Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Jan 16.
Published in final edited form as: Science. 2006 Sep 15;313(5793):1626–1628. doi: 10.1126/science.1128115

High Gamma Power Is Phase-Locked to Theta Oscillations in Human Neocortex

R T Canolty 1,*, E Edwards 1,2, S S Dalal 3, M Soltani 1,2, S S Nagarajan 3,4, H E Kirsch 5, M S Berger 6, N M Barbaro 5,6, R T Knight 1,2,3,5,6
PMCID: PMC2628289  NIHMSID: NIHMS14964  PMID: 16973878

Abstract

We observed robust coupling between the high- and low-frequency bands of ongoing electrical activity in the human brain. In particular, the phase of the low-frequency theta (4 to 8 hertz) rhythm modulates power in the high gamma (80 to 150 hertz) band of the electrocorticogram, with stronger modulation occurring at higher theta amplitudes. Furthermore, different behavioral tasks evoke distinct patterns of theta/high gamma coupling across the cortex. The results indicate that transient coupling between low- and high-frequency brain rhythms coordinates activity in distributed cortical areas, providing a mechanism for effective communication during cognitive processing in humans.


Neuronal oscillations facilitate synaptic plasticity (1), influence reaction time (2), correlate with attention (3) and perceptual binding (4), and are proposed to play a role in transient, long-range coordination of distinct brain regions (5). Direct cortical recordings reveal that ongoing rhythms encompass a wide range of spatial and temporal scales— ultraslow rhythms less than 0.05 Hz coexist with fast transient oscillations 500 Hz or greater (1), with spatial coherence between these oscillations extending from several centimeters for the corticospinal tract (6) to the micrometer scale for subthreshold membrane oscillations in a single neuron (7). Exactly how these transient oscillations influence each other and coordinate processing at both the single-neuron and population levels remains unknown.

Evidence for cross-frequency coupling, where one frequency band modulates the activity of a different frequency band, is more abundant in animal than human data. For example, the theta rhythm can modulate the firing rate and spike timing of a single neuron (811) as well as the gamma power of the intracortical local field potential (8, 12, 13). Task-related changes in theta power have been observed in humans (1416), and cross-frequency coupling at frequencies up to 40 Hz has been detected at the scalp (17, 18). However, given the difficulty in localizing electrical sources from scalp recordings alone (19), subdural electrodes that record directly from the human cortex are needed to address this question. Furthermore, subdural electrodes are ideal for studying activity in the recently described human high gamma band (HG) at 80 to 150 Hz. HG activity is modulated by sensory, motor, and cognitive events (20), is functionally distinct from low gamma (30 to 80 Hz) with different physiological origins (21), and is correlated with the functional magnetic resonance imaging blood oxygen level–dependent (fMRI BOLD) signal (2224). There have been no reports of coupling between any low-frequency rhythm and HG in signals recorded either at the scalp or directly from human sensory, motor, or association cortex. We therefore focus exclusively on theta/HG coupling in this report.

We analyzed multichannel subdural electrocorticogram (ECoG) data from five patients undergoing neurosurgical treatment for epilepsy. Typically, the events of interest in behavioral paradigms are the stimulus onsets and motor responses that evoke frequency-specific changes in the electrical activity of the brain. In contrast, the events of interest in cross-frequency coupling are features of the ongoing oscillatory activity itself. That is, cross-frequency coupling refers to statistical dependence between distinct frequency bands of the ongoing ECoG rather than dependence between the ECoG and external stimulus events. The dependence between two frequencies f1 and f2 can assume many forms, including coupling between the amplitude envelopes A1(t) and A2(t), the phase time series φ1(t) and φ2(t), or an amplitude-phase coupling between A1(t) and φ2(t). We focus here on the last type of coupling and use an index of cross-frequency coupling that directly combines the amplitude envelope time series A1(t + τ) of a high-frequency band with the phase time series φ2(t) of alow-frequency band into one composite, complex-valued signal z(t, τ). The (normalized) temporal mean of this composite signal provides a sensitive measure of the coupling strength and preferred phase between the two frequencies (25).

