Skip to main content
Environmental Health logoLink to Environmental Health
. 2008 Nov 4;7:54. doi: 10.1186/1476-069X-7-54

Exposure to mobile telecommunication networks assessed using personal dosimetry and well-being in children and adolescents: the German MobilEe-study

Silke Thomas 1, Anja Kühnlein 1, Sabine Heinrich 1, Georg Praml 1, Rüdiger von Kries 2, Katja Radon 1,
PMCID: PMC2614418  PMID: 18983641

Abstract

Background

Despite the increase of mobile phone use in the last decade and the growing concern whether mobile telecommunication networks adversely affect health and well-being, only few studies have been published that focussed on children and adolescents. Especially children and adolescents are important in the discussion of adverse health effects because of their possibly higher vulnerability to radio frequency electromagnetic fields.

Methods

We investigated a possible association between exposure to mobile telecommunication networks and well-being in children and adolescents using personal dosimetry. A population-based sample of 1.498 children and 1.524 adolescents was assembled for the study (response 52%). Participants were randomly selected from the population registries of four Bavarian (South of Germany) cities and towns with different population sizes. During a Computer Assisted Personal Interview data on participants' well-being, socio-demographic characteristics and potential confounder were collected. Acute symptoms were assessed three times during the study day (morning, noon, evening).

Using a dosimeter (ESM-140 Maschek Electronics), we obtained an exposure profile over 24 hours for three mobile phone frequency ranges (measurement interval 1 second, limit of determination 0.05 V/m) for each of the participants. Exposure levels over waking hours were summed up and expressed as mean percentage of the ICNIRP (International Commission on Non-Ionizing Radiation Protection) reference level.

Results

In comparison to non-participants, parents and adolescents with a higher level of education who possessed a mobile phone and were interested in the topic of possible adverse health effects caused by mobile telecommunication network frequencies were more willing to participate in the study. The median exposure to radio frequency electromagnetic fields of children and adolescents was 0.18% and 0.19% of the ICNIRP reference level respectively.

Conclusion

In comparison to previous studies this is one of the first to assess the individual level of exposure to mobile telecommunication networks using personal dosimetry, enabling objective assessment of exposure from all sources and longer measurement periods. In total, personal dosimetry was proofed to be a well accepted tool to study exposure to mobile phone frequencies in epidemiologic studies including health effects on children and adolescents.

Background

The use of mobile communication devices has increased within the last years. At the same time, there is growing public concern that radio frequency electromagnetic fields could cause adverse health effects even at exposure levels far below the reference levels [1]. In a recent study in Germany, 27% of the participants reported concerns about such effects [2].

Mainly people living near to a mobile phone base station are concerned about potential harmful effects of their radiation [3]. People relating their health problems to base stations or mobile phones often report having unspecific symptoms like headache, restlessness, sleep disturbances, concentration and memory problems, absence of appetite as well as tinnitus [4-6].

In the discussion of possible adverse health effects caused by mobile phones children and adolescents are relatively important, because they are possibly more sensitive to radio frequency electromagnetic fields. Among others, a possible higher vulnerability of children and adolescents is mainly discussed, because of a greater susceptibility of their developing nervous system. Furthermore today's children and adolescents have a higher cumulative exposure during their lifetime as today's adults, because they start their use earlier in life [7-9]. In addition, recent studies indicated higher SAR values for children in comparison to adults [10-12].

Up to now there are only few epidemiologic studies published that investigated a possible association between exposure to mobile telecommunication networks and health outcomes in children and adolescents. The results of two Finnish studies and one Swedish study that investigated the use of mobile phones and the association with health symptoms in adolescents showed that frequent mobile phone use was associated with poor perceived health [13-15]. One main drawback of these studies was that the exposure assessment had to rely on self-reports of the participants.

The same problem underlines most other epidemiologic studies which focus on adults [16,17]. Most of such studies using self-reported exposure and outcome found some association which might be due to a differential misclassification of exposure. This bias is also called "awareness bias" [18].

Hutter and colleagues have attempted to address this problem in their pilot study by measuring exposure to base stations in the participants' bedrooms [3]. However, the issue could not be fully resolved. Electromagnetic emissions of a mobile phone base station vary over time and indoor field strength strongly depends on the station's position, making it difficult to capture the exposure accurately [19]. In addition, bedtime exposure reflects only one part of the overall exposure. Using the same method of exposure assessment, Berg et al. examined in a population based cross-sectional study also possible health effects caused by exposure to mobile phone base stations on adults. The results showed that participants who were concerned about or attributed adverse health effects on mobile phone base stations reported more often health complaints than the remaining participants. No association between measured exposure and health was found [20].

Until recently, there is only one epidemiological study which used personal dosimetry to assess the individual exposure. The aim of the study – also performed by our workgroup-was to investigate a possible association between exposure to mobile telecommunication networks and well-being in adults. The results showed an exposure markedly below the ICNIRP reference levels (highest exposure in Munich: 0.41% of the ICNIRP reference level). No association between exposure and symptoms was found [21].

Objectives

The objective of this study was to investigate a potential association between exposure to mobile telecommunication networks and well-being in children and adolescents and to assess the level of exposure in a general-population sample of children and adolescents living in Bavaria (Southern Germany).

The current paper presents the methods, descriptive data and the results of a non-response analysis. Separate papers, based on the full MobilEe-study, will address the possible association between exposure to mobile phone frequencies and (1) acute symptoms, (2) chronic symptoms and (3) mental health behaviour.

