Skip to main content
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior logoLink to Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
. 2009 Jan;91(1):21–39. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2009.91-21

The Stay/Switch Model of Concurrent Choice

James S MacDonall 1,
PMCID: PMC2614816  PMID: 19230510

Abstract

This experiment compared descriptions of concurrent choice by the stay/switch model, which says choice is a function of the reinforcers obtained for staying at and for switching from each alternative, and the generalized matching law, which says choice is a function of the total reinforcers obtained at each alternative. For the stay/switch model two schedules operate when at each alternative. One arranges reinforcers for staying there and the other arranges reinforcers for switching from there. Rats were exposed to eight or nine conditions that differed in the arrangement of the values of the stay and switch schedules. The generalized matching law described preferences when arrangements were similar to those found when using two concurrently running interval schedules. It did not, however, describe all preferences when using different arrangements. The stay/switch model described all preferences in one analysis. In addition, comparisons of selected conditions indicated that changing the ratio of obtained reinforcers was neither necessary nor sufficient for changing preference as measured by response ratios. Taken together these results provide support for the stay/switch model as a viable alternative to the generalized matching law and that the critical independent variable is allocation of stay and switch reinforcers.

Keywords: concurrent schedule, generalized matching law, momentary maximizing, optimal foraging theory, stay/switch model, lever press, rats


Concurrent variable-interval (VI) VI schedules are used to analyze recurrent choice among two alternatives. In the standard model, subjects choose to respond at one alternative or the other (Herrnstein, 1961). Hence, the standard analysis of the resulting performance compares responding at the two alternatives using the generalized matching law. It says the ratio of the rates of responding at each alternative is a power function of the ratio of the rates of reinforcers obtained at each alternative (Baum, 1974). The rates of responding (and reinforcers) at each alternative are based on session duration, thus, each response (and reinforcer) rate is based on the same time. Consequently, time cancels in the both ratios. Thus, the generalized matching law may be expressed in logarithmic form as,

graphic file with name jeab-91-01-02-e01.jpg 1

where Bn is the responses or time at alternative n, and Rn is the reinforcers obtained at alternative n. The two fitted parameters are s, which is the sensitivity to the ratio of reinforcers, and log b, which is the preference or bias towards one alternative that is not captured by the reinforcer ratio. Implicitly, the generalized matching law views each choice as starting from a neutral position and proceeding to a response on one of two alternatives (Sugrue, Corrado, & Newsome, 2004.

A different approach to understanding two-alternative concurrent choice starts by viewing choice not from a neutral position but from the alternative where responding is occurring. The choice is staying at the present alternative or switching from the present alternative. Thus, according to this view—the stay/switch model—concurrent choice consists of two independent sets of choices, one at each alternative (MacDonall, 2005; MacDonall, Goodell, & Juliano, 2006). Switching alternatives presents the other set of choices. The critical variables influencing choice are the events that occur during visits at each alternative: the reinforcers for staying and responding during visits and the reinforcers for ending visits by switching from that alternative. These reinforcers-per-visit control the behavior during visits, that is, the run lengths (the mean responses during visits) and the visit durations (the mean duration of visits).

According to the stay/switch model, run lengths and visit durations are functions of the ratio of the stay to switch reinforcers per visit (MacDonall, 1999, 2000, 2003) and the sum of the stay and switch reinforcers per visit (MacDonall, 2006). The reinforcers per visit for staying are the number of reinforcers obtained while pressing one lever, divided by the number of visits (i.e., changeovers from that lever). The reinforcers per visit for switching from a lever is the number of reinforcers obtained for switching by pressing the other lever divided by the number of visits (i.e., changeovers). Applying this model to concurrent choice, the equation with subscripts for one alternative is divided by the equation with subscripts for the other alternative. Formally the equation is (MacDonall et al., 2006):

graphic file with name jeab-91-01-02-e02.jpg 2

Bstn is the number of stay responses at Alternative n or the time spent staying at Alternative n. Bswn is the number of switches from Alternative n; 2Bswn equals, plus or minus 1, the total changeovers from both levers. Rstn is the number of reinforcers obtained for staying at Alternative n, and Rswn is the number of reinforcers obtained for switching from Alternative n. Note the subscripts refer to the alternative changed from and the alternative where reinforces were arranged. The three fitted parameters are k, which is the bias towards an alternative that is not a function of the distribution of stay and switch reinforcers, l, which is the sensitivity to the ratio of the ratio of the stay and switch reinforcers and m, which is the sensitivity to the ratio of the sum of the stay and switch reinforcers (MacDonall et al., 2006). Because the number of switches from each alternative must be within 1 of each other, and provided the number of switches is large (≫ 100) they cancel. Expressing the resulting equation in logarithmic form produces:

graphic file with name jeab-91-01-02-e03.jpg 3

which plots as a flat surface and can be fitted to the data using least-squares linear regression. This equation says that preference is a joint function of the ratio of the ratios of the reinforcers for staying and for switching (henceforth, the ratio of ratios) and the ratio of the sums of the reinforcers for staying and for switching (henceforth, the ratio of sums). Because response preference is measured using only stay responses, it is called the stay-response ratio.

Support for Equation 3 was provided by varying the ratio of the ratios as the ratio of the sums remained approximately constant (MacDonall, 2005). Rats were exposed to concurrent VI VI schedules composed of two pairs of stay and switch schedules. In one set of conditions the pair of schedules was arranged symmetrically, that is, the value of the stay schedule in each pair equaled the value of the switch schedule in the other pair. For example, a VI 36 s and a VI 320 s operated only while the subject was at Alternative 1. The VI 36 s arranged reinforcers for staying and responding at Alternative 1 while the VI 320 s arranged reinforcers for switching from Alternative 1. A VI 320 s and a VI 36 s operated only while the subject was at Alternative 2. The VI 320 s arranged reinforcers for staying and responding at Alternative 2 while the VI 36 s arranged reinforcers for switching from Alternative 2. Changing alternatives exchanged the pair of schedules operating. This procedure was equivalent to a standard concurrent VI 36 s VI 320 s schedule. Rats were exposed to four or five conditions that used symmetrical pairs of schedules and the resulting preferences were described by the generalized matching law (r2 > .93).

In a second set of conditions, the influence of the ratio of the sums on preference was evident (MacDonall, 2005). In these conditions the values of the pairs of schedules were arranged asymmetrically, that is, the value of the stay schedule in each pair equaled the value of the switch schedule in the same pair. An asymmetrical arrangement based on the schedules in the above example produces a VI 36 s for staying at Alternative 1 and a VI 36 s for switching from Alternative 1 and a VI 320 s for staying at Alternative 2 and a VI 320 s for switching from Alternative 2. With an asymmetrical arrangement the sum of the scheduled reinforcers arranged per visit is greater at one alternative, in this case, Alternative 1; however, at both alternatives the ratio of the scheduled stay and switch reinforcers arranged per visit equals 1.0. The actual ratio of the ratios in each condition equaled approximately one and there were clear preferences, indicating that the ratio of the sums influenced preference. The response preferences maintained by the symmetrical conditions combined with the response preferences maintained by the asymmetrical conditions were not described by the generalized matching law (MacDonall, 2005, reproduced here in Table 6). A reanalysis of those data showed that Equation 3 described the combined data (.88 < r2 < .96). Although these data support the stay/switch model, these two arrangements sampled a limited area in the two-dimensional space defined by the ratio of ratios and the ratio of sums.

Table 6.

The results of linear regression using the generalized matching law (Equation 5) and response data from all conditions of MacDonall (2005). Also shown is the result of the AIC analysis.

Rat s SE log b SE r2 df AIC
407 0.36 0.18 −0.03 0.08 .34 8 −16
408 0.52 0.18 0.01 0.09 .50 8 −18
409 0.66 0.10 0.02 0.04 .87 7 −32
410 0.45 0.09 0.03 0.04 .77 8 −34
411 0.19 0.11 −0.01 0.05 .28 7 −24
412 0.44 0.09 0.04 0.04 .79 7 −30

The primary purpose of the following experiment was to further assess the stay/switch model by exposing rats to other combinations of the ratio of ratios and ratio of sums. If the resulting data were described by the Equation 3 then the stay/switch model was further assessed by determining whether the behavior at each alternative was described by the stay/switch model. If the stay/switch model was supported by these data, a secondary purpose of this experiment was to assess the generalized matching law by seeing whether changing the ratio of obtained reinforcers was necessary or sufficient for changing preference when the magnitude, immediacy and quality of reinforcers remained unchanged.

In order to show that changing the ratio of obtained reinforcers is not necessary for changing preference, the ratio of obtained reinforcers must remain unchanged as preference changes. The ratio of obtained reinforcers was kept constant by using two pairs of schedules in which each switch schedule was programmed to produce 10% more reinforcers than its paired stay schedule. Previous research (MacDonall, 2005) showed that when the value of the stay and switch schedules were the same, about 10% more reinforcers were obtained for staying. This occurred because rats obtained stay reinforcers almost as soon as they were arranged, but obtained switch reinforcers after a delay when they switched alternatives, reducing the number obtained.

In order to show that changing the ratio of obtained reinforcers is not sufficient for changing preference, the ratio of the obtained reinforcers should change but preference should remain unchanged. One way to produce a constant preference is suggested by Equation 3; select values of random-interval (RI) schedules so that the ratio of the sums favor one alternative and the ratio of the ratios favor the other alternative. With appropriate schedule values these tendencies to favor opposite alternatives would then counteract each other, yielding no preference. The values of the schedules that will produce indifference cannot be determined a priori because the preferences produced by the each of these two ratios would be, in part, a function of the sensitivity to these ratios. Consequently, initial values were used and then modified depending on the resulting response preferences.

The symmetrical conditions were included to show that concurrent schedules implemented by using two pairs of stay and switch schedules produce data that are described by the generalized matching law. The values of the schedules in the asymmetrical conditions were selected to provide additional data for evaluating Equation 3. Generally, schedule values for the asymmetrical conditions were selected to try to produce to unique combinations of the ratio of ratios and the ratio of sums.

Method

Subjects

The subjects were 6 male naïve Sprague-Dawley rats (Hilltop Lab Animals, Scottdale, PA), and were approximately 120 days old when the experiment began. They were deprived to 85% of their just determined free-feeding weights and maintained at this weight throughout the experiment. They were housed individually in a temperature controlled colony room with a 14-hr light, 10-hr dark cycle beginning at 5 AM. Water was freely available in their home cages.

Apparatus

Six operant conditioning chambers were each located in a light- and sound-controlled enclosure. Four chambers were approximately 225 mm wide and 195 mm high; three of the chambers were 235 mm in length, whereas the fourth was 350 mm in length. A 50-mm square opening to the food cup was centered horizontally on one 225-mm × 195-mm wall, 20 mm above the floor. Two response levers (Model G6312, R. Gerbrands Co.), 45 mm long × 13 mm thick, protruded 15 mm into the chamber. The centers of the levers were 60 mm to the left or right from the center of the food cup and 50 mm above the floor. Each lever required a minimum force of approximately 0.3 N to operate. The other two chambers were 305 mm wide, 270 mm high and 245 mm long. On one 305-mm × 270-mm wall were three response levers (Model G6312, R. Gerbrands Co.) 90 mm above the floor. One lever was centered on the wall and the other two were 90 mm to the left or right of the center of the center lever. Only the two outside levers were used in this experiment. A 24-VDC stimulus light was centered approximately 75 mm above each lever. Centered horizontally on the opposite wall, 30 mm above the floor, was a 50-mm square opening to the food cup. The food cup was located on the opposite wall so that the food cup was approximately equidistant to each of the three levers. In each of the six chambers, a pellet dispenser, located behind the wall containing the food cup, dispensed 45-mg food pellets (Formula A/1, P. J. Noyes) which were 85% Purina rodent chow. Each chamber was illuminated during sessions by a pair of houselights mounted on the top center of the chamber. White noise was presented through a speaker centered between the houselights. An IBM-compatible computer and MED-PC® software and hardware (MED Associates Inc.) controlled the experimental events and recorded responses.

Procedure

Rats were placed in the chambers and every response on either lever was reinforced. After about 50 reinforced responses, RI 10-s schedules were in effect for all responses. Over several sessions the values of the RI schedules were increased to the values in the first condition. Rats were not explicitly magazine trained nor was lever pressing explicitly shaped. Rats usually pressed a lever within 30 min of the first session and usually pressed that lever exclusively. As the value of the RI schedules increased pressing of the other lever emerged.

Rats were trained on the same two-lever concurrent procedure (Herrnstein, 1961) throughout the experiment. At the beginning of each session no schedules operated. The first press at either lever started the associated pair of stay and switch schedules. Subsequently, the first press at the left lever started the stay and switch schedules associated with the left lever and stopped the stay and switch schedules associated with the right lever (unless this was the first response in the session). Similarly, the first press at the right lever started the stay and switch schedules associated with the right lever and stopped the schedules associated with the left lever (again, unless this was the first response in the session). When a stay or switch schedule arranged a reinforcer, that schedule stopped and resumed operating after the reinforcer was delivered. The first response at an alternative could only deliver a reinforcer if one was arranged by the schedule for switching from the other alternative. Subsequent responses at that alternative could only deliver a reinforcer if one was arranged by the schedule for staying at that alternative. If the rat switched alternatives, by pressing the other lever, before an arranged stay reinforcer was delivered, that stay reinforcer was held until the rat switched back to that alternative by responding at the associated lever, and the next response at that lever delivered that stay reinforcer. There was no change-over delay (COD).

Conditions differed according to the arrangement of stay and switch schedules at the alternatives. In the symmetrical conditions the value of the stay schedule that operated while at an alternative equaled the value of the switch schedule that operated while at the other alternative (see Table 1).

Table 1.

For each rat, the sequence of symmetrical (SY), necessary (N), sufficient (SU) and unsymmetrical (U) conditions, the values of the RI schedules in each condition and the number of session in each condition are shown. Also shown are the sums over the last five sessions of each condition of the left and right stay responses, the reinforcers obtained from the stay and switch schedule associated with each alternative, the time at each alternative and the changeovers from each alternative.

Condition Sessions RI schedule
Stay Responses
Stay at Left Switch from Left Stay at Right Switch from Right Left (Bst1) Right (Bst2)
Rat 758
SY 16 294 33 33 294 1410 22112
SY 19 33 294 294 33 16303 2098
N* 15 179 161 36 32 2160 14730
N 15 36 32 179 161 9164 3667
U* 26 50 10 83 417 4655 2051
SU 16 63 417 63 10 4868 5018
SU 15 63 10 50 417 4030 5905
SY 16 38 152 152 38 8850 4255
Rat 759
SY 28 294 33 33 294 598 21624
SY 16 33 294 294 33 25130 1661
N 20 179 161 36 32 1421 12504
N 16 36 32 179 161 19541 1619
SU* 15 83 8 28 500 4146 4538
U 17 28 500 83 8 11142 1916
SU 22 28 500 28 8 4017 5230
SY 17 38 152 152 38 11326 6859
Rat 760
SY 20 294 33 33 294 783 24665
SY 27 33 294 294 33 23201 2043
N 16 179 161 36 32 2664 18959
N 22 36 32 179 161 15293 3003
U* 23 63 8 29 500 4070 8351
U 16 42 500 63 8 9704 5236
SU 17 50 8 40 500 5668 6590
SU* 15 63 500 42 8 5979 7898
Rat 761
SY 16 294 33 33 294 258 17518
SY 25 33 294 294 33 19570 1648
SY 16 294 33 33 294 1707 23156
N 18 179 161 36 32 3986 16397
SU* 15 100 9 40 333 5874 6795
U 17 40 333 100 9 13013 2486
N 16 36 32 179 161 18280 2587
SU* 15 67 333 40 10 3197 5782
Rat 762
SY 42 294 33 33 294 1408 5523
SY 17 33 294 294 33 5921 1494
N 17 179 161 36 32 2010 8259
N 23 36 32 179 161 5592 2282
SU 19 83 8 40 333 2517 3009
U 15 40 333 83 8 2922 2769
SY 20 294 33 33 294 719 13064
SU 15 67 333 67 10 4220 1000
U 20 500 79 100 9 1866 4043
Rat 763
SY 27 294 33 33 294 2069 6183
SY 19 33 294 294 33 6878 2453
N 20 179 161 36 32 2119 14460
N 15 36 32 179 161 3495 9743
SU* 15 45 8 71 333 6708 3397
U 15 71 333 45 8 3970 4459
SY 18 294 33 33 294 4569 4415
U 26 25 23 250 227 2119 14460
SU 20 125 125 56 8 5316 2096
*

Several sessions that used other values of the RI schedules preceded this condition. See text for explanation.

Condition Reinforcers obtained for
Time at (sec)
Switches from
Stay at left (Rst1) Switch from left (Rsw1) Stay at Right (Rst2) Switch from right (Rsw2) Left (Bst1) Right (Bst2) Left (Bsw1) Right (Bsw2)
Rat 758
SY 15 132 432 46 4816 15379 1617 1618
SY 441 47 12 126 15590 3470 1613 1610
N* 34 45 276 272 7212 10619 1941 1940
N 283 273 29 41 11228 6378 2611 2609
U* 103 451 60 11 5264 6221 2195 2198
SU 123 23 77 404 9263 5359 2435 2432
SU 73 401 138 13 5133 7748 2464 2466
SY 343 107 40 136 15588 6456 2711 2707
Rat 759
SY 8 106 452 60 3136 15968 1404 1405
SY 488 44 8 87 16872 2541 902 901
N 21 21 330 263 3360 11755 717 717
N 314 268 20 27 11008 4475 1211 1209
SU* 48 343 233 5 3918 6269 939 941
U 326 18 33 254 8831 2635 931 929
SU 155 16 132 323 4563 4197 703 699
SY 323 90 48 166 12542 6708 1693 1693
Rat 760
SY 12 90 403 45 3146 14635 2148 2146
SY 441 53 11 93 15445 3136 1906 1903
N 35 32 228 270 6700 8586 2995 2992
N 275 287 28 35 10493 5718 2045 2043
U* 42 352 222 10 3323 7036 1820 1822
U 180 20 60 365 8014 3761 2096 2093
SU 65 426 127 7 4328 5982 2270 2270
SU* 113 8 91 413 7749 4474 2621 2618
Rat 761
SY 8 91 477 50 3318 16620 1668 1669
SY 430 62 12 119 14821 3976 1557 1555
SY 6 86 478 52 3488 17602 1634 1632
N 48 24 269 283 7511 11562 2310 2309
SU* 31 390 185 19 4098 7912 1839 1842
U 186 18 48 374 9545 4056 1868 1866
N 272 284 37 34 9582 6264 2086 2085
SU* 54 13 124 435 4857 5681 2184 2181
Rat 762
SY 19 212 351 45 7286 13454 1607 1610
SY 457 55 13 101 17170 3843 1002 999
N 35 59 278 255 7585 11540 1115 1117
N 269 270 42 46 9775 6687 874 871
SU 49 376 169 32 4361 8420 1021 1022
U 223 32 42 332 9288 3973 835 830
SY 16 118 450 44 4271 18455 831 835
SU 88 24 90 424 7189 6969 919 919
U 14 84 65 462 7034 6502 1101 1102
Rat 763
SY 20 128 423 56 5064 22077 660 661
SY 443 57 9 118 21337 3323 959 957
N 54 65 268 240 9464 11135 1484 1485
N 245 283 42 58 11565 8907 1115 1116
SU* 89 377 131 29 5234 12449 1450 1453
U 135 34 74 341 17582 4914 1646 1647
SY 7 112 410 63 4732 24892 1396 1400
U 304 278 19 26 10848 5991 1096 1096
SU 88 107 67 364 15403 5645 1496 1494
*

Several sessions that used other values of the RI schedules preceded this condition. See text for explanation.

In the necessary conditions (conditions designed to determine if a change in the ratio of obtained reinforcers is necessary to produce a change in the response ratios), the ratio of sums favored one alternative and the ratio of ratios equaled 1.0. This was accomplished by using, at one alternative, RI 179 s as the stay schedule and RI 161 s as the switch schedule, and at the other alternative, RI 36 s as the stay schedule and RI 32 s as the switch schedule.

In the sufficient conditions (conditions designed to determine if a change in the ratio of obtained reinforcers is sufficient to produce a change in the response ratios), the ratio of sums favored one alternative and the ratio of ratios favored the other alternative. This was accomplished by using similar values of the RI schedules for staying at each alternative. The value of the switch schedule at one alternative was much longer than its associated stay schedule and the value of the switch schedule at the other alternative was much shorter than its associated stay schedule.

In the unsymmetrical conditions the combinations of ratios of ratios and ratios of sums were different from those used in other conditions. The operating of the schedules and the arranging of reinforcers was identical in all four arrangements of schedules.

For each RI schedule a probability generator was sampled every 0.5 s. If a signal occurred, then the next response associated with that schedule was reinforced. For each rat, Table 1 lists the sequence of conditions, the number of sessions that each condition was in effect, and the arrangement of schedules in each condition. All rats were first exposed to symmetrical conditions. Then they were exposed to two necessary conditions; the sum of the reinforcers was greater at the left alternative in one condition and greater at the right alternative in the other condition. For Rat 761, two sufficient conditions occurred between the two necessary conditions. Next, rats were exposed to two sufficient conditions. Across rats, after several sessions it was clear that for eight sufficient arrangements there was a preference for one alternative. Rather than continue using the values of these arrangements, the schedules were changed in order to produce indifference, because that was the aim of the sufficient condition. Data from these incomplete conditions are not reported. All other conditions remained in effect for at least 15 sessions and until visual inspection showed there were no apparent upward or downward trends in the logs of the ratios of the number of reinforcers, of responses, and of times for five consecutive sessions. Sessions were typically conducted 7 days a week and ended after the first changeover response following the 125th reinforcer. Fifteen to 30 min after each session rats were fed sufficient rat chow, usually 5 to 10 grams, to maintain their 85% weight.

Results

All results reported here are based on the sums of the data from the last five sessions of each condition. Table 1 presents these sums of responses for staying at the left and right alternatives, reinforcers obtained from the stay and switch schedules at each alternative, time spent at the left and right alternatives, and the changeovers (switches) from the left and right alternatives. The first press at an alternative (except for the first response in a session) was the response for switching from the other alternative (Bswi); subsequent presses, until the first press at the other alternative, were responses for staying at an alternative (Bsti). Time spent at the alternative was the cumulative time from the first press at the alternative to the first press at the other alternative.

The stay/switch model described the results of each rat in this experiment. The logs of the stay-response ratios and the logs of the time ratios increased approximately linearly as a joint function of the logs of the ratios of the sums and the logs of the ratios of the ratios (with stay-response ratios shown in Figure 1 and time ratios shown in Figure 2). The results of least-squares linear regressions, presented in Table 2, show that Equation 3 described the stay-response ratios and time ratios for each rat (r2 > .90 in every case). The best-fitting planes from the linear regressions are shown in the figures. The vertical line at each symbol indicates the residual—the amount the obtained datum deviated from the predicted datum. Often the residual is not visible because it is so small, or it is obscured by the filled symbol. This means, with few exceptions, the data points fall almost exactly on the plane. There was no consistent bias: the estimates of log k were usually within 2 SE of 0. For stay-response ratios, log k was at least 2 SE greater than 0 for Rat 758; log k was at least 2 SE less than 0 for Rat 762. For visit-duration ratios, log k was at least 2 SE greater than 0 for Rats 758 and 763. Both the ratio of the sums and the ratio of the ratios are necessary for the good descriptions. For stay-response ratios, estimates of both sensitivities, l and m, were more than 2 SE greater than 0 and they differed from each other by more than 2 SE, indicating they were not equal. For time ratios, estimates of these sensitivities were more than 2 SE greater than 0 for all rats, except m for Rat 763. For Rats 759, 761 and 763 the sensitivities differed from each other by more than 2 SE indicating they were different. Finally, the sensitivities to the ratios of the sums for stay-response ratios were always more than 2 SE greater than for ratios of visit durations. The sensitivity to the ratios of ratios for Rat 763 was more than 2 SE greater than for the ratios of the sums.

Fig 1.

Fig 1

The logs of the ratios of stay responses plotted as a joint function of the logs of the ratios of the sums of the stay and switch reinforcers obtained per visit and the logs of the ratios of the ratios of the stay and switch reinforcers obtained per visit. The plane shows the best-fitting plane, using the stay/switch model (Equation 3) and data from all conditions.

Fig 2.

Fig 2

The logs of the ratios of times plotted as a joint function of the logs of the ratios of the sums of the stay and switch reinforcers obtained per visit and the logs of the ratios of the ratios of the stay and switch reinforcers obtained per visit. The plane shows the best-fitting plane, using the stay/switch model (Equation 3) and data from all conditions.

Table 2.

The results of least-squares multiple linear regressions using the stay/switch model (Equation 3) and the data from all conditions. Also shown are the results of the F-test for each regression using time allocation.

Rat l SE m SE log k SE r2 df F
Ratio of run lengths
758 0.27 0.04 0.78 0.09 −0.15 0.06 0.96 5
759 0.23 0.07 0.98 0.17 −0.02 0.11 0.92 5
760 0.29 0.04 0.96 0.1 −0.08 0.07 0.96 5
761 0.36 0.07 0.92 0.17 0.1 0.12 0.93 5
762 0.15 0.04 0.71 0.1 0.14 0.06 0.93 6
763 0.25 0.05 0.61 0.1 −0.1 0.06 0.93 6
Ratio of visit durations
758 0.22 0.01 0.23 0.02 0.05 0.01 0.99 5 130.7*
759 0.18 0.03 0.45 0.06 0.01 0.04 0.96 5 42.47*
760 0.22 0.02 0.31 0.05 0.05 0.03 0.97 5 30.48*
761 0.22 0.01 0.3 0.03 0 0.02 0.99 5 96.82*
762 0.21 0.03 0.23 0.08 0.04 0.04 0.91 6 11.19*
763 0.38 0.02 0.06 0.04 0.08 0.02 0.99 6 3.47
*

p < .05

Because the stay/switch model described preference the following analysis assessed whether the stay/switch model described the performances at each alternative. The equation for applying the stay/switch model to behavior at Alternative 1 is,

graphic file with name jeab-91-01-02-e04.jpg 4

The equation for performance at Alternative 2 is similar, with subscripts denoting Alternative 2. The results of linear regressions using Equation 4 for run lengths and visit durations at each alternative show that Equation 4 described the performances at each alternative (Table 3), although the goodness-of-fit, in terms of r2, were usually smaller than the goodness-of-fit of the ratios of performances at both alternatives (Table 2). This difference in the goodness-of-fit may be because Equation 3 takes the ratio of the data from each alternative. In Equation 3, each ratio consists of a value plus error in the numerator and a value plus error in the denominator. Then, when dividing using logs, the errors subtract, which reduces the magnitude of error for the ratios. This results in a better goodness-of-fit (greater r2). With the exceptions of Rat 761 on Alternative 1 and Rat 763 on Alternative 2, the descriptions of run lengths were better than for visit durations.

Table 3.

The results of linear regressions using Equation 4, the stay/switch model applied to one alternative. Also shown are the differences between the l, m, and log k for the left and right alternatives and the SE of this difference.

Rat Alternative 1
Alternative 2
l1-l2 SEl1-l2 m1-m2 SEm1-m2 log k1-log k2 SElog k1 – log k2
l1 m1 log k1 r2 l2 m2 log k2 r2
Run lengths
758 0.33 0.69 0.97 0.97 0.21 0.84 1.25 0.86 0.12 0.09 −0.15 0.19 −0.29 0.2
759 0.29 1.04 1.3 0.9 0.15 0.71 1.12 0.75 0.14 0.15 0.33 0.33 0.19 0.24
760 0.36 0.96 1.33 0.95 0.21 0.74 1.2 0.83 0.16 0.09 0.22 0.23 0.12 0.23
761 0.4 0.98 1.4 0.8 0.35 0.71 1.07 0.9 0.05 0.16 0.27 0.35 0.33 0.36
762 0.2 0.7 0.93 0.92 0.13 0.65 0.75 0.86 0.07 0.07 0.05 0.17 0.18 0.11
763 0.23 0.69 0.82 0.93 0.26 0.59 0.86 0.85 −0.03 0.1 0.1 0.19 −0.04 0.15
Visit durations
758 0.25 0.11 0.69 0.75 0.19 0.37 0.89 0.81 0.07 0.09 −0.25 0.19 −0.19 0.18
759 0.19 0.35 0.98 0.84 0.15 0.36 0.98 0.68 0.04 0.1 −0.02 0.22 0 0.16
760 0.23 0.34 0.78 0.83 0.17 0.13 0.54 0.63 0.06 0.08 0.21 0.21 0.25 0.22
761 0.18 0.34 0.89 0.85 0.25 0.26 0.81 0.83 −0.07 0.08 0.09 0.18 0.07 0.17
762 0.21 0.13 0.99 0.83 0.21 0.31 1.07 0.8 0 0.07 −0.19 0.17 −0.08 0.11
763 0.31 0.15 1.06 0.79 0.4 0.19 1.03 0.86 −0.09 0.11 −0.04 0.21 0.03 0.16

Response and time ratios from the symmetrical conditions increased with increases in obtained reinforcer ratios (Figures 3 and 4) and the generalized matching law described these data. Explicitly noting the stay and switch responses and the stay and switch reinforcers in the generalized matching law (Equation 1) produces

graphic file with name jeab-91-01-02-e05.jpg 5

It is important to note that the subscripts refer to the alternative switched from and the alternative where reinforcers were arranged. The symbols are the same as in previous equations. Reinforcer ratios were the sum of the stay reinforcers obtained at the left lever plus the switch reinforcers obtained by changing over from the right lever and pressing the left lever. This sum was divided by the sum of the stay reinforcers obtained at the right lever plus the switch reinforcers obtained by changing over from the left lever and pressing the right lever. This analysis is the standard application of the generalized matching law to concurrent choice, with stay and switch responses and reinforcers explicitly noted (MacDonall, 2000; MacDonall, 2005). Table 4, which shows the results of linear regressions using the data from symmetrical conditions, shows that although there were few data points (at most three), Equation 5 described response and time ratios for each rat (r2 > .90 in every case). Excluding the rats with only two data points, the sensitivity, s, is within 2 SE of 1.0 for Rats 759 and 761, using response ratios. There was no consistent bias. The bias, log b, for response ratios for Rat 758 was at least 2 SE less than 0 and the bias for time ratios was at least 2 SE greater than 0. These results suggest that the generalized matching law described response and time ratios from the symmetrical conditions using the procedures in this experiment. Although there were only two or three control conditions in this experiment, the generalized matching law has also described data when rats were trained in the same procedure on four or five symmetrical conditions (MacDonall, 2005). Thus, any failure of the generalized matching law to describe data from all conditions cannot be attributed to this procedure.

Fig 3.

Fig 3

The logs of the ratios of responses plotted as a function of the logs of the ratios of obtained reinforcers. The solid line shows the best-fitting line, using the generalized matching law (Equation 5) and data from symmetrical conditions. The horizontal and vertical range bars show the range of the reinforcer ratios and the response ratios, respectively, during the 5 days of stable responding.

Fig 4.

Fig 4

The logs of the ratios of times plotted as a function of the logs of the ratios of obtained reinforcers. The solid line shows the best-fitting line, using the generalized matching law (Equation 5) and data from symmetrical conditions. The horizontal and vertical range bars show the range of the reinforcer ratios and the time ratios, respectively, during the 5 days of stable responding.

Table 4.

Results of the least-squares linear regressions using the generalized matching law, with stay and switch reinforcers noted (Equation 5), for response and time ratios from symmetrical conditions only.

Rat s SE log b SE r2 df
Responses
758 0.80 0.05 −0.14 0.04 1.00 1
759 1.01 0.16 −0.19 0.14 .97 1
760 0.95 0.00 −0.07 0.00 1.00 0
761 0.94 0.06 0.00 0.06 1.00 1
762 0.49 0.15 0.18 0.14 .91 1
763 0.61 0.17 −0.12 0.15 .93 1
Times
758 0.60 0.00 0.07 0.00 1.00 1
759 0.76 0.09 −0.04 0.08 .99 1
760 0.73 0.00 0.02 0.00 1.00 0
761 0.68 0.00 −0.02 0.00 1.00 1
762 0.59 0.16 0.11 0.15 .93 1
763 0.83 0.03 0.04 0.03 1.00 1

The two necessary conditions produced response and time preferences first for one alternative and then for the other alternative. Figures 3 and 4 show these data (squares) are aligned approximately vertically over 0 on the x axis. There was a preference for the left alternative and then for the right alternative when the ratio of the obtained reinforcers did not change. The horizontal range bars show the reinforcer ratios overlapped during these two conditions; the vertical range bars show that the preferences did not overlap from one condition to the other.

In the two sufficient conditions no changes in preferences were found, when measured by response ratios. The data points (triangles) are approximately horizontally aligned near 0 on the y axis. The vertical range bars overlap for Rats 759, 760, 761 and 763. For Rats 758 and 762, the bars miss overlapping by 0.03 and 0.05, respectively; the horizontal range bars do not overlap, indicating that the ratio of obtained reinforcers was different in the two sufficient conditions. In these conditions, measuring preference in terms of time ratios shows that preferences did change. The data for Rat 763 lie on the matching line; however, the data for the other 5 rats show an attenuation of the preference compared to the expected preference based on each rat's generalized matching line. The vertical error bars generally were close but did not overlap, indicating the preference differed somewhat in the two conditions. The horizontal error bars did not come close to overlapping, indicating the ratio of obtained reinforcers was clearly different in the two conditions. The reason the preference for time ratios was attenuated but not eliminated, as it was for the response ratios, will be discussed later.

As suggested by Figures 3 and 4, the generalized matching law did not describe the combined data from all conditions. Table 5 shows the results of least-squares linear regressions using Equation 5, which did not describe response ratios well (r2 < .53 in all cases). Equation 5 described the time ratios for Rat 763 very well(r2  =  .98), but not the time ratios for the other 5 rats (with r2 ranging from .66 to .81).

Table 5.

Results of the least-squares linear regressions using the generalized matching law (Equation 5) for response and time ratios from all conditions.

Rat s S.E. log b S.E. r2 df
Responses
758 0.43 0.22 −0.02 0.15 .38 6
759 0.62 0.28 0.00 0.22 .45 6
760 0.41 0.23 −0.03 0.18 .35 6
761 0.59 0.23 −0.02 0.18 .52 6
762 0.22 0.16 0.02 0.12 .22 7
763 0.47 0.18 −0.05 0.11 .50 7
Time
758 0.46 0.09 0.09 0.06 .81 6
759 0.54 0.16 −0.01 0.12 .66 6
760 0.45 0.10 0.05 0.08 .77 6
761 0.49 0.11 −0.06 0.09 .77 6
762 0.43 0.09 −0.02 0.07 .75 7
763 0.82 0.05 0.08 0.03 .98 7

Equation 3, which has an additional fitted parameter, provided much better descriptions of time and response ratios, in terms of r2, except for the time ratios of Rat 763, than did Equation 5. Because these equations differ in the number of fitted parameters, r2 is not appropriate for comparing the descriptions. Because both models used time ratios, the variances are equal and the descriptions can be compared using an F test (Motulsky & Christopoulos, 2003, Ch. 21). The descriptions were significantly different for all except Rat 763 (Table 2). When measuring preference in terms of response ratios Equations 3 and 5 use different dependent variables, which means the variances are different, therefore the models cannot be compared using the F test. Instead, the predicted response (or stay-response) ratios using Equations 3 and 5, respectively, were plotted against the obtained ratios. Figure 5 shows that Equation 3 provided better descriptions. The r2 for stay-response ratios (Equation 3) were at least .45 greater than the r2 for response ratios (Equation 5). Additionally, the regression line for stay-response ratios was always closer to 1.0, perfect prediction, than the line for response ratios. Taken together, these comparisons indicate the stay/switch model (Equation 3) provided better descriptions of the response and time preferences than did the generalized matching law (Equation 5).

Fig 5.

Fig 5

The predicted (stay) response ratio plotted as a function of the obtained (stay) response ratio using the generalized matching law (GML, Equation 5) and the stay/switch model (S/S, Equation 3). The r2 for descriptions by the generalized matching law is in the upper left-hand corner of each plot. The r2 for descriptions by the stay-switch model is in the lower right-hand corner of each plot. The best-fitting line to the data using the stay/switch model is solid. The best-fitting line to the data using generalized matching law is dashed. The dotted line is the locus of perfect prediction.

Discussion

The generalized matching law has widespread empirical support. Consequently, caution should be used when comparing it to another model of choice, such as the stay/switch model. Nonetheless, the stay/switch model described the results of the present experiment when both the ratio of the ratios and the ratio of sums were varied and its description was generally better than that of the generalized matching law. The stay/switch model (Equation 3) described the time preferences from all conditions for each rat, and for 5 rats its descriptions (Table 2) were better than those of the generalized matching law (Table 5). The stay/switch model described the response data from all conditions of each rat and these descriptions were better than those of the generalized matching law. Because the stay/switch model is based on performances at the alternatives, it should also describe those performances. The stay/switch model applied to one alternative (Equation 4) described those performances (Table 3). Finally, the results in Figures 3 and 4 show that changing the ratio of obtained reinforcers, the predictor in the generalized matching law, was neither necessary nor sufficient for changing preference as measured using response allocations: Changing the ratio of obtained reinforcers was not necessary for changing preference as measured by time ratio. Changing the obtained reinforcer ratio, however, was sufficient for changing preference as measured by time ratios.

The reason the obtained reinforcer ratio was sufficient for changing time ratios but not for response ratios was the different sensitivities of stay-response ratios and time ratios to the ratios of sums. The sensitivities to the ratio of the sums was much greater for the stay-response ratio. Consequently, when selecting values for the stay and switch schedules at each alternative that would produce no preference in terms of response ratios, these ratios did produce a preference when measured using time ratios. With time ratios, the much lower sensitivity to the ratios of sums means, comparatively, the ratios of ratios had more influence on time ratios producing a preference. Presumably, one could select values of the schedules that would produce no preference in terms of time ratios, but which could produce a preference in terms of response ratios. It remains for future research to confirm this speculation.

Comparing The Generalized Matching Law and The Stay/Switch Model

The generalized matching law and the stay/switch model focus on different independent and different dependent variables. The generalized matching law focuses on total reinforcers obtained at each alternative and the stay/switch model focuses on stay and switch reinforcers obtained at each alternative. For the dependent variables, the generalized matching law focuses on the ratio of total behavior at each alternative while the stay/switch model focuses on the ratio of the stay responses (run lengths). They both focus on the ratio of time at each alternative. The ratio of visit durations is equivalent to the ratio of times because the switches from each alternative in the visit durations cancel.

Dependent Variables

Time ratios are the same in both models and the stay/switch model provided better descriptions of time allocations. Response measures, however, are different. In two-manipulanda procedures, such as used in the present experiment, the stay/switch model excludes the first response at the alternative, the switch response, from the ratio of stay responses at the alternative. The generalized matching law, on the other hand, includes these responses in the ratio of responses. Thus, although the stay/switch model was generally better, a comparison of the two models using the same dependent variables would be clearer. The difference in dependent variables disappear when using a switch-lever procedure (Findley, 1958), because identical response measures are used. In a switch-lever procedure responses at one lever (the main lever) earn and obtain reinforcers; responses at the other lever (the switch lever) change the alternative in effect. In this procedure the switch responses are the responses at the switch lever and stay responses are responses at the main lever. Consequently, the ratio of responses and the ratio of stay responses are, for both models, the responses at the main lever. Thus, using a switch-lever procedure allows a comparison of the two models with identical calculations of response ratios.

A previous experiment used a switch-lever procedure and varied the rates of earning stay and switch reinforcers at the alternatives (MacDonall, 2005). Table 6 shows the results of linear regressions using Equation 5 and response ratios. Table 7 shows the results of recalculating the linear regressions using Equation 3 and the same response ratios. The linear regressions are recalculated here because MacDonall (2005) did not use Equation 3. Because the same dependent measure was used, the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC, Akaike, 1974) was an appropriate statistic for comparing the models. AIC compares the residual sums-of-squares, considering the number of data points, after adding a penalty for extra fitted parameters. The absolute size of AIC is not meaningful. The difference in AIC is meaningful and the model with the lower AIC is the preferred model (Motulsky & Christopoulos, 2003, Ch. 21). For 5 rats, AIC was lower when using Equation 3 (Tables 6 and 7), meaning this equation was preferred. The AIC for Rat 409 favored Equation 5. This probably occurred because the ratio of the sums had no effect on response ratios; the value of 1 did not differ from 0. This variable did not affect response ratios and did not reduce the residuals. Consequently the penalty in AIC, for the extra parameter in the stay/switch model, increased AIC.

Table 7.

Results of linear regressions using the stay/switch model (Equation 3) and response data from all conditions in MacDonall (2005). Also shown are the results of the AIC analysis.

Rat l SE m SE log k SE r2 df AIC
407 0.07 0.04 0.39 0.06 −0.04 0.03 .89 7 −33
408 0.15 0.03 0.40 0.04 −0.01 0.03 .96 7 −37
409 0.28 0.06 0.07 0.07 0.03 0.04 .90 6 −27
410 0.16 0.03 0.16 0.04 0.03 0.02 .92 7 −39
411 0.05 0.03 0.20 0.03 −0.01 0.02 .88 6 −33
412 0.15 0.03 0.22 0.06 0.03 0.03 .92 6 −31
Independent Variables

The influence of switch reinforcers as used in the present experiment and previous experiments (MacDonall, 1999, 2000, 2003, 2005; MacDonall et al., 2006) is seen in the results reported by Vaughan, Kardish and Wilson (1982). They exposed pigeons to a concurrent VI 3 min VI 3 min and then added a VI 1 min to only one alternative, the varied alternative, that arranged reinforcers for switching from the varied alternative. These additional switch reinforcers were delivered by a response in the other (constant) alternative following a COD. Their procedure increased the switch reinforcers obtained in the constant alternative and they reported that preference shifted towards that alternative.

According to Equation 3 adding the VI 1-min schedule had two effects. First, the number of reinforcers obtained for switching to the constant alternative approximately quadrupled. This decreased the ratio of stay to switch reinforcers at the varied alternative to about 25% of its baseline value, which resulted in more switches to the constant alternative and shorter run lengths at the varied alternative. Second, because these switch reinforcers were delivered after a COD and one or more stay responses would occur during the COD, and because a stay response obtained the switch reinforcer, these switch reinforcers also had the effect of strengthening stay responses at the constant alternative, increasing run lengths at the constant alternative. These two effects, taken together, resulted in a shift in preference towards the constant alternative.

There are perhaps three reasons that the importance of stay-and-switch reinforcers in concurrent schedules was not recognized until now, and they result from the symmetrical arrangement of schedules in standard concurrent procedures. First, the value of the schedule for staying at an alternative is confounded with the value of the schedule for switching to that alternative. Second, sensitivity to the log of the ratio of the ratios, in the stay/switch model (Equation 3), equals half the sensitivity to the log of the ratio of obtained reinforcers in the generalized matching law (Equation 5; MacDonall, 1999). Third, the log of the ratio of the sums equals zero, thus it has no effect on preference. In effect, the stay/switch model is equivalent to the generalized matching law only when using symmetrical arrangements. In summary, I propose that the generalized matching law adequately described the data from concurrent choice procedures because they used symmetrical arrangements, even though obtaining reinforcers was not the critical variable.

Quantitative Descriptions of Preference

Equation 3, which describes a flat plane, provided good descriptions of the stay-response and time ratios from all rats. There were no more than nine data points for any rat, although these data covered a moderately wide range of each variable. The present experiment clearly showed that the generalized matching law did not describe these data, but Equation 3 described these data well. It remains for future research to fully evaluate Equation 3.

In the present experiment, the generalized matching law described the response ratios from the symmetrical conditions. This suggests that the generalized matching law might describe response ratios when the same rule, such as in the necessary conditions or sufficient conditions, is used to generate the schedule values. Figure 1 provides limited support for this idea. It appears data from each arrangement fall in a line going approximately through the origin. For each rat, however, visual inspection indicates the slopes for fits by the generalized matching law to the symmetrical, necessary, and sufficient conditions were very different. Although there are only two data points for each necessary and sufficient condition, similar results were obtained in previous experiments (MacDonall, 2005). Those experiments had more conditions per arrangement. One experiment compared symmetrical and asymmetrical arrangements, and the other compared symmetrical and weighted arrangements. Inspection of Figure 1 (MacDonall, 2005) suggests that the generalized matching law described the behavior from each arrangement, with a slope < 1 for symmetrical conditions and > 1 in the asymmetrical conditions. Inspection of Figure 3 (MacDonall, 2005) shows the generalized matching law described the data from each arrangement, with a different bias for each arrangement.

Although in the previous experiment (MacDonall, 2005) and the present experiment the generalized matching law can describe the results from each arrangement separately, the generalized matching law does not appear to provide a rationale for the changes in slopes or biases for each different arrangement. It appears that factors outside the generalized matching law, that is, the different arrangements, are the factors changing slopes and biases. Fitting the generalized matching law only to data obtained from conditions using the same arrangement of schedules means that data from each arrangement are fitted by the generalized matching law, each producing different bias and sensitivity parameters, one for each arrangement. A more parsimonious approach applies one model simultaneously to all the data from one experiment, regardless of the arrangements of schedules. This is the how the stay/switch model (Equation 3) was applied to the data from MacDonall (2005) and the present experiment. Thus, even though the generalized matching can describe data from each condition in isolation, the stay/switch model's approach is more parsimonious.

The stay/switch model may explain how changing the ratios of the sums changes slopes and biases. This provides additional support for the stay/switch model. The changes in slope and bias in descriptions by the generalized matching law result from the generalized matching law ignoring the ratio of the sums. That is, the changes in preference are caused by changes in the ratio of the sums, which is an independent variable not captured by the generalized matching law.

Summary

The stay/switch model has been presented as a viable alternative to the generalized matching law. It is more parsimonious in that it describes all the data in one analysis, whereas the generalized matching law may describe all the data but needs to apply the law to each different arrangement of schedule values. The stay/switch model explains why slope and bias change in the application of the generalized matching law to different arrangements. The obtained reinforcer ratio was found to be neither necessary nor sufficient for describing response ratios and not necessary for describing time ratios. This seems to imply that the generalized matching law does not identify the critical independent variable.

Although this and recent experiments (MacDonall, 2005, MacDonall et al., 2006) support the stay/switch model, additional experimental evidence is needed before concluding that it offers a better approach than the generalized matching law. First, and most importantly, it remains for sufficient data, covering a wide enough range of the ratio of ratios and ratio of sums, to conclude that a flat surface (Equation 3) describes the relation between reinforcer allocations and behavior. Second, the vast majority of choice experiments use a COD, but the stay/switch model has only been successfully applied using a COD with one pair of stay and switch schedules. Third, the generalized matching law describes choice when magnitudes, delays, and qualities of reinforcers differ. It remains to be demonstrated that the stay/switch model describes choice when varying these features of reinforcers. Fourth, the generalized matching law implies interactions among the two alternatives and the stay/switch model conceptualizes the two sets of choices, one set at each alternative, as independent of each other. Data related to whether or not the choices interact will clarify the stay/switch model. Fifth, in complex human behavior, choice is frequently between topographically different responses. Experiments extending the stay/switch model to choice among topographically different responses remain to be reported.

Acknowledgments

I thank Peter Killeen for discussions that led to this experiment. I am grateful to Jonathan Galente for constructing and maintaining the equipment used in this experiment. I also thank Jon Goodell, Anthony Juliano, Gigi McGraw, Kevin O'Neil, Lillian Riley and Danielle Siwek for assistance in data collection, and Kevin O'Neil for preparing the figures. Preparation of this manuscript was supported, in part, by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH81266. Portions of these data were presented at the 2004 meeting of the Psychonomic Society and the 2005 meeting of the Society for the Quantitative Analyses of Behavior.

References

  1. Akaike H. A new look at statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions Automatic Control. 1974;19:716–722. [Google Scholar]
  2. Baum W.M. On two types of deviation from the matching law: Bias and undermatching. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1974;22:231–242. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1974.22-231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Findley J.D. Preference and switching under concurrent scheduling. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1958;1:123–144. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1958.1-123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Herrnstein R.J. Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1961;4:267–272. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1961.4-267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. MacDonall J.S. A local model of concurrent performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1999;71:57–74. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1999.71-57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. MacDonall J.S. Synthesizing concurrent interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 2000;74:189–206. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2000.74-189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. MacDonall J.S. Reinforcing staying and switching while using a changeover delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 2003;79:219–232. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2003.79-219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. MacDonall J.S. Earning and obtaining reinforcers under concurrent interval scheduling. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 2005;84:167–183. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2005.76-04. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. MacDonall J.S. Some effects of overall rate of earning reinforcers on run lengths and visit durations. Behavioural Processes. 2006;73:13–21. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2006.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. MacDonall J.S, Goodell J, Juliano A. Momentary maximizing and optimal foraging theories of performances on concurrent VR schedules. Behavioural Processes. 2006;72:283–299. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2006.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Motulsky H, Christopoulos A. Fitting Models to Data Using Linear and Nonlinear Regression. Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  12. Sugrue L.P, Corrado G.S, Newsome W.T. Matching behavior and the representation of value in the parietal cortex. Science. 2004;304:1782–1787. doi: 10.1126/science.1094765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Vaughan W, Kardish T.A, Wilson M. Correlation versus contiguity in choice. Behaviour Analysis Letters. 1982;2:153–160. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior are provided here courtesy of Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior

RESOURCES