Animal evidence for theta phase modulation of single-unit firing and the strong connection of theta to learning, attention, and memory (26, 27) suggested to us that high-frequency oscillations in human neocortex may be modulated by the theta rhythm. Accordingly, we analyzed the ECoG across a range of behavioral tasks (25). Figure 1B shows a time-frequency plot for data recorded from an electrode over the left middle frontal gyrus during an auditory language–related target detection task (Fig. 1A, arrow). Theta trough–locked averaging of the normalized time-frequency plane shows significant coupling (P < 0.001, corrected) between theta phase and high-frequency power, with an increase or decrease in power relative to baseline occurring at the theta trough or peak. Theta coupling was broadband from ∼20 to 200 Hz, with the strongest modulation occurring in the HG band. Fig. 1D and fig. S4, using the modulation index discussed above, also show that coupling is strongest between theta phase and HG amplitude.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

High gamma (80 to 150 Hz) power is modulated by theta (4 to 8Hz) phase. (A) Structural MRI showing position of 64-channel ECoG grid over frontal and temporal lobes in subject 1. (B) Example of phase-locked modulation of power in the ECoG signal from an electrode over the anterior portion of the middle frontal gyrus (arrow in Fig. 1A). (Top) Time-frequency plot of mean power modulation time-locked to the theta trough. Outermost contour indicates statistical significance (P < 0.001, corrected). Normalization permits comparison across frequencies; red and blue indicate a power increase or decrease, respectively, relative to the mean power. (Bottom) Theta trough–locked average of raw ECoG signal. (C) Best-fit gamma distributions for the high gamma analytic amplitude values that occurred at the peak (black, 0 radians) or the trough (red, p radians) of the theta waveform for the same electrode as in Fig. 1B. The difference in parameter values is significant (P < 0.001). (D) The modulation index (25) as a function of analytic amplitude (5 to 200 Hz) and analytic phase (2 to 20 Hz) for the same electrode as in Fig. 1B. Outermost contour indicates statistical significance (P < 0.001, corrected). Larger values indicate stronger cross-frequency coupling. Maximal coupling for this electrode is 146.2 Hz amplitude and 5.6 Hz phase (see also fig. S4).

Across all tasks and subjects, 252 out of 299 tested electrodes (84.3%) showed significant theta/gamma coupling (P < 0.001 for each electrode, corrected). Excluding the 60 electrodes over resected tissue (which includes both epileptic and healthy tissue) increases this percentage to 88.7%, whereas only 66.7% of electrodes over resected tissue showed significant coupling. The largest HG amplitudes tended to occur at the trough of the theta waveform in electrodes with strong coupling (Figs. 1C and 2A). The coupling strength between the HG analytic amplitude time series AHG(t + τ) and theta analytic phase φTH(t) should decrease to chance levels as the magnitude of the time lag t increases. Figure 3A, displaying all ECoG electrodes for one subject, shows that this is indeed the case (see fig. S8 for all subjects).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Theta/HG coupling strength is a function of theta amplitude. (A) Theta/HG coupling strength and preferred theta phase. (Bottom) One theta cycle (schematic), from theta peak (0 radians) to trough (π radians) to peak (π radians). (Top) Modulation index (25) computed separately for all electrodes in all subjects for each task. Larger magnitudes indicate stronger coupling (vertical axis), whereas the horizontal axis indicates the theta phase at which larger HG amplitudes tend to occur. Most electrodes with strong theta/HG coupling have a preferred theta phase of π, corresponding to the theta trough (see also Fig. 1C). The red dot indicates the electrode and recording block examined in Fig. 1. The red horizontal line corresponds to the significance threshold after correction for multiple comparisons. (B) Modulation index versus mean theta amplitude for all significant values from Fig. 2A (black dots) and best linear fit (red line), indicating their positive correlation. (C) Modulation index versus mean HG amplitude for all significant values from Fig. 2A (black dots) and best linear fit (red line), indicating their weak negative correlation (see also fig. S6).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Task-specific changes in the spatial pattern of theta/HG coupling strength. (A) Theta/HG coupling strength falls to chance at large time lags. Modulation index (25)asa function of lag for all electrodes over all tasks from subject 5. Electrodes are sorted by the time-lag τmax associated with maximal coupling (black line). For ease of comparison, horizontal traces were renormalized so that the peak value for each channel is one (see also fig. S8). (B) The change in modulation index values from the mean for all electrodes in subject 2 during one task (passive listening to predictable tones). (C) As in (B), for a difficult working memory task. Subjects listened to a list of phonemes and responded when the current phoneme and the phoneme presented two items earlier were identical. (D) Similar tasks evoke similar spatial patterns of theta/HG coupling. Correlation matrix for all tasks in subject 2. Tasks: 1 to 4, passive listening to tones or phonemes; 5, mouth motor activation; 6, verb generation; 7, hand motor activation; 8 to 11 auditory working memory; 12 and 13, linguistic target detection; 14 to 17, auditory-vibrotactile target detection (see SOM text). (E) Mean correlation and standard error between similar tasks (positive, P < 0.01, corrected, 58 task pairs) as well as different tasks (not significant, 617 task pairs) for all electrodes in all subjects over all tasks.

The strength of theta/HG coupling depends on theta power as well as theta phase. We observed stronger coupling in electrodes with greater mean theta amplitude (Fig. 2B). That is, HG amplitudes have a stronger theta phase preference at greater theta power, indicating that theta/HG coupling strength can be modulated by adjusting theta power in a local cortical region. This contrasts with the weak negative correlation observed between theta/HG coupling strength and mean HG amplitude (Fig. 2C and fig. S6). Thus, mean HG power and the strength of theta/HG coupling appear to reflect independent dimensions of cortical activity.

Task-dependent modulation of theta power has been shown in humans (26), prompting the hypothesis that theta/HG coupling may be task-dependent. Two examples of task-specific changes in the spatial pattern of theta/HG coupling strength over all electrodes in one subject are shown in Fig. 3, B and C. Figure 3D shows that behavioral tasks evoke distinct and reproducible patterns of coupling in this subject, with similar tasks evoking similar coupling patterns whereas different tasks evoked alternate patterns. Spatial patterns associated with two runs of similar tasks were positively correlated, whereas runs of different tasks exhibited a null or negative correlation. This trend held across all tasks and subjects, as shown by Fig. 3E. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that transient cross-frequency coupling modulates network engagement, enabling flexible control of cognitive processing.

Oscillations are rhythmic fluctuations in neuronal excitability that modulate both output spike timing and sensitivity to synaptic input (5). Therefore, effective communication between neuronal populations requires precise matching of the relative phase of distinct rhythms to axonal conduction delays. An oscillatory hierarchy operating across multiple spatial and temporal scales could regulate this proposed long-range communication (13). Basal forebrain cortical-projecting GABAergic (γ-aminobutyric acid–releasing) neurons are well positioned to control theta/HG coupling; these neurons preferentially synapse onto intracortical GABAergic neurons throughout the cortex, with disinhibitory spike bursts causing a brief increase in gamma power at the theta trough (28). Our observations that (i) HG power is modulated by theta phase, (ii) an increase in theta power strengthens theta/HG coupling, and (iii) the topography of theta/HG coupling is task-dependent support the hypothesis that cross-frequency coupling between distinct brain rhythms facilitates the transient coordination of cortical areas required for adaptive behavior in humans.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary

References and Notes

  • 1.Buzsaki G, Draguhn A. Science. 2004;304:1926. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Linkenkaer-Hansen K, Nikulin VV, Palva S, Ilmoniemi RJ, Palva JM. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:10186. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2584-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Womelsdorf T, Fries P, Mitra PP, Desimone R. Nature. 2006;439:733. doi