Methods

Study population

The MobilEe-study is a population based cross-sectional study taking place in four Bavarian cities with different population size: Munich (~1.300.000 inhabitants), Augsburg (~260.000 inhabitants), Rosenheim (~60.000 inhabitants) and Landsberg (~28.000 inhabitants) [22]. The participants were randomly selected from the registration offices of these four towns. The following selection criteria were used:

▪ Children between 8–12 years old, adolescents between 13–17 years.

▪ German nationality in order to diminish language difficulties.

▪ If more than one sibling from one household was selected they were excluded to minimise cluster effects.

Study design and procedure

The field-phase of the study started in February 2006 and was completed in December 2007. Overall, 1.498 children and 1.524 adolescents were included. Written informed consent was obtained of the participants' parents and – if they were older than 14 years – of the adolescents themselves.

Each family of a potential participant got a letter consisting of information about the study, an informed consent form and a short questionnaire. All subjects invited were asked to answer the short questionnaire, irrespective of their participation in the field study. This information was used to assess potential selection bias. In case of the children the parents answered the questionnaire, in case of the adolescents they answered themselves. Non-responders got up to two postal reminders and were contacted by phone up to five times. Missing data on the short questionnaire were assessed in a phone interview.

Those who declared consent were invited to a local study centre, where they completed a Computer Assisted Personal Interview (CAPI) with questions on chronic symptoms and potential confounding variables. The duration of the interview was about 30 minutes. In case of the children one parent was also invited to complete an interview. Besides the same questions as their children the parents were also asked about their environmental worries and socioeconomic characteristics (e.g. income, education level). For adolescents this information was obtained directly from the teenagers.

After the interview the ESM-140 dosimeter was handed out to the children and adolescents for a 24 hours measurement. During this measurement, the participants were asked to fill in a diary recording acute symptoms at noon and in the evening before bedtime as well as the frequency of mobile phone calls and DECT phone (Digital Enhanced Cordless Phone) calls in the previous hours. As an incentive, participants obtained a 20-Euro purchase voucher (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Study procedure of the MobilEe-study.

After an interview training, all field workers were retrained again during the field phase. Quality controls were done on a weekly base. In addition, some participants were selected at random after the field study and asked by the coordinators whether they felt comfortable.

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty of the Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich (285/03).

Questionnaire instruments

The following described questionnaire instruments, that were used in the study, are shown in table 1.

Table 1.

Questionnaire instruments

Outcome Measuring instrument
Acute symptoms Typical symptoms mentioned in the context of exposure to mobile phone frequencies, answers following the German "Zerssen complaint list" [23]
Chronic symptoms Following the questions of the HBSC-study (Health Behaviour in School-aged Children) [24]
Mental health German version of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) [25-27]
Potential confounding variables
Sociodemographic data Questionnaire of the German Health Interview and Examination Survey for Children and Adolescents [28]
Environmental worry Short form of the environmental worry scale [29,30]
Exposure
Objective exposure Measurement using personal dosimetry
Subjective exposure Questionnaire of the annual survey of the infas from 2003 [31]

Acute symptoms

The following acute symptoms were assessed three times during the 24 hours measurement using a paper-based diary: headache, irritation, nervousness, dizziness, fatigue and concentration problems.

The diary items were taken from the "Zerssen complaint list" [23] and assessed on a four point Likert scale (heavy, moderate, barely, not at all). The symptoms were considered present if they were reported with an at least barely intensity.

Chronic symptoms

Chronic symptoms during the last six months were assessed during the CAPI. The symptoms were taken from the questionnaire of the HBSC-survey (Health behaviour in School-Aged Children) and consisted of the following ones: headache, irritation, nervousness, dizziness, fatigue, fear and sleeping problems [24].

The list was assessed on a five point Likert scale (nearly daily, several times a week, nearly every week, about once a month, seldom or never) during the CAPI. All symptoms were considered present if they occurred at least nearly every week according to self-report.

Mental health

The 25 questions concerning mental health behaviours were asked during the CAPI following the German version of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). The questionnaire contained 5 scales: emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems and prosocial behaviour. Each scale comprised 5 items with three possible forms ("certainly true", "somewhat true", "not true"). The summary score for each scale could range from 0–10 points. The difficulties score was generated by the summation of all four scales except the prosocial scale. The overall score could range from 0–40 points. For each scale as well as for the total difficulties score the participants were classified as "normal", "borderline" or "abnormal". [25-27]

Due to the small numbers of children and adolescents that could be classified as "borderline", we used two groups for the analysis ("normal" and "borderline/abnormal") and those participants who were in the borderline group were classified as "abnormal".

Potential confounder

Sociodemographic data, the four study towns and environmental worries taken from the CAPI were considered as potential confounder.

The sociodemographic data included age, sex and level of education as confounder [28]. All variables were dichotomised:

▪ Age group:

8–10 and 11–12 years (children) respectively 13–15 and 16–17 years (adolescents)

▪ Level of education:

at least 12 years of education (parents of the children) respectively at least grammar school ("Gymnasium") (adolescents)

The four study towns Munich, Augsburg, Rosenheim and Landsberg were also considered as potential confounding variables.

Environmental worries

Environmental worries were assessed during the CAPI for the parents of the children and for the adolescents themselves with a short form of the environmental worry scale [29,30]. This scale consists of 12 questions about general and specific environmental worries (e.g. noise exposure or general environmental pollution) and was assessed on a four-point Likert scale (fully agree, partly agree, partly disagree, fully disagree). We a priori decided to dichotomise the variable using the median as cut off. This was done because there is no standard strategy to evaluate the environmental worry scale.

Self estimated exposure to mobile telecommunication networks (subjective exposure)

Besides the objective assessment of exposure using personal dosimetry participants were asked to estimate their exposure to mobile telecommunication networks. Participants estimated their exposure during the measurement day using a diary ("How many minutes did you use a mobile phone or DECT phone within the last 8 hours?"). In the baseline interview participants were also asked about the distance to the next mobile phone base stations in the living environment ("How many meters is the next mobile phone base station away from your home?"). The questions were taken from the questionnaire of the annual survey of the Institute for applied sciences (infas) and provided different reply categories [31].

These exposure estimates have been used to compare our results to previous studies on possible associations between self-reported exposure to mobile phone frequencies and health [4,16,17,32,33].

Exposure assessment using personal dosimetry

Exposure was measured using the personal dosimeter ESM-140 (Maschek Electronics). For 24 hours the dosimeter was placed on the upper arm of the participants opposite to the side which they usually used to hold the mobile phone or DECT phone during phone calls. During the measurement, subjects were asked to perform their routine daily tasks. At night, the participants placed the dosimeter next to their beds. After handing back the dosimeter the local assistants immediately read out the profiles and checked them for completeness and plausibility.

Exposure was assessed every second resulting in 86.400 measurements over 24 hours. The following frequency ranges were covered [17]:

▪ GSM 900 (up and down link [up link: mobile phone; down link: mobile phone base station]; frequency range: 890–960 MHz)

▪ GSM 1800 (up and down link; frequency range: 1710–1880 MHz) including UMTS 2100 (frequency range: 1920–2170 MHz) and DECT (frequency range: 1880–1900 MHz)

▪ WLAN 2400 frequencies (frequency range: 2400–2480 MHz)

For the exposure assessment, a combination of the frequency bands had to be used because the dosimeter has a low selectivity between the up- and down-link channels.

Four dosimeters were selected at random and tested at the Technical Inspection Agency South (TÜV Süd) to confirm the technical data given by the manufacturer and to investigate if the different dosimeters used in the study are comparable. The findings showed a good comparability of the different dosimeters. At a given exposure level of 0.5 V/m the relative standard deviation was between 4% (DECT) and 17% (GSM 1800). Furthermore the findings proved the validity of the technical data provided by the manufacturer. According to the results of these laboratory measurements, the limit of determination was set at 0.05 V/m. To classify the exposure all measured values that were below this limit were replaced previous to the analysis by half of the limit (0.025 V/m). This method is often used in context of environmental epidemiology and the results seem plausible, because all values have to be between 0 and the limit of determination [34].

As a summary of the measured exposure we calculated in a first step the squared field strength, averaged over the waking hours, for the three frequency ranges. This way e.g. the squared field strength Ē2 for GSM 900 has been calculated via the following formula:

E¯GSM 9002=((Σwaking hoursEGSM 900 uplink2(t))/duration of waking hours)+=((Σwaking hoursEGSM 900 downlink2(t))/duration of waking hours)

The overall simultaneous exposure to multiple frequency fields was calculated by summation on the squared field strengths, averaged over waking hours, which are weighted by the inverse of squared ICNIRP reference level [35]. Extracting the square-root over this sum returned an overall exposure in terms of field strengths percentage of the reference level [21]:

Overall exposure% = square-root ((Ē2GSM 900/limit2900 MHz) + (Ē2GSM 1800/limit21800 MHz) + (Ē2WLAN/limit22400 MHz)) * 100

Level of exposure during bedtime

During night the participants were asked to fix the dosimeter next to their bed on a bottle filled with water as a replacement for the participants' arm. The question came up if the measured values at this fixed position could represent the real exposure.

Therefore, the Technical Inspection Agency South (TÜV Süd) also investigated if the stationary measured bedtime exposure was a valid proxy of night time exposure. It was confirmed in the lab measurements that the measurement results of the dosimeter measurements depend on the direction in the field. So valid measurements of the average exposure can only be obtained if the dosimeter is moved. Due to these results bedtime exposure levels were not considered to be a valid proxy of night time exposure. Thus these levels had to be excluded and only exposure levels during individual waking hours were used.

Five day measurements

To verify the representativeness of the 24 hours measurements 54 participants carried the dosimeter for five consecutive days from Monday afternoon to Saturday afternoon.

For the analysis the time between 4.p.m of one day until 4 p.m. of the following day (night time values were excluded) was summed up and exposure during waking hours was used. Exposure was classified into quartiles for each study day. The overall agreement of these quartiles between the weekdays was assessed. As a random misclassification of exposure on neighbouring cells always results in an underestimation of the effect [36], the deviation by at maximum 1 category was also considered.

Statistical analysis

In the main analysis the potential association between the measured exposure to mobile telecommunication networks and acute and chronic well-being was analysed.

The objective exposure was divided into quartiles. For these analyses, the mean percentage of the ICNIRP reference level during waking hours was used to analyze the association between exposure to mobile phone frequencies and chronic symptoms. The mean percentage of the ICNIRP reference level during morning hours (time when diary was completed in the morning – time when diary was completed at noon) was considered relevant for the relationship between morning exposure and acute symptoms at noon. Finally, the mean percentage of the ICNIRP reference level during afternoon hours (time when diary was completed at noon – time when diary was completed in the evening) was calculated to test the potential association between afternoon exposure and acute symptoms before bedtime.

In a secondary analysis, self-reported data on distance of the home to the next mobile phone base station as well as the data on usage of mobile phones and DECT phones on the study day were used as exposure proxy. Self estimated distance of the home of the participants to the next base station was used as exposure proxy to analyze the association with chronic symptoms. For acute symptoms, diary data on usage of mobile phones and DECT phones during the study day were used as exposure proxy.

In a sensitivity analysis we also evaluated exposure separately for the three frequency bands GSM 900, GSM 1800 (including UMTS and DECT) and WLAN and thus included three variables in the model. In a second sensitivity analysis exposure was considered a binary cut-off (90% percentile).

Chi2-tests were used to assess bivariate associations. Multivariate analyses were done using logistic regression models adjusted for age, sex, level of education, environmental worry, frequency of mobile phone use, frequency of DECT phone use, estimated distance to the next mobile phone base station and study place. These potential confounding variables were defined a priori and included in all analyses. Statistical analyses were carried out using SAS (SAS version 9.1; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA).

Results

Participation

Overall 6.386 children and adolescents were invited, of which 5.870 were eligible for the study. 4.452 persons (76%) answered the short questionnaire and 3.022 children and adolescents (52%) participated in the study. Adolescents (78%) answered the questionnaire more frequent than parents (74%), however parents participated in the study a little more frequent (53%) than adolescents (51%). The readiness to take part in the study was lowest in Augsburg (46%) and highest in Landsberg (59%).

Non-response analysis

To analyse a possible bias caused by selective non-participation, we compared the data of the short questionnaire of those who participated in the field study to those who did not. Parents of participating children were more likely mobile phone owners (93% of the participants, 90% of the non-participants; pChi2 0.02). Parents who had a higher level of education (at least 12 years of education) and those who were concerned about mobile phone exposure were more likely to take part in the study. Furthermore participating parents more often knew the distance to the next base station (don't know: 3%) than those who did not want to participate (don't know: 22%).

Participating adolescents were also more likely to have a higher level of education (at least grammar school) and to be more frequent mobile phone owners (91%) than non-participants (87%). Regarding the self estimated distance from home to the next mobile phone base station, non-participating adolescents (17%) were more likely to choose the "don't know" category than those who participated (3%). Furthermore participating adolescents were also more concerned about mobile phone exposure (at least fairly concerned: 12%) than non-participants (at least fairly concerned: 8%) (Table 2).

Table 2.

Comparison of parents and adolescents who participated in the field study to non- participants (data from the short questionnaire)

Variable Participants Non-participants
Prevalence n (%) Prevalence n (%) p-value
Parents of the children n = 1477 n = 628
Sex 0.29
  male 359 (24.3) 139 (22.2)
Level of education
  at least 12 years of education 748 (51.0) 250 (40.4) <0.0001
Nationality (mother) <0.0001
  German 902 (85.6) 347 (72.0)
Mobile phone ownership 0.02
1365 (92.9) 562 (89.8)
Use of a mobile phone per day 0.16
  < 5 minutes 1057 (72.4) 475 (76.4)
  > 5 minutes 404 (27.7) 147 (23.7)
Self-reported concerns about adverse health effects to EMF <0.0001
  not or little concerned 580 (55.4) 350 (72.5)
  fairly concerned 386 (36.9) 103 (22.0)
  deeply concerned 80 (7.7) 27 (5.6)
Self-estimated distance to the next base station <0.0001
  Don't know 39 (2.7) 136 (22.0)
  < 500 meters 945 (56.3) 303 (49.0)
  = 500 meters 464 (32.0) 179 (29.0)
Adolescents n = 1508 n = 814
Sex 0.009
  male 730 (48.4) 440 (54.1)
Level of education
  grammar school 743 (49.9) 253 (31.2) <0.0001
Nationality (mother) <0.0001
  German 946 (85.1) 465 (74.9)
Mobile phone ownership 0.004
1373 (91.2) 711 (87.4)
Use of a mobile phone per day 0.25
  < 5 minutes 1153 (77.1) 593 (73.6)
  > 5 minutes 343 (22.9) 213 (26.5)
Self-reported concerns about adverse health effects to EMF <0.0001
  not or little concerned 969 (87.7) 572 (92.1)
  fairly concerned 120 (10.9) 38 (6.1)
  deeply concerned 17 (1.5) 11 (1.8)
Self-estimated distance to the next base station <0.0001
  don't know 47 (3.2) 139 (17.3)
  < 500 meters 922 (62.6) 390 (48.5)
  = 500 meters 503 (34.2) 274 (34.1)

EMF: electromagnetic fields

Exposure

Objective exposure

21 measurements hat to be excluded from the analysis due to technical errors. All exposure levels were far below the ICNIRP reference level and ranged from a mean of 0.13% (all measurement values below the limit of determination) to a mean of 0.92% of the ICNIRP reference level per second during waking hours. Median exposure was slightly higher for adolescents (0.19) than for children (0.18). The majority of measured values were below the limit of determination (82% of the measured values during waking hour).

Exposure was higher during afternoon hours than during morning hours. This applied for the children (median morning hours: 0.17, median afternoon hours: 0.19) as well for the adolescents (median morning hours: 0.18; median afternoon hours: 0.20) (table 3).

Table 3.

Data of measured exposure to mobile telecommunication networks for the children and adolescents

% ICNIRP-reference level Children n = 1484 Adolescents n = 1508
Exposure during waking hours
Range 0.13–0.92 0.13–0.78
Quartiles 0.15 0.15
0.17 0.17
0.20 0.21
Mean (standard deviation) 0.18 (0.06) 0.19 (0.06)
Exposure during morning
Range 0.13–0.80 0.13–0.74
Quartiles 0.14 0.14
0.15 0.16
0.19 0.20
Mean (standard deviation) 0.17 (0.06) 0.18 (0.07)
Exposure during afternoon
Range 0.13–1.20 0.13–0.87
Quartiles 0.15 0.15
0.17 0.17
0.20 0.22
Mean (standard deviation) 0.19 (0.08) 0.20 (0.07)
Exposure during waking hours (binary cut-off)
Range
  low exposure 0.13–0.25 0.25–0.92
  high exposure 0.13–0.26 0.26–0.78

Exposure varied by the size of the town of residence. Median exposure levels during waking hours were highest in Munich and lowest in the smallest town (Landsberg) (figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Comparison of median exposure (% ICNIRP) to mobile telecommunication networks during waking hours by study town.

Five day measurements

Between 20% and 57% of the participants were in exactly the same exposure quartile on two days of the week (perfect agreement). Highest complete agreement was always seen between two consecutive days. However, the weekend (Friday to Saturday) differed considerably from the weekdays. For weekdays, exposure categories differed by at most one exposure quartile for more than 80% of the population indicating that misclassification of exposure might result in an underestimation of the effect (Table 4).

Table 4.

Agreement of the exposure quartiles on different days of the week (in%)

Weekdays Tuesday Wednesday Thursday Friday
Monday 57 (1)/85 (2) 46/83 41/85 30/74
Tuesday 37/81 44/83 20/67
Wednesday 50/81 35/72
Thursday 31/61

(1) Perfect agreement of the exposure quartiles/(2) Deviation in at most one exposure quartile

Subjective exposure

Sixty-seven percent of the parents of the children and 65% of the adolescents estimated that the next mobile phone base station was less then 500 meters away from their home. Regarding the self estimated use of mobile phones during the field study, nearly every child (98%) and 90% of the adolescents stated a very low usage (< 5 minutes) in the morning hours. For the afternoon hours also 98% of the children and 86% of the adolescents reported less than 5 minutes of usage. The participating children reported longer usage of a DECT phone in the afternoon (< 5 minutes: 85%) than in the morning (< 5 minutes: 91%). The same was stated for the adolescents (afternoon: <5 minutes: 63%; morning: < 5 minutes: 86%). In general adolescents had a higher DECT phone use than children. As expected, highest use was seen in the afternoon.

Discussion

The MobilEe-study is the first of its kind to investigate a possible association between exposure to mobile telecommunication networks and well-being in children and adolescents using personal dosimetry. In consideration of the participants' time expenditure the response of 52% was satisfying and underlines the interest in the topic of a possible association between mobile phone exposure and health.

Exposure levels were on average less than 1% of the ICNIRP reference level which is an agreement with other studies [20,21,37]. Furthermore exposure levels were highest in the largest city Munich, which is also in agreement with other studies [20,21,38].

Strengths and limitations

The use of personal dosimetry to assess individual exposure to mobile telecommunication network frequencies enables accounting for all sources of exposure, including own and neighbour's use of mobile phones, mobile phone base stations, DECT phones and WLAN in the home environment. One major advantage of personal dosimetry is that it enables longer measurement periods and the estimation of the personal exposure not only at the place of living but also at work and during leisure activities [39,40]. Disadvantages of personal dosimetry are its low validity when the participant stays at one place for a longer time or the fact, that the body of the participant can influence the measured values [39,41,42]. Overall, personal dosimetry is considered a better measure of exposure than stationary measurements or estimation (self-reported or calculated) alone [19,40,41,43].

A drawback of the dosimeter used in this study is the limited selectivity to differentiate between the three frequency ranges. For this reason we calculated the cumulative exposure over all frequency ranges. This way we obtained a mean value which is comparable to the ICNIRP reference level. Another drawback are the measured values during night. Measuring the night-time exposure levels is a common difficulty of studies involving personal dosimetry. Our study participants placed the dosimeters near their beds, which resulted in a constant, but arbitrary measurement during the night. As shown in the lab measurements, the dosimeter measurements depend on the direction in the field. Therefore, valid measurements can only be obtained if the dosimeter is moved. It also has to be kept in mind that inside a room variations in exposure are possible if the room is e.g. close to a mobile phone base station. The position of the fixed dosimeter can lead to different measurement levels, depending on whether the dosimeter is in a wave crest or in a wave trough.

Selection bias

Forty-five percent of the participating parents were at least fairly concerned about possible health effects of exposure to mobile phone frequencies as compared with 27% reported in a recent questionnaire study in the German general population. The proportion of concerned persons in the group of the participating adolescents was also higher (24%) than in the previous questionnaire study (14%). [44] It appears that primarily those parents and adolescents took part in the study who were concerned about a possible association between mobile phone exposure and health. We cannot rule out a preferential selection in our study of concerned subjects. Due to the objective exposure measurement a differential misclassification seems to be unlikely and therefore an overestimation of the results is also unlikely. Furthermore, several studies have shown that the Bavarian residents tend to be more concerned about mobile phone exposure and health than people living in the North or the East of Germany [20,44].

There are still missing information about non-responder as we could not reach all of these subjects for the non-response-analysis. As primarily those persons participated who were concerned about possible health effects caused by exposure to mobile telecommunication networks a selection bias is possible. Furthermore concerned participants could overestimate subjective exposure and symptoms. Due to the objective exposure assessment a differential misclassification seems to be unlikely.

Confounding

Regarding the potential confounding variables in the association between mobile phone exposure and health it has to be kept in mind that these have to be associated with the outcome variables and exposure. While own mobile phone use might be influenced by e.g. socioeconomic status or environmental worries, it is unlikely that exposure to mobile phone base stations is largely affected. As shown in this paper adolescents and especially children's own mobile phone use was low during the study day and therefore does most likely not substantially change the overall exposure. Based on that fact, confounding is not considered a major issue in this study.

Misclassification

Due to the fact that the measurement was limited to 24 hours, only the status quo was assessed. Yet it is possible that the individual exposure levels during the study day may not be representative. Therefore some participants carried the dosimeter for five consecutive days from Monday afternoon to Saturday afternoon. The results showed that the assessment of exposure on a single weekday reflects the typically weekday exposure quite good. However, weekend exposure differs considerably which is plausible as children and adolescents spend most parts of the weekdays in school while at weekends they might spend more time at home or at different places. Due to the fact that the change of the participants was mostly in one exposure quartile it could be that a non-differential misclassification occurred, which leads to an underestimation of the risk especially for the chronic symptoms.

In case of the subjective exposure awareness bias is possible. It could be that the participants overestimated their usage of mobile phones and DECT phones during the study day and that the self estimated distance to the next mobile phone base station was incorrect [39]. Since mainly those parents and their children respectively those adolescents participated who were concerned about possible adverse health effects to electromagnetic fields an overestimation of subjective exposure seems to be likely. Thus a differential misclassification and overestimation of the results is possible which might result in specious findings.

The examined outcomes were also assessed once. Therefore a misclassification concerning chronic symptoms is also possible. It might be that the participants overestimated their symptoms during the measurement day, because they were more aware of them. As the participants were not aware of their actual objective exposure level differential misclassification is unlikely [45-47].

Statistical methods

Due to the results of the laboratory measurements we decided to replace all values that were below the limit of determination (0.05 V/m) by half of this limit instead of taking the real measured values. One has to keep in mind that this could lead to more conservative results. In a pilot study we used different methods for the handling of values below the limit of determination like e.g. multiple imputation for the children's data. No differences in the calculated Odds Ratios and the Confidence Intervals were seen, therefore our way of substitution of the values below the limit of determination is not considered to bias the results [48].

We a priori decided to analyse the association between measured objective exposure and well-being using exposure levels as quartiles. This enabled us to detect possible dose-response relationships. We could not take exposure as a continuous variable, because too many values were below the limit of determination.

In a sensitivity analysis we divided exposure at 90% percentile to compare those 10% of the participants who had the highest exposure levels to the remaining participants. One reason to use also a binary cut-off has been that the range of exposures within the highest quartile seemed to be larger than differences between quartiles. In another sensitivity analysis we also evaluated exposure separately for the three frequency bands. Due to the different models in the analysis, multiple comparisons have to be taken into account.

Conclusion

In summary, this study is the first to use personal dosimetry to assess the individual exposure to mobile telecommunication networks in children and adolescents, enabling objective assessment of exposure from all sources. Participation in the field study was fairly good and dosimeters were well accepted. We found an exposure to mobile phone frequencies far below the current ICNIRP reference levels.

Abbreviations

CAPI: Computer Assisted Personal Interview; DECT: Digital Enhanced Cordless Phone; EMF: electromagnetic fields; GSM: Global System for Mobile Communications; ICNIRP: International Commission on Non-Ionizing Radiation Protection; UMTS: Universal Mobile Telecommunication System; WLAN: Wireless Local Area Network

Competing interests

The authors declare they have no competing financial interests.

Authors' contributions

ST was the one of the principle investigators responsible for design, conduct and writing the manuscript. AK was also one of the principle investigators and responsible for data analysis and interpretation of the data. SH was also one of the principle investigators responsible for design and acquisition of data. GP coordinated the technical parts of the study. RK made contributions to draft the manuscript. KR made contributions to conception and design and also to analysis and drafting the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgements

We thank all participants for their participation as well as the study team for the field work. The participating towns are acknowledged for their support and the provision of rooms for the interviews. Parts of this study have been used for the PhD thesis of Sabine Heinrich, Anja Kühnlein and Silke Thomas. Funded by the Federal Office for Radiation Protection (Germany).

Contributor Information

Silke Thomas, Email: silke.thomas@med.lmu.de.

Anja Kühnlein, Email: anja.kuehnlein@med.lmu.de.

Sabine Heinrich, Email: sabine.heinrich@med.lmu.de.

Georg Praml, Email: georg.praml@med.lmu.de.

Rüdiger von Kries, Email: Ruediger.Kries@med.uni-muenchen.de.

Katja Radon, Email: katja.radon@med.lmu.de.

References

  1. Hutter HP, Moshammer H, Wallner P, Kundi M. Public perception of risk concerning celltowers and mobile phones. Soz Praventivmed. 2004;49:62–66. doi: 10.1007/s00038-003-2094-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Institute for Applied Social Sciences (infas) Final Report of the survey 2006. Bonn, Germany; 2006. Identifying the general public's fears and anxieties with regard to the possible risks of high frequency electromagnetic fields of mobile telecommunications (annual survey) [Google Scholar]
  3. Hutter H-P, Moshammer H, Wallner P, Kundi M. Subjective symptoms, sleeping problems, and cognitive performance in subjects living near mobile phone base stations. Occupational and Environmental Medicine. 2006;63:307–313. doi: 10.1136/oem.2005.020784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Hocking B. Preliminary report: symptoms associated with mobile phone use. Occupational Medicine. 1998;48:357–360. doi: 10.1093/occmed/48.6.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Oftedal G, Straume A, Johnsson A, Stovner L. Mobile phone headache: a double blind, sham-controlled provocation study. Cephalalgia. 2007;27:447–455. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2007.01336.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Röösli M, Moser M, Baldinini Y, Meier M, Braun-Fahrländer C. Symptoms of ill health ascribed to electromagnetic field exposure–a questionnaire survey. International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health. 2004;207:141–150. doi: 10.1078/1438-4639-00269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Kheifets L, Repacholi M, Saunders R, van Deventer E. The sensitivity of children to electromagnetic fields. Pediatrics. 2005;116:e303–313. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-2541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Independent Expert Group on Mobile Phones (IEGMP) Mobile Phones and Health (The Stewart Report) 2000. http://www.iegmp.org.uk/report/text.htm
  9. Leitgeb N. Mobile phones: are children at higher risk? Wiener Medizinische Wochenschrift. 2008;158:36–41. doi: 10.1007/s10354-007-0447-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Wiart J, Hadjem A, Gadi N, Bloch I, Wong M, Pradier A, Lautru D, Hanna V, Dale C. Modeling of RF Head Exposure in Children. Bioelectromagnetics. 2005;7:19–30. doi: 10.1002/bem.20155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Wiart J, Hadjem A, Wong M, Bloch I. Analysis of RF exposure in the head tissues of children and adults. Physics in Medicine and Biology. 2008;53:3681–3695. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/53/13/019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. de Salles A, Bulla G, Rodriguez C. Electromagnetic absorption in the head of adults and children due to mobile phone operation close to the head. Electromagn Biol Med. 2006;25:349–360. doi: 10.1080/15368370601054894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Punamäki RL, Wallenius M, Nygard CH, Saarni L, Rimpela A. Use of information and communication technology (ICT) and perceived health in adolescence: the role of sleeping habits and waking-time tiredness. Journal of Adolescence. 2007;30:569–585. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Koivusilta L, Lintonen T, Rimpela A. Intensity of mobile phone use and health compromising behaviours – how is information and communication technology connected to health-related lifestyle in adolescents? Journal of Adolescence. 2005;28:35–47. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2004.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Söderquist F, Carlberg M, Hardell L. Use of wireless telephones and self-reported health symptoms: a population based study among Swedish adolescents aged 15–19 years. Environ Health 2008 May 21;7:18. 2008;7:18. doi: 10.1186/1476-069X-7-18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Navarro E, Segura J, Portoles M, Gomez-Peretta C. The Microwave Syndrome: A Preliminary Study in Spain. Electromagnetic Biology and Medicine. 2003;22:161–169. doi: 10.1081/JBC-120024625. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Santini R, Santini P, Danze JM, Le Ruz P, Seigne M. Investigation on the health of people living near mobile telephone relay stations: I/Incidence according to distance and sex. Pathol Biol (Paris) 2002;50:369–373. doi: 10.1016/s0369-8114(02)00311-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Moffatt S, Mulloli T, Bhopal R, Foy C, Phillimore P. An exploration of awareness bias in two environmental epidemiology studies. Epidemiology. 2000;11:199–208. doi: 10.1097/00001648-200003000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bornkessel C, Schubert M, Wuschek M, Schmidt P. Determination of the General Public Exposure around GSM and UMTS Base Stations. Radiation Protection Dosimetry. 2007;124:40–47. doi: 10.1093/rpd/ncm373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Berg G, Breckenkamp J, Kowall B, Riedel J, Blettner M, Schüz J, Schmiedel S, Schlehofer B, Wahrendorf J, Potthof P, Schroeder E. Cross-sectional study to record and evaluate possible adverse health effects due to electromagnetic fields from cell-phone base stations (QUEBEB) Federal Ministry for the Environment, Nature Conservation and Nuclear Safety, Germany.
  21. Thomas S, Kühnlein A, Heinrich S, Praml G, von Kries R, Nowak D, Radon K. Personal Exposure to Mobile Phone Frequencies and Well-Being in Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study Based on Dosimetry. Bioelectromagnetics. 2008;29:463–470. doi: 10.1002/bem.20414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Bavarian State Office for Statistics and DataProcessing. http://www.statistik.bayern.de/daten/intermaptiv/archiv/home.asp?RS = &TH = &UT = bevoelkerung.csv&SP = 1
  23. Zerssen D. Complaint List. Manual. Weinheim: Beltz; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  24. Haugland S, Wold B. Subjective health complaints in adolescence–reliability and validity of survey methods. Journal of Adolescence. 2001;24:611–624. doi: 10.1006/jado.2000.0393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Goodman R, Meltzer H, Nailey V. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A pilot study on the validity of the self-reported version. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1998;7:125–130. doi: 10.1007/s007870050057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Klasen H, Woerner W, Rothenberger A, Goodman R. German version of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ-German)–overview and evaluation of initial validation and normative results. Praxis Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie. 2003;52:491–502. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Robert Koch-Institute KiGGS – The German Health Interview and Examination Survey for Children and Adolescents. 2007. http://www.kiggs.de/experten/erste_ergebnisse/Basispublikation/index.html [DOI] [PubMed]
  29. Rethage T, Eikmann T, Herr C. Arbeitsmedizinische Aspekte der Metallbearbeitung – Gesundheitsschutz bei Einsatz neuer Technologien. Innsbruck: Arbeitsmedizinisches Zentrum Hall in Tirol, Austria; 2004. Einführung in die mehrdimensionale Erfassung von Umweltbesorgnis. [Google Scholar]
  30. Hodapp V, Neuhann HF, Reinschmidt U. Evaluation eines Fragebogens zur Erfassung von Umweltbesorgnis. Zeitschrift für Gesundheitspsychologie. 1996;4:22–36. [Google Scholar]
  31. Institute for Applied Social Sciences (infas) Identifying the general public's fears and anxieties with regard to the possible risks of high frequency electromagnetic fields of mobile telecommunications (annual survey). Final Report of the survey. Bonn, Germany; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  32. Chia S, Chia H, Tan S. Prevalence of headache among handheld cellular telephone users in Singapore: a community study. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2000;108:1059–1062. doi: 10.2307/3434959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Sandström M, Wilen J, Oftedal G, Hansson Mild K. Mobile phone use and subjective symptoms. Comparison of symptoms experienced by users of analogue and digital mobile phones. Occupational Medicine. 2001;51:25–35. doi: 10.1093/occmed/51.1.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hornung RW, Reed LD. Estimation of average concentration in the presence of nondetectable values. Applied Occupational Environmental Hygiene. 1990;5:48–51. [Google Scholar]
  35. International Commission on Non-Ionizing Radiation Protection (ICNIRP) Guidelines for limiting exposure to time-varying electric, magnetic, and electromagnetic fields (up to 300 GHz) Health Physics 74. 1998;4:494–522. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dosemeci M, Wacholder S, Lublin J. Does Nondifferential Misclassification of Exposure always bias a true effect towards the null value? American Journal of Epidemiology. 1990;132:746–748. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Joseph W, Vermeeren G, Verloock L, Masache Heredia M, Martens L. Characterization of Personal RF Electromagnetic Field Exposure and Actual Absorption for the General Public. Health Phys. 2008;95:317–330. doi: 10.1097/01.HP.0000318880.16023.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Bergquist U. Exposure Assessment in Epidemiology and Practice. Exposure and radiofrequency fields and mobile telephony. Arbeite Och Hälsa. 2001;10:499–507. [Google Scholar]
  39. Radon K, Spegel H, Meyer N, Klein J, Brix J, Wiedenhofer A, Eder H, Praml G, Schulze A, Ehrenstein V, von Kries R, Nowak D. Personal Dosimetry of Exposure to Mobile Telephone Base Stations? An Epidemiologic Feasibility Study Comparing the Maschek Dosimeter Prototype and the Antenessa DSP-090 System. Bioelectromagnetics. 2006;27:77–81. doi: 10.1002/bem.20175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Neubauer G, Feychting M, Hamnerius Y, Kheifets L, Kuster N, Ruiz I, Schuz J, Uberbacher R, Wiart J, Roosli M. Feasibility of future epidemiological studies on possible health effects of mobile phone base stations. Bioelectromagnetics. 2007;28:224–230. doi: 10.1002/bem.20298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Blas J, Lago FA, Fernandez P, Lorenzo RM, Abril EJ. Potential exposure assessment errors associated with body-worn RF dosimeters. Bioelectromagnetics. 2007;28:573–576. doi: 10.1002/bem.20355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Knafl U, Lehmann H, Riederer M. Electromagnetic Fields Measurements Using Personal Exposimeters. Bioelectromagnetics. 2007;29:160–162. doi: 10.1002/bem.20373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Schüz J, Mann S. A discussion of potential exposure metrics for use in epidemiological studies on human exposure to radiowaves from mobile phone base stations. Journal of Exposure Analysis and Environmental Epidemiology. 2000;10:600–605. doi: 10.1038/sj.jea.7500115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Institute for Applied Social Sciences (infas) Identifying the general public's fears and anxieties with regard to the possible risks of high frequency electromagnetic fields of mobile telecommunications (annual survey). Final Report of the survey from 2003 to 2006. Bonn, Germany; [Google Scholar]
  45. Ahlborg G. Validity of exposure data obtained by questionnaire. Two examples from occupational reproductive studies. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1990;16:284–288. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.1783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Vrijheid M, Deltour I, Krewski D, Sanchez M, Cardis E. The effects of recall errors and selection bias in epidemiologic studies of mobile phone use and cancer risk. Journal of Exposure Science and Environmental Epidemiology. 2006;16:371–384. doi: 10.1038/sj.jes.7500509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kaye W, Hall H, Lybarger J. Recall bias in diseases status associated with perceived exposure to hazardous waste. Ann Epidemiol. 1994;4:393–397. doi: 10.1016/1047-2797(94)90074-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Seidl C. Behandlung des Problems von Bestimmungs- und Nachweisgrenzen in epidemiologischen Studien. Ludwigs-Maximilians-University: Munich, Germany; 2007. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Environmental Health are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES