Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Sep 16.
Published in final edited form as: Brain Res. 2008 Jul 11;1230:202–210. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.008

Evaluating frontal and parietal contributions to spatial working memory with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation

Massihullah Hamidi 1, Giulio Tononi 1, Bradley R Postle 1
PMCID: PMC2612637  NIHMSID: NIHMS72273  PMID: 18662678

Abstract

Functional neuroimaging studies have produced contradictory data about the extent to which specific regions of the frontal and the posterior parietal cortices contribute to the retention of information in spatial working memory. We used high frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to assess the necessity for the short-term retention of spatial information of brain areas identified by previous functional imaging studies: dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), frontal eye fields (FEF), superior parietal lobule (SPL) and intraparietal sulcus (IPS). 10 Hz rTMS spanned the 3-sec delay period of a spatial delayed-recognition task. The postcentral gyrus (PCG) was included to control for any regionally nonspecific effects of rTMS. The only regionally specific effect was a significant decrease in reaction time when rTMS was applied to SPL. Additionally, rTMS lowered accuracy to a greater extent when applied to left than to right hemisphere, and was more disruptive when applied contralaterally vs. ipsilaterally to the visual field in which the memory probe was presented. Although seemingly paradoxical, the finding of rTMS-induced improvement in task performance has a precedent, and is consistent with the idea that regions associated with spatial sensory-motor processing make necessary contributions to the short-term retention of this information. Possible factors underlying rTMS-induced behavioral facilitation are considered.

Keywords: Transcranial magnetic stimulation, Working memory, Prefrontal cortex, Parietal cortex, Spatial

1. INTRODUCTION1

Performance of spatial working memory tasks is associated with robust, sustained activity in many regions, including the dlPFC (Leung et al., 2002, Corbetta et al., 2002, D'Esposito and Postle, 1999), FEF (Curtis and D'Esposito, 2006, Postle, 2006a, Corbetta et al., 2002, Postle and Hamidi, 2007), SPL (Curtis, 2006), and IPS (Curtis, 2006, Postle, 2006a, Schluppeck et al., 2006). However, there is controversy about the functions supported by each of these areas, particularly the dlPFC (Leung et al., 2002, Passingham and Rowe, 2002, Lebedev et al., 2004, Postle, 2005, Watanabe, 1996). Some ascribe a storage function to this frontal region (Courtney et al., 1998, Leung et al., 2002), whereas others emphasize roles in such control mechanisms as attentional selection (Passingham and Rowe, 2002, Lebedev et al., 2004), controlling interference (Knight et al., 1999, Postle, 2005), preparing a response (Jiang and Kanwisher, 2003, Schumacher et al., 2003) and anticipating a reward (Watanabe, 1996). To date, neuroimaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have failed to resolve conclusively this controversy, in part because they only provide correlational evidence about brain-behavior relationships (Logothetis and Pfeuffer, 2004, Fellows et al., 2005). Using rTMS to temporarily alter neural processing, however, offers one means of testing hypotheses of necessity (Pascual-Leone et al., 1999, Walsh and Rushworth, 1999).

In this study we applied rTMS during the entire delay-period of a spatial delayed-recognition task to evaluate the necessity of the areas summarized above in the short-term retention (storage) of information. Based on previous studies by our group and others, we assumed that, for critical regions, rTMS would have the effect of inducing a “virtual lesion,” and thus produce decrements in performance (e.g., Postle et al., 2006, Sack and Linden, 2003, Feredoes et al., 2007). Because previous studies have shown that 10 Hz rTMS has functionally-relevant effects (Feredoes et al., 2007, Kennerley et al., 2004)”, we also used a stimulation frequency of 10 Hz. Due to practical and safety considerations limiting how long a rTMS experimental session can last, our study was divided into two experiments with two groups of subjects. Both experiments involved the same experimental procedures, with the exception that different areas were targeted with rTMS. In Experiment 1, the dlPFC and SPL were targeted, with the area representing the leg in the primary somatosensory cortex of the postcentral gyrus (PCG) serving as a control area. In Experiment 2, the FEF and IPS were targeted. Because a different group of subjects participated in Experiment 2, the PCG was once again targeted to serve as a control. Finally, our counterbalanced study design allowed us the opportunity to assess the whether there exist differential effects of rTMS with respect to hemisphere of stimulation or visual field of stimulus presentation.

2. RESULTS

Presented here is a summary of the significant results from the two experiments. Full details of the results (e.g., full reporting of ANOVAs) are available in the supplementary materials. We computed accuracy in terms of mean percentage correct and the signal detection measures of d’ and criterion. Because the results were the same for both indices of performance, signal detection measures, too, are only presented in the supplementary materials.

ANOVAs included 3 levels of the factor Brain Area (frontal region, parietal region, PCG), 2 levels of rTMS (present, absent), 2 levels of hemisphere of stimulation (left, right), and 2 levels of visual hemifield of probe presentation (ipsilateral, contralateral to hemisphere targeted). When significant interactions suggested region-specific effects, we evaluated evidence for these by calculating the rTMS effects for each region (e.g., rTMSabsent – rTMSpresent for dlPFC) and comparing these region-specific effects versus the analogous effect for PCG with t-tests. The same procedure was followed for hemisphere-of-stimulation and visual-hemifield-of-presentation analyses.

Experiment 1

Accuracy

Experiment 1 revealed that rTMS had a general effect of decreasing accuracy (main effect of rTMS [F(1,28) = 8.73; p<0.01]; Fig 1a), more so when applied to the left than the right hemisphere (Hemisphere x rTMS interaction [F(1,28) = 9.84; p<0.01]). There was also a Brain Area x rTMS interaction [F(2,56)=4.34; p<0.05], reflecting the fact that rTMS had its largest effect on dlPFC, a smaller effect on PCG, and a still smaller effect on SPL. However, follow-up tests indicated that the rTMS effect of neither dlPFC nor SPL was significantly different from that of the (control area) PCG [dlPFC: t(29) = −1.52; n.s., SPL: t(29) = 0.87; n.s.]. There was also a Brain Area x Visual Hemifield interaction [F(2,56) = 4.34; p<0.05], and a marginal Visual Hemifield x rTMS x Hemisphere of stimulation interaction [F(1,28) = 3.89; p=0.06]. The latter was driven by the fact that accuracy was decreased for contralateral stimuli only when rTMS was applied to the left hemisphere [t(29) = −5.22; p<0.0001; supplementary Fig. S1].

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Experiment 1: Performance by Brain Area. Accuracy, in proportion correct, (a) and RT (b) in Experiment 1, collapsed over hemisphere and visual hemifield. Error bars indicate standard errors. * - p<0.05.

Reaction Time

With reaction time (RT) there was no main effect of rTMS [F(1,28) = 0.05; n.s.], however, there was a region-specific effect (Brain Area x rTMS interaction [F(2,56) = 4.78; p<0.05]; Fig 1b), with rTMS of the SPL producing faster responses (compared to rTMS effect on PCG: t(29) = −2.30; p<0.05). Within SPL, the pairwise comparison of RT in the rTMSabsent versus the rTMSpresent condition was significant [t(29) = −2.29; p<0.05]. The only relevant effect involving visual hemifield of probe presentation was that, relative to ipsilateral stimuli, response to trials in which stimuli appeared contralaterally was faster with left hemisphere rTMS and slower with right hemisphere rTMS [t(28) = 3.12; p<0.005].

Experiment 2

The results of Experiment 2 revealed that rTMS had an overall effect of decreasing accuracy [t(23) = 2.82; p<0.01] and RT [t(23) = 2.68; p<0.05; Fig 2]. It also had the general effect of lowering accuracy when probe stimuli appeared contralateral to the hemisphere to which rTMS was applied [t(23) = 2.49; p<0.05; Fig 3]. There were neither region-specific (all Fs < 1.41) nor hemisphere-specific (all Fs < 0.29) effects on either accuracy or RT.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Experiment 2: Performance by Brain Area. Accuracy, in proportion correct, (a) and RT (b) in Experiment 2, collapsed over hemisphere and visual hemifield. Error bars indicate standard errors.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Experiment 2: rTMS effect on visual hemifield. Accuracy, collapsed over brain area and hemisphere. Error bars indicate standard errors.

3. DISCUSSION

Effects of rTMS on accuracy

In both experiments, rTMS produced a general decrease in accuracy over all the brain areas we tested, with no difference between experimental and control areas. Although the absence of any region-specific effects was not expected, there were nonetheless selective effects of rTMS on accuracy, with Experiment 1 producing a hemisphere-specific effect and Experiment 2 producing a visual hemifield-specific effect (both of which are discussed further below). Of primary theoretical interest, however, were the effects of rTMS on RT.

Parietal cortex is sensitive to delay-period rTMS – a role in storage?

Our principal goal in this study was to use rTMS to assess whether any specific brain area plays a necessary role in the storage of items in spatial working memory. We will start with the SPL. The role of the parietal lobes in spatial processing is well established, as is evidence for sustained activity in SPL and IPS during spatial working memory tasks (Schluppeck et al., 2006, Postle, 2006a). Furthermore, with damage to the parietal lobe, in addition to the expected neglect syndrome, there is evidence that patients have difficulty with working memory tasks even when stimuli are restricted to the intact visual hemifield (Ferber and Danckert, 2006). In the present study, we found that rTMS of the SPL resulted in a significant decrease in RT. Nonsignificant trends in this direction were also seen with rTMS of FEF and IPS. Abstracted from the specific pattern of the data, the fact that performance on a task requiring the short-term retention of spatial information was sensitive to rTMS of SPL is consistent with a role for this region in the short-term retention of spatial information (Postle and Hamidi, 2007, Postle, 2006b). It is nevertheless the case, however, that our a priori prediction was that delay-period rTMS would produce a decrement in performance. It is noteworthy, in this regard, that another study has also found that delay-period rTMS to SPL, but not PFC, can speed RT in a delayed-recognition task (Luber et al., 2007). This phenomenon also generalized beyond working memory with another study describing improvement in a task of spatial perception with rTMS of SPL (Klimesch et al., 2003). Why do these studies and the current study produce facilitation, whereas other studies employing rTMS to the SPL have produced disruption of behavior (Postle et al., 2006, Feredoes et al., 2006, Terao and Ugawa, 2006)?

Facilitation with rTMS

That the improvement in RT with rTMS of SPL was not accompanied by a corresponding decline in accuracy rules out the possibility that the effect resulted from a simple speed-accuracy tradeoff. There are at least two other possible explanations that might account for the speeding of RT with rTMS. One is that improved performance with rTMS may be the result of a shift in resources available in cognitive processing. If two areas have reciprocally inhibitory influence on each other, goes the reasoning, the disruption of one area can result in hyperactivity of the second area (Théoret et al., 2003). A second possible explanation is that rTMS, when delivered at the right frequency, interacts with a region’s endogenous activity in a manner than improves performance (Klimesch et al., 2003). Amplitude and phase dynamics of oscillations in the α-band (8–13 Hz) of the EEG correlate with performance on a variety of cognitive tasks, including tests of attention and working memory (Palva and Palva, 2007). Because the amplitude of parietal lobe delay-period α-band activity increases monotonically with memory load (Jensen et al., 2002, Busch and Herrmann, 2003), rTMS interactions with oscillations in the α band might underlie the improved performance observed here and in previous studies (Luber et al., 2007, Klimesch et al., 2003). Consistent with this explanation, Klimesch et al. (2003) delivered parietal rTMS at various frequencies and found that performance on a mental rotation task improved only when rTMS was delivered at a frequency slightly above the peak individual resting α frequency (IAF) of each subject. The fact that our stimulation frequency of 10 Hz is approximately equal to the mean IAF in young adults fits with this idea. More directly relevant to the present results, Luber and colleagues (2007) used this logic to apply rTMS at 1, 5, and 20 Hz to the dlPFC and parietal cortex during the delay period of a working memory task. They found that application of rTMS speeded RT only when applied to the parietal cortex at 5 Hz, which they suggested was effective because it is a subharmonic of the average IAF. Of course it is possible that rTMS may lead to facilitation through many other mechanisms, including effects on other frequency bands (Brignani et al., 2007, Fuggetta et al., 2007) or downstream effects in brain areas distal to the point of stimulation (Valero-Cabré et al., 2005, Ferrarelli et al., 2004). Without measurement of the neurophysiological effects of SPL rTMS, it is not possible to determine the mechanism behind the facilitation observed in this study.

Failure to find evidence of a role for dlPFC in storage

Although compared to the rTMSabsent condition we did observe a decrease in accuracy with rTMS applied to the dlPFC, a similar decrease was also present when targeting the PCG control area. Therefore, we cannot rule out the possibility that the effect of dlPFC rTMS on accuracy resulted from regionally non-specific effects of rTMS. That the change in accuracy or RT with application of rTMS did not differ between dlPFC and PCG suggests that the dlPFC may not have a necessary role in the short-term retention of spatial information. Although one must always be cautious when interpreting null results, several factors support this interpretation. First, the sample size of 30 subjects in Experiment 1 does not support a low-sensitivity argument. Second, the present null result for the dlPFC replicates analogous results from previous studies that have targeted the dlPFC and SPL with rTMS (Luber et al., 2007, Postle et al., 2006, Feredoes et al., 2007). One study employed two versions of a verbal working memory task, one requiring subjects to simply retain a list of letters in the order presented, the other requiring them to reorder the list into alphabetical order during the delay period (Postle et al., 2006). In that study, accuracy decreased with rTMS of dlPFC only when subjects were required to manipulate the items in memory. rTMS of SPL, in contrast, impaired performance in both task conditions. Finally, the present null results are also consistent with evidence that patients with frontal lobe lesions show intact performance on tasks requiring the short-term retention of information (D'Esposito and Postle, 1999).

Effects on hemisphere of stimulation

Previous experiments have provided evidence of hemispheric asymmetry during tasks of working memory (D'Esposito et al., 1998, Hester et al., 2007, Muri et al., 2000). The overall pattern of these findings is such that with spatial working memory right hemisphere activity dominates, whereas with verbal and other nonspatial working memory tasks the left hemisphere dominates. Although we did not find evidence of region-specific hemispheric asymmetry in the effects of rTMS, we did find, in Experiment 1, a general effect of rTMS decreasing accuracy to a greater extent when targeting the left versus the right hemisphere. This finding runs counter to the idea that the right hemisphere dominates storage of spatial information.

Relationship with visual hemifield of stimulus presentation

Spatial perception is organized in a cross-hemispheric manner according to visual hemifield. Similarly, there is evidence that spatial working memory is organized in a retinotopic manner (Hagler and Sereno, 2006, Kastner et al., 2007). In the present study we observed a relationship between change in performance with rTMS and the visual hemifield in which the probe stimulus appeared. Experiment 2 produced a lateralized visual-field specific effect on accuracy in that subjects had lower performance on trials in which probe stimuli appeared in the visual hemifield contralateral to the brain hemisphere targeted. It is noteworthy that Experiment 2 targeted the FEF and IPS, both areas involved in oculomotor control. To our knowledge, such an effect of contralateral delay-period rTMS has not been described before, although there is previous evidence of parietal rTMS disrupting perception of targets in the visual hemifield contralateral to the hemisphere of stimulation (Valero-Cabré et al., 2006). There were also visual-field specific effects in Experiment 1, although those results were more complex. Nonetheless, the fact that in both experiments there is a significant difference in the effect of rTMS based only on the part of the visual field in which the probe stimulus appears supports the idea that spatial working memory is organized retinotopically.

Relation to other TMS studies

Previous rTMS studies of spatial working memory have used a variety of TMS and behavioral procedures, which complicates comparison with our findings. For example, many studies have used sham TMS as a control (as opposed to rTMS of a cortical control area) and therefore blocked trials by TMS condition, both differences that may influence the outcome of a study. Furthermore, there have been several studies that have used either a behavioral task (e.g., n-back) or a TMS stimulation procedure (e.g., “preconditioning” designed to suppress cortical activity for tens of minutes) that do not allow for isolation of any single component process of working memory (e.g., encoding vs. storage vs. response selection). Here we focus only on studies that restricted rTMS to the delay period. Two early studies of spatial delayed response described selective impairment of accuracy with TMS delivered to the PFC but not parietal cortex (Brandt et al., 1998, Muri et al., 2000). The seeming disparity with the present results may be attributable to procedural factors. These two studies used a delayed-response procedure, which affords anticipatory motor preparation during the delay period (e.g., Takeda and Funahashi, 2002). The delayed-response procedure also produces relatively greater activation of PFC, whereas the delayed-recognition procedure (which we used in the present study) produces relatively greater activity of the SPL (Curtis et al., 2004). More recently, Koch et al. (2005) found a slowing of RT with rTMS (at 25 Hz for the first 300 ms of a 3.3 s delay period) to either dlPFC or SPL in a task that required memory for the sequential order of spatially defined stimuli. Interpreted from the perspective of our findings, the effect of rTMS to dlPFC reported by Koch et al. may be attributable to the role of this region in the processing of the sequential order of target stimuli (e.g., Knutson et al., 2004).

General Conclusions

We found evidence for SPL playing a necessary role in retention of spatial information in working memory, whereas we did not find such evidence for dlPFC. This is in accordance previous fMRI studies that have found spatial-specific SPL activity, but have not found evidence for domain-specific sustained delay-period activity in dlPFC (reviewed in Postle, 2006b). It is also consistent with previous proposals that spatial working memory behaviors may rely heavily on mechanisms of spatial perception, spatial attention, and motor preparation, which in turn are dependant on dorsal-stream regions posterior to the PFC (e.g., Postle, 2006b).

The nature of the region-specific rTMS effect, an improvement of performance, is inconsistent with a disruption account of rTMS. Although analogous effects have been described before with cognitive tasks, the mechanism behind it is not known. Direct investigation of hypothesized interactions between rTMS and endogenous neuronal oscillations (Luber et al., 2007, Klimesch et al., 2003) may prove to be fruitful in understanding when rTMS will have disruptive versus facilitatory effects on performance.

4. EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES

Subjects

54 young adults (28 male, mean age = 22.7 [S.D. = 4.4]) were recruited from the University of Wisconsin community. 30 (16 male) participated in Experiment 1, 24 (12 male) in Experiment 2. Subjects did not have any psychiatric or neurological conditions, as determined by a psychiatrist or clinical psychologist who administered a structured psychiatric diagnostic interview (MINI, Sheehan et al., 1998) and mood assessment (HAM-D, Hamilton, 1960). All subjects were compensated monetarily.

Behavioral task

In both experiments the delayed-recognition task began with a 500-ms presentation of a central fixation cross, followed by a 1-sec presentation of a target set: four white circles of 1.4° of visual angle, each located at a random location within a different quadrant of the screen. This was followed by a 3-sec delay, after which a probe circle identical to the targets was presented for 1 sec. The probe appeared in each of the four quadrants with p=.25, and at one of the target locations with p=.5. If the probe was non-matching, the distance from the nearest target circle location varied between 3.33 and 4.74° of visual angle. Subjects were instructed to make a yes/no response with a hand-held button box (right thumb/left thumb) as to whether the location of the probe circle matched that of any of the four target circles (Fig. 4). Each trial was followed by a 10-sec intertrial interval (ITI) consisting of a blank, dark screen.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Schematic of the spatial delayed-recognition task in this study. rTMS (10 Hz, 110% motor threshold, corrected) was delivered throughout the entire 3-sec delay period. Task procedures were the same in both Experiment 1 and Experiment 2.

The behavioral task was administered in 4 runs of 12 trials each per target brain area with trials blocked by brain area. The experimental factor of principle theoretical interest was rTMS, which was present or absent for an equal number of trials in each block, and randomized orthogonal to the factor of probe validity. This yielded 48 trials per brain area targeted, 24 with rTMS, 24 without. Of these, in 12 trials probe stimuli were presented ipsilateral to stimulation, 12 contralateral to stimulation.

Anatomical MRI

Whole-brain images were acquired with a 3T scanner (GE Signa VH/I). High resolution T1-weighted images (256 axial slices, 0.5mm × 0.5mm × 0.8mm) were obtained for all participants. This scan was used to reconstruct a 3-dimensional image of each subject’s head, which was used to target rTMS. Because the principal function of the imaging data for this study was to provide a guide for the targeting of rTMS, the analyses were performed at the single-subject level, on the unsmoothed, native-space data. Transforming a subject’s data into a “normalized” atlas space would not be appropriate with this approach for the simple reason that rTMS can only be applied to a subject’s brain in its “native” configuration. For a similar approach, see (Herwig et al., 2003).

For seven subjects in Experiment 1, prior to the TMS session, functional scans were acquired while they performed the delayed-recognition task. For these subjects targets were chosen based on the presence of delay-period fMRI activity during the task. Because the results between fMRI-guided and anatomy-guided rTMS did not differ2, we combined the data here for analysis. fMRI data acquisition and analysis procedures are detailed in the supplementary materials.

rTMS Session

rTMS procedures

For the rTMS session the subject was seated comfortably and his/her head was localized in space via an infrared-based frameless stereotaxy system (eXimia Navigated Brain Stimulation (NBS), Nexstim, Helsinki, Finland). The TMS coil was also fitted with infrared-reflecting beacons, thereby permitting us to accurately target specific regions of the brain.

Prior to the start of the behavioral task, resting motor threshold was determined for each subject as measured by an electromyograph (Bagnoli Handheld EMG System, Delsys, Boston, MA). For the first 12 subjects of Experiment 1, stimulation for all brain regions targeted during the experimental task occurred at 110% of motor threshold. For the remainder of the subjects, we modified the stimulation intensity by starting at 110% of motor threshold, but accounting for scalp-to-cortex distance for each brain area targeted (Stokes et al., 2005). The correction generally resulted in a minor change in stimulation intensity and there were no significant differences in rTMS effect between the two groups3.

In both experiments, during rTMSpresent trials a 3-sec train of 10 Hz rTMS (30 pulses) began with the offset of the target set. Because it is possible that rTMS-induced scalp sensations and muscle contractions can affect performance, prior to the start of the experiment, all subjects experienced 10 Hz stimulation. Care was taken to ensure that subjects were fully comfortable with rTMS at each brain area targeted. TMS was delivered with a Magstim Standard Rapid magnetic stimulator fit with a 70mm figure 8 stimulating coil (Magstim Co., Whitland, U.K.).

rTMS Controls

We used two levels of control in this study. The first level consisted of the half of the trials in each block during which rTMS was not applied. This allowed us to establish an estimate of the “baseline” performance for each subject by the following logic. Although our procedure was intended to target only the delay period, it is possible that the effects of rTMS may last beyond the duration of stimulation. Were this to happen, our estimate of the effect of delay-period rTMS could be contaminated by “spillover” effects on processes other than storage (e.g., encoding, response selection, etc.). Our randomized interleaving of rTMSpresent and rTMSabsent trials controlled for this possibility by ensuring that any long-lasting effects of rTMS would also affect the latter trial types, and this hypothetical “spillover” effect would thereby be removed upon computing the difference between performance on rTMSabsent and rTMSpresent trials4.

The second level of control was the inclusion of a brain area that was not expected to have a direct involvement in working memory-- the area representing the leg in the primary somatosensory cortex of the PCG.

We used this approach rather than such alternatives as sham stimulation or stimulation over the vertex, for several reasons. First, although some sham coils attempt to simulate some of the side effects of TMS (such as the noise produced), the sensations they produce are not indistinguishable from real stimulation (Robertson et al., 2003). Thus, having a control brain area would allow us to account for all the nonspecific superficial effects of rTMS such as scalp sensations and noise. Second, rTMS of a control area will reveal any regionally nonspecific effects on performance that induction of current in the cortex might have. This is not necessarily true for rTMS of the vertex, for which cortical stimulation may be minimal.

Target selection

Five brain areas (dlPFC, FEF, PCG, SPL and IPS) were targeted by rTMS across two experiments. Subjects were randomly assigned to receive rTMS of the left hemisphere or the right hemisphere, with the exception of the 7 subjects for whom rTMS was guided by fMRI (see below).

Because of safety concerns, the study was split into two experiments. In Experiment 1 the dlPFC, SPL, and PCG were targeted. In Experiment 2, the FEF, IPS and PCG were targeted (Fig. 5). Of the 30 subjects participating in Experiment 1, 18 received rTMS to the left hemisphere, 12 of the right. For 7 subjects (5 left hemisphere), rTMS was guided by individual brain activity as measured by fMRI. Areas of activation within dlPFC and SPL were chosen as targets of rTMS. For PCG, an area was chosen that did not include any significantly active voxels. rTMS targeting for the remaining 23 subjects was based on anatomy. The dlPFC target was identified as the middle frontal gyrus on the ventral bank of the superior frontal sulcus at the level of the sulcus frontalis medius (approximately at ±40, +45, +28 atlas coordinates, Talairach and Tournoux, 1988)5, a region corresponding to the border of Brodmann areas 9 and 46 (Petrides, 2000, Oliveri et al., 2001). The PCG target was chosen as an area immediately posterior to the central sulcus, close to the midline. SPL targets were chosen dorsal and medial to the intraparietal sulcus and posterior to the postcentral sulcus (corresponding to Brodmann area 7).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Targets of rTMS. For 23 subjects in Experiment 1 and all the subjects in Experiment 2, targets of rTMS were chosen based on individual brain anatomy, as illustrated on one subject’s MRI scan by the orange markers. The orientation of the marker indicates the orientation of the TMS-related magnetic field at that region. In Experiment 1, the dlPFC and SPL were experimental targets and the PCG was used as a control. In Experiment 2, the FEF and IPS were experimental targets and PCG, once again used as a control area. The pseudocolor painting on the surface of the brain illustrates an estimated projection of the induced electric current produced by a TMS coil held over the dlPFC. The area in red indicates the area of maximal induced electric current.

Of the 24 subjects who participated in Experiment 2, 12 had rTMS applied to the right hemisphere, 12 to the left. All targets were chosen based on anatomy. The FEF stimulation site was chosen as the rostroventral portion of the intersection of the superior frontal and precentral sulci. The PCG target was chosen in the same manner as described above. The IPS target was located at the medial bank of the IPS, at the level of the parieto-occipital fissure.

In both experiments the order of brain area stimulated was counterbalanced across the subjects.

Supplementary Material

01
02
03

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We thank Emilee Castelli, Steve Esser, Eva Feredoes, Andrew Nick, and Eric Steinbrueck for technical and experimental assistance. Supported by NIH MH064498 (B.R.P.) and NARSAD (G.T.).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

1

Abbreviations: repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), frontal eye fields (FEF), superior parietal lobule (SPL), intraparietal sulcus (IPS), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), post-central gyrus (PCG), intertrial interval (ITI), reaction time (RT), individual resting alpha frequency (IAF)

2

The 3-way ANOVA (Brain Area x rTMS x Targeting technique) was not significant for either accuracy [F(2,56) = 0.78; n.s.] or RT [F(2,56) = 1.37; n.s.].

3

The mean change in stimulation intensity was -4.18% of the maximum stimulator output. The 3-way ANOVA (Brain Area x rTMS x Stimulation Intensity Calculation technique) was not significant for either accuracy [F(2,56) = 2.16; n.s.] or RT [F(2,56) = 1.92; n.s.].

4

A possibility that this procedure would not account for would be if the hypothetical “spillover” were differentially time-limited in different brain areas (e.g., spillover may affect encoding in rTMSabsent trials in brain area X, but not brain area Y). To address this comparison of rTMSpresent trials across brain areas was also performed. The results, which were qualitatively similar to those reported here, are presented in the supplementary materials.

5

For 7 subjects, because of excessive facial muscle contractions when targeting the ventral bank of the superior frontal sulcus, we stimulated the dorsal bank. Comparison of subjects with dorsal bank stimulation versus those with ventral bank stimulation revealed no differences in rTMS effect on accuracy [t(28) = 1.35; n.s.] or RT [t(28) = 1.24; n.s.].

REFERENCES

  1. Brandt SA, Pioner CJ, Meyer BU, Leistner S, Villringer A. Effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortex on memory-guided saccades. Experimental Brain Research. 1998;118:197–204. doi: 10.1007/s002210050272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Brignani D, Manganotti P, Rossini PM, Miniussi C. Modulation of cortical oscillatory activity during transcranial magnetic stimulation. Human Brain Mapping. 2007:9999. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20423. NA. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Busch NA, Herrmann CS. Object-load and feature-load modulate EEG in a short-term memory task. Neuroreport. 2003;14:1721–1724. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200309150-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Shulman GL. Neural Systems for Visual Orienting and Their Relationships to Spatial Working Memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14:508–523. doi: 10.1162/089892902317362029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Courtney SM, Petit L, Maisog JM, Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV. An Area Specialized for Spatial Working Memory in Human Frontal Cortex. Science. 1998;279:1347–1351. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5355.1347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Curtis CE. Prefrontal and parietal contributions to spatial working memory. Neuroscience. 2006;139:173–180. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Curtis CE, D'Esposito M. Selection and Maintenance of Saccade Goals in the Human Frontal Eye Fields. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95:3923–3927. doi: 10.1152/jn.01120.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Curtis CE, Rao VY, D'Esposito M. Maintenance of Spatial and Motor Codes during Oculomotor Delayed Response Tasks. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:3944–3952. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5640-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. D'Esposito M, Aguirre GK, Zarahn E, Ballard D, Shin RK, Lease J. Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. Cognitive Brain Research. 1998;7:1–13. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(98)00004-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. D'Esposito M, Postle BR. The dependence of span and delayed-response performance on prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:1303–1315. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00021-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fellows LK, Heberlein AS, Morales DA, Shivde G, Waller S, Wu DH. Method Matters: An Empirical Study of Impact in Cognitive Neuroscience. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005;17:850–858. doi: 10.1162/0898929054021139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ferber S, Danckert J. Lost in space--The fate of memory representations for non-neglected stimuli. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:320–325. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Feredoes E, Tononi G, Postle BR. Direct evidence for a prefrontal contribution to the control of proactive interference in verbal working memory. PNAS. 2006;103:19530–19534. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604509103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Feredoes E, Tononi G, Postle BR. The neural bases of the shortterm storage of verbal information are anatomically variable across individuals. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:11003–11008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1573-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ferrarelli F, Haraldsson HM, Barnhart TE, Roberts AD, Oakes TR, Massimini M, Stone CK, Kalin NH, Tononi G. A [17F]-fluoromethane PET/TMS study of effective connectivity. Brain Research Bulletin. 2004;64:103–113. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2004.04.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fuggetta G, Pavone EF, Fiaschi A, Manganotti P. Acute modulation of cortical oscillatory activities during short trains of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human motor cortex: A combined EEG and TMS study. Human Brain Mapping. 2007:9999. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20371. NA. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hagler JDJ, Sereno MI. Spatial maps in frontal and prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage. 2006;29:567–577. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Herwig U, Abler B, Schonfeldt-Lecuona C, Wunderlich A, Grothe J, Spitzer M, Walter H. Verbal storage in a premotor-parietal network: evidence from fMRI-guided magnetic stimulation. NeuroImage. 2003;20:1032–1041. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00368-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hester R, D'Esposito M, Cole MW, Garavan H. Neural mechanisms for response selection: comparing selection of responses and items from working memory. NeuroImage. 2007;34:446–454. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jensen O, Gelfand J, Kounios J, Lisman JE. Oscillations in the Alpha Band (9–12 Hz) Increase with Memory Load during Retention in a Shortterm Memory Task. Cereb. Cortex. 2002;12:877–882. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.8.877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jiang Y, Kanwisher N. Common Neural Substrates for Response Selection across Modalities and Mapping Paradigms. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2003;15:1080–1094. doi: 10.1162/089892903322598067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kastner S, Desimone K, Konen CS, Szczepanski SM, Weiner KS, Schneider KA. Topographic Maps in Human Frontal Cortex Revealed in Memory-Guided Saccade and Spatial Working-Memory Tasks. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;97:3494–3507. doi: 10.1152/jn.00010.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kennerley SW, Sakai K, Rushworth MFS. Organization of Action Sequences and the Role of the Pre-SMA. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:978–993. doi: 10.1152/jn.00651.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Gerloff C. Enhancing cognitive performance with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at human individual alpha frequency. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;17:1129–1133. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02517.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Knight RT, Staines WR, Swick D, Chao LL. Prefrontal cortex regulates inhibition and excitation in distributed neural networks. Acta Psychologica. 1999;101:159–178. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(99)00004-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Knutson KM, Wood JN, Grafman J. Brain activation in processing temporal sequence: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2004;23:1299–1307. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Koch G, Oliveri M, Torriero S, Carlesimo GA, Turriziani P, Caltagirone C. rTMS evidence of different delay and decision processes in a fronto-parietal neuronal network activated during spatial working memory. NeuroImage. 2005;24:34–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lebedev MA, Messinger A, Kralik JD, Wise SP. Representation of Attended Versus Remembered Locations in Prefrontal Cortex. PLoS Biology. 2004;2:e365. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Leung HC, Gore JC, Goldman-Rakic PS. Sustained Mnemonic Response in the Human Middle Frontal Gyrus during On-Line Storage of Spatial Memoranda. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14:659–671. doi: 10.1162/08989290260045882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Logothetis NK, Pfeuffer J. On the nature of the BOLD fMRI contrast mechanism. Magnetic Resonance Imaging. 2004;22:1517–1531. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2004.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Luber B, Kinnunen LH, Rakitin BC, Ellsasser R, Stern Y, Lisanby SH. Facilitation of performance in a working memory task with rTMS stimulation of the precuneus: Frequency- and time-dependent effects. Brain Research. 2007;1128:120–129. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Muri RM, Gaymard B, Rivaud S, Vermersch A-I, Hess CW, Pierrot-Deseilligny C. Hemispheric asymmetry in cortical control of memory-guided saccades. A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:1105–1111. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00030-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Oliveri M, Turriziani P, Carlesimo GA, Koch G, Tomaiuolo F, Panella M, Caltagirone C. Parieto-frontal Interactions in Visual-object and Visual-spatial Working Memory: Evidence from Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation. Cerebral Cortex. 2001;11:606–618. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.7.606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Palva S, Palva JM. New vistas for α-frequency band oscillations. Trends in Neurosciences. 2007:30. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Pascual-Leone A, Bartres-Faz D, Keenan JP. Transcranial magnetic stimulation: studying the brain--behaviour relationship by induction of 'virtual lesions'. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1999;354:1229–1238. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1999.0476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Passingham D, Rowe JB. Dorsal prefrontal cortex: maintenance in memory or attentional selection? In: Stuss DT, Knight RT, editors. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  38. Petrides M. Frontal Lobes and Memory. In: Boller F, Grafman J, editors. Handbook of Neuropsychology. Second ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  39. Postle BR. Delay-period Activity in the Prefrontal Cortex: One Function Is Sensory Gating. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2005;17:1679–1690. doi: 10.1162/089892905774589208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Postle BR. Distraction-resistant sustained activity during delayed recognition of locations. NeuroImage. 2006a;30:950–962. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Postle BR. Working Memory as an Emergent Propertry of the Mind and Brain. Neuroscience. 2006b;139:23–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Postle BR, Ferrarelli F, Hamidi M, Feredoes E, Massimini M, Peterson M, Alexander A, Tononi G. Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Dissociates Working Memory Manipulation from Retention Functions in the Prefrontal, but not Posterior Parietal, Cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006;18:1712–1722. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Postle BR, Hamidi M. Nonvisual Codes and Nonvisual Brain Areas Support Visual Working Memory. Cereb. Cortex. 2007;17:2151–2162. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Robertson EM, Theoret H, Pascual-Leone A. Studies in Cognition: The Problems Solved and Created by Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2003;15:948–960. doi: 10.1162/089892903770007344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Sack AT, Linden DEJ. Combining transcranial magnetic stimulation and functional imaging in cognitive brain research: possibilities and limitations. Brain Research Reviews. 2003;43:41–56. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(03)00191-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Schluppeck D, Curtis CE, Glimcher PW, Heeger DJ. Sustained Activity in Topographic Areas of Human Posterior Parietal Cortex during Memory-Guided Saccades. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:5098–5108. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5330-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Schumacher EH, Elston PA, D'Esposito M. Neural Evidence for Representation Specific Response Selection. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2003;15:1111–1121. doi: 10.1162/089892903322598085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1998;59:22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Stokes MG, Chambers CD, Gould IC, Henderson TR, Janko NE, Allen NB, Mattingley JB. A simple metric for scaling motor threshold based on scalp-cortex distance: Application to studies using transcranial magnetic stimulation. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;94:4520–4527. doi: 10.1152/jn.00067.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Takeda K, Funahashi S. Prefrontal Task-Related Activity Representing Visual Cue Location or Saccade Direction in Spatial Working Memory Tasks. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:567–588. doi: 10.1152/jn.00249.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. Stuttgart, Germany: Thieme; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  52. Terao Y, Ugawa Y. Studying higher cerebral functions by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Supplements to Clinical Neurophysiology. 2006;59:9–17. doi: 10.1016/s1567-424x(09)70006-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Théoret H, Kobayashi M, Valero-Cabré A, Pascual-Leone A. Exploring paradoxical functional facilitation with TMS. Supplements to Clinical neurophysiology. 2003;56:211–219. doi: 10.1016/s1567-424x(09)70224-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Valero-Cabré A, Payne BR, Rushmore J, Lomber SG, Pascual-Leone A. Impact of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the parietal cortex on metabolic brain activity: a 14C-2DG tracing study in the cat. Experimental Brain Research. 2005;163:1–12. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2140-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Valero-Cabré A, Rushmore RJ, Payne BR. Low frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation on the posterior parietal cortex induces visuotopically specific neglect-like syndrome. Experimental Brain Research. 2006;172:14–21. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0307-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Walsh V, Rushworth M. A primer of magnetic stimulation as a tool for neuropsychology. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:125–135. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Watanabe M. Reward expectancy in primate prefrental neurons. Nature. 1996;382:629–632. doi: 10.1038/382629a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

01
02
03

RESOURCES