood et al., 2005). A recent study (Knutson et al., 2007) demonstrated that product preference and subsequent purchasing was correlated with deactivation of the insula. We found brain activity related to negative attitudes and no activity related to positive attitudes (Cunningham et al., 2003). A lack of positive effects could be related to the selection of objects in our study because we did not pre-select objects with above average attractiveness as in the study that additionally reported some positive effects (Knutson et al., 2007). Thus, our findings seem in line with the behavioural evidence that in economic decisions, people are more concerned with avoiding losses than acquiring gains (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979). Also, from a decision making perspective, it is critical for survival of an organism to be able to decline quickly the many irrelevant choice options and to consider the small list of positive alternatives. From these results, we conclude that the distributed set of brain regions that includes a number of limbic structures is selectively activated by negative attitudes that are very instrumental for buying decisions.

Finally, one may ask what we have learned about persuasion from using neuroscience that we did not know from existing behavioural research already. Firstly, the current study brings a neurobiological account to the research of persuasion, i.e. our study more precisely specifies which processes are underlying the well-known persuasive effect of expertise. Whereas under low elaboration expertise is generally considered to work as a peripheral cue, on the neural level expertise appears to activate a combination of three processes: more semantic processing and elaboration on the celebrity–object combination (leading to) a deeper encoding of the object, and an emotional induction of trust to the object. Furthermore, our study hopes to bridge fields of neuroscience and psychology, and contribute to an evolving interdisciplinary perspective on persuasion. Traditionally, behavioural persuasion research conceptualizes the persuasive impact of source variables (e.g. source expertise) as an effect of context of communication (O’Keefe, 2002). We show how neuroscientific tools developed to investigate neural contextual effects (Erk et al., 2003, 2005; Bar, 2004) can be applied to the study of persuasion. We have demonstrated that the expert context modulates memory formation at the level of the MTL and attitude formation at the level of the caudate nucleus. Moreover, the observed declarative memory and attitude routes of persuasion are parallel and rather independent (Lieberman et al., 2001). Finally, our findings indicate that attention modulation probably is not a major mechanism of ‘expert power’, i.e. we did not find persuasive effects in sensory cortices. After this first step, further neuroimaging studies of persuasion probing the effects of persuasive messages, different levels of elaboration and various attributes of communicators are needed to help to understand better the mechanisms underlying psychological theories of persuasion. The current study investigated neural mechanisms of persuasion predictive for subsequent attitudes and recognition. Thus, we studied processes of ‘persuasion per se’ and not simply consequences of processing of objects following being persuaded. One can speculate that if objects are later encountered again, it will trigger brain processes related to more positive emotional evaluation, fluent processing and extensive associative retrieval. Thus, the processing might pave the way for more favourable attitudes (activating ventral striatum) and stronger integration into semantic schemata (PFC and MTL).

CONCLUSIONS

Our results show that a single short exposure to an expert results in long-term modulation of memory and attitude for an object following the expert shortly after. Our results indicate a combination of routes of neural processing underlying these persuasive effects of expertise. First, a celebrity with perceived expertise induces left lateralized activity due to higher elaboration of celebrity–object pairs, i.e. the retrieval and processing of semantic (social) information related to the celebrity and the object. Second, objects to be avoided trigger emotional processing retrieving initial attitudes within the amygdala and insular cortex. Third, experts enhance MTL activity related to successful memory formation resulting in better memory of the objects. Finally, experts modulate caudate and dorsomedial PFC activity that predicts more favourable attitudes toward the objects. Involvement of the caudate nucleus suggests a possible biological mechanism of persuasion, i.e. the experts’ context modulates evaluation of the object in terms of its perceived value, trust or risk–reward tradeoffs. By and large, our data suggest that experts (persuaders) modulate the activity in a set of brain regions involved in trustful behaviour learning and declarative memory encoding that probably enables effective persuasion. Our results thus start to uncover neuronal mechanisms underlying persuasion.

SUPPLEMENTARY DATA

Supplementary data are available at SCAN online.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENT

We thank Vasiliki Folia for assistance in preparation of study design and pilot tests, Paul Gaalman and Gitty Smit for assistance in fMRI experiments, Jens Schwarzbach for his expertise in the fMRI data processing, Ivan Toni for his fruitful comments and John Rossiter for his expertise in persuasive communication.

REFERENCES

  1. Aaker DA, Kumar V, Day GS. 7th edn. New York: Wiley; 2005. Marketing Research. [Google Scholar]
  2. Adcock RA, Thangavel A, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Knutson B, Gabrieli JD. Reward-motivated learning: mesolimbic activation precedes memory formation. Neuron. 2006;50:507–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anders S, Lotze M, Erb M, Grodd W, Birbaumer N. Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal: a response-related fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping. 2004;23:200–9. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Apicella P, Ljungberg T, Scarnati E, Schultz W. Responses to reward in monkey dorsal and ventral striatum. Experiential Brain Research. 1991;85:491–500. doi: 10.1007/BF00231732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bar M. Visual objects in context. Nature reviews. Neuroscience. 2004;5:617–29. doi: 10.1038/nrn1476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Berns GS, Chappelow J, Zink CF, Pagnoni G, Martin-Skurski ME, Richards J. Neurobiological correlates of social conformity and independence during mental rotation. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;58:245–53. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bless H, Strack F, Walther E. Memory as a target of social influence? Memory distortions as a function of social influence and meta-cognitive knowledge. In: Forgas JP, Williams KD, editors. Social Influence: Direct and Indirect Processes. Philadelphia: Psychology Press; 2001. pp. 167–83. [Google Scholar]
  8. Brewer JB, Zhao Z, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD. Making memories: brain activity that predicts how well visual experience will be remembered. Science. 1998;281:1185–7. doi: 10.1126/science.281.5380.1185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cacioppo JT, Berntson GG, Lorig TS, Norris CJ, Rickett E, Nusbaum H. Just because you're imaging the brain doesn't mean you can stop using your head: a primer and set of first principles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;85:650–661. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cacioppo JT, Petty RE. Effects of message repetition on argument processing, recall, and persuasion. Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 1989;10:3–12. [Google Scholar]
  11. Chaiken S, Liberman A, Eagly AH. Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In: Uleman JS, Bargh JA, editors. Unintended thought. New York: Guilford Press; 1989. pp. 212–52. [Google Scholar]
  12. Chaiken S, Maheswaran D. Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;66:460–73. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.66.3.460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cialdini RB, Goldstein NJ. Social influence: compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology. 2004;55:591–621. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Coricelli G, Critchley HD, Joffily M, ODoherty JP, Sirigu A, Dolan RJ. Regret and its avoidance: a neuroimaging study of choice behavior. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1255–62. doi: 10.1038/nn1514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cromwell HC, Schultz W. Effects of expectations for different reward magnitudes on neuronal activity in primate striatum. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;89:2823–38. doi: 10.1152/jn.01014.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cunningham WA, Johnson MK, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Banaji MR. Neural components of social evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;85:639–49. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cunningham WA, Johnson MK, Raye CL, Chris Gatenby J, Gore JC, Banaji MR. Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science. 2004a;15:806–13. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00760.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cunningham WA, Raye CL, Johnson MK. Implicit and explicit evaluation: FMRI correlates of valence, emotional intensity, and control in the processing of attitudes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004b;16:1717–29. doi: 10.1162/0898929042947919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cunningham WA, Zelazo PD. Attitudes and evaluations: a social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2007;11:97–104. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. de Quervain DJ, Fischbacher U, Treyer V, et al. The neural basis of altruistic punishment. Science. 2004;305:1254–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1100735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Delgado MR, Frank RH, Phelps EA. Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1611–8. doi: 10.1038/nn1575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Delgado MR, Locke HM, Stenger VA, Fiez JA. Dorsal striatum responses to reward and punishment: effects of valence and magnitude manipulations. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;3:27–38. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.1.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dolcos F, LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Dissociable effects of arousal and valence on prefrontal activity indexing emotional evaluation and subsequent memory: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2004;23:64–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Eagly AH, Chaiken S. The Psychology of Attitudes. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  25. Eagly AH, Kulesa P, Chen S, Chaiken S. Do attitudes affect memory? Tests of the congeniality hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2001;10:5–9. [Google Scholar]
  26. Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, Michael A, Paykel ES, Dolan RJ. Abnormal neural response to feedback on planning and guessing tasks in patients with unipolar depression. Psychological Medicine. 1998;28:559–71. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798006709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Erk S, Kiefer M, Grothe J, Wunderlich AP, Spitzer M, Walter H. Emotional context modulates subsequent memory effect. Neuroimage. 2003;18:439–47. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00015-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Erk S, Martin S, Walter H. Emotional context during encoding of neutral items modulates brain activation not only during encoding but also during recognition. Neuroimage. 2005;26:829–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fernandez G, Effern A, Grunwald T, et al. Real-time tracking of memory formation in the human rhinal cortex and hippocampus. Science. 1999;285:1582–5. doi: 10.1126/science.285.5433.1582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Reading: Addison-Wesley; 1975. Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: an introduction to theory and research. [Google Scholar]
  31. French JPRJ, Raven B. The bases of social power. In: Cartwright D, Zander A, editors. Group dynamics. New York: Harper and Row; 1960. pp. 607–23. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gallagher HL, Frith CD. Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2003;7:77–83. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)00025-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Grimm S, Schmidt CF, Bermpohl F, et al. Segregated neural representation of distinct emotion dimensions in the prefrontal cortex-an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2006;30:325–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2001;98:4259–64. doi: 10.1073/pnas.071043098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hamann S. Level-of-processing effects in conceptually driven implicit tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1990;16:970–7. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.22.4.933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Heesacker MH, Petty RE, Cacioppo JT. Field dependence and attitude change: source credibility can alter persuasion by affecting message-relevant thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1983;51:653–66. [Google Scholar]
  37. Henson RN. Neuroimaging studies of priming. Progress in Neurobiology. 2003;70:53–81. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(03)00086-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hsu M, Bhatt M, Adolphs R, Tranel D, Camerer CF. Neural systems responding to degrees of uncertainty in human decision-making. Science. 2005;310:1680–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1115327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Jacoby L, Dallas M. On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1981;110:306–40. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.110.3.306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Johnson MK, Kim JK, Risse G. Do alcoholic Korsakoff's syndrome patients acquire affective reations? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1985;11:22–36. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.11.1.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jordan LD. Newspaper effects on policy preferences. Public Opinion Quarterly. 1993;57:191–204. [Google Scholar]
  42. Kahneman D, Tversky A. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica. 1979;47:263–91. [Google Scholar]
  43. Kawagoe R, Takikawa Y, Hikosaka O. Expectation of reward modulates cognitive signals in the basal ganglia. Nature Neuroscience. 1998;1:411–6. doi: 10.1038/1625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. King-Casas B, Tomlin D, Anen C, Camerer CF, Quartz SR, Montague PR. Getting to know you: reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange. Science. 2005;308:78–83. doi: 10.1126/science.1108062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Knutson B, Rick S, Wimmer GE, Prelec D, Loewenstein G. Neural predictors of purchases. Neuron. 2007;53:147–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kraut MA, Kremen S, Segal JB, Calhoun V, Moo LR, Hart J., Jr Object activation from features in the semantic system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14:24–36. doi: 10.1162/089892902317205294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kroeber-Riel W, Esch F.-R. Strategie und Technik der Werbung. Stuttgart. Germany: Kohlhammer; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  48. Kruglanski AW, Thompson EP. Persuasion by a single route: a view from the unimodel. Psychological Inquiry. 1999;10:83–109. [Google Scholar]
  49. Lauwereyns J, Takikawa Y, Kawagoe R, et al. Feature-based anticipation of cues that predict reward in monkey caudate nucleus. Neuron. 2002;33:463–73. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00571-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2000;23:155–84. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lee A, Labroo A. The effect of conceptual and perceptual fluency on brand evaluation. Journal of Marketing Research, XLI. 2004:151–65. [Google Scholar]
  52. Leveroni CL, Seidenberg M, Mayer AR, Mead LA, Binder JR, Rao SM. Neural systems underlying the recognition of familiar and newly learned faces. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:878–86. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-02-00878.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lieberman MD. Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:259–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Lieberman MD, Jarcho JM, Satpute AB. Evidence-based and intuition-based self-knowledge: an FMRI study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2004;87:421–35. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Lieberman MD, Ochsner KN, Gilbert DT, Schacter DL. Do amnesics exhibit cognitive dissonance reduction? The role of explicit memory and attention in attitude change. Psychological Science. 2001;12:135–40. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Maguire EA. Neuroimaging studies of autobiographical event memory. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological sciences. 2001;356:1441–51. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0944. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Maratos EJ, Dolan RJ, Morris JS, Henson RN, Rugg MD. Neural activity associated with episodic memory for emotional context. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:910–920. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00025-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. McClure SM, Li J, Tomlin D, Cypert KS, Montague LM, Montague PR. Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks. Neuron. 2004;44:379–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. McGuire WJ. The nature of attitudes and attitude change. In: Lindzey G, Aronson E, editors. The Handbook of Social Psychology. Reading: Addison-Wesley; 1969. pp. 136–314. [Google Scholar]
  60. Meyer DE, Schvaneveldt RW. Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1971;90:227–34. doi: 10.1037/h0031564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Miller GR. On being persuaded: some basic distinctions. In: Roloff ME, Miller GR, editors. Persuasion: New directions in theory and research. Beverly Hills: Sage; 1980. pp. 11–27. [Google Scholar]
  62. Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS, Gilboa A, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: a unified account based on multiple trace theory. Journal of Anatomy. 2005;207:35–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00421.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Northoff G, Heinzel A, Bermpohl F, et al. Reciprocal modulation and attenuation in the prefrontal cortex: an fMRI study on emotional-cognitive interaction. Human Brain Mapping. 2004;21:202–12. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. O’Doherty J, Critchley H, Deichmann R, Dolan RJ. Dissociating valence of outcome from behavioral control in human orbital and ventral prefrontal cortices. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:7931–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-21-07931.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. O’Keefe D. 2nd edn. Vol. 2. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 2002. Persuasion. Theory & Research. [Google Scholar]
  66. Page B, Shapiro R, Dempsey G. Television news and changes in Americans’ policy preferences. American Political Science Review. 1987;83:23–44. [Google Scholar]
  67. Paller KA, Kutas M, Mayes AR. Neural correlates of encoding in an incidental learning paradigm. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1987;67:360–71. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(87)90124-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Paller KA, Wagner AD. Observing the transformation of experience into memory. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2002;6:93–102. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01845-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Peter JP, Olsen JC. Consumer behavior and marketing strategy. 7th. Irwin, New York: McGraw-Hill; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  70. Petty RE, Cacioppo JT. Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York: Springer; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  71. Petty RE, Cacioppo JT, Goldman R. Personal involvement as a determinant of argument-based persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1981;41:847–55. [Google Scholar]
  72. Petty RE, Wegener DT. Attitude change: multiple roles for persuasion variables. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G, editors. The Handbook of Social Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1998. pp. 323–90. [Google Scholar]
  73. Piekema C, Kessels RP, Mars RB, Petersson KM, Fernandez G. The right hippocampus participates in short-term memory maintenance of object-location associations. Neuroimage. 2006;33:374–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Pieters R, Wedel M. Attention capture and transfer in advertising: brand, pictorial, and text-size effects. Journal of Marketing. 2004;68:36–50. [Google Scholar]
  75. Poldrack RA, Clark J, Pare-Blagoev EJ, et al. Interactive memory systems in the human brain. Nature. 2001;414:546–50. doi: 10.1038/35107080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Priester RJ, Petty ER. The influence of spokesperson trustworthiness on message elaboration, attitude strength, and advertising effectiveness. Journal of Consumer Psychology. 2003;13:408–21. [Google Scholar]
  77. Pringle H. Celebrity Sells. Chichester, West Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  78. Reber R, Winkielman P, Schwarz N. Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments. Psychological Science. 1998;29:45–8. [Google Scholar]
  79. Rhine R, Severance L. Ego-involvement, discrepancy, source credibility, and attitude change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1970;16:175–90. [Google Scholar]
  80. Rilling J, Gutman D, Zeh T, Pagnoni G, Berns G, Kilts C. A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron. 2002;35:395–405. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00755-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Rossiter J, Bellman S. Marketing Communications. Frenchs Forest: Prentice Hall; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  82. Sawyer AG. Repetition, cognitive response and persuasion. In: Richard P, Tom O, Timothy B, editors. Cognitive Responses to Persuasion. New York: Hillsdale; 1981. pp. 237–62. [Google Scholar]
  83. Shohamy D, Myers CE, Grossman S, Sage J, Gluck MA, Poldrack RA. Cortico-striatal contributions to feedback-based learning: converging data from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Brain. 2004;127:851–9. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Singer T. The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: review of literature and implications for future research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2006;30:855–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Smith AP, Henson RN, Dolan RJ, Rugg MD. fMRI correlates of the episodic retrieval of emotional contexts. Neuroimage. 2004;22:868–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-Planar Stereotactic Atlas of the Human Brain. Stuttgart: Thieme; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  87. Tormala Z, Briñol P, Petty RE. When credibility attacks: the reverse impact of source credibility on persuasion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2006;42:684–91. [Google Scholar]
  88. Tse D, Langston RF, Kakeyama M, et al. Schemas and memory consolidation. Science. 2007;316:76–82. doi: 10.1126/science.1135935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Voermans NC, Petersson KM, Daudey L, et al. Interaction between the human hippocampus and the caudate nucleus during route recognition. Neuron. 2004;43:427–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Wagner AD, Schacter DL, Rotte M, et al. Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science. 1998;281:1188–91. doi: 10.1126/science.281.5380.1188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Winston JS, Strange BA, O’Doherty J, Dolan RJ. Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nature Neuroscience. 2002;5:277–83. doi: 10.1038/nn816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Wood JN, Romero SG, Knutson KM, Grafman J. Representation of attitudinal knowledge: role of prefrontal cortex, amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:249–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Yin HH, Knowlton BJ. The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7:464–76. doi: 10.1038/nrn1919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Zink CF, Pagnoni G, Martin-Skurski ME, Chappelow JC, Berns GS. Human striatal responses to monetary reward depend on saliency. Neuron. 2004;42:509–17. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00183-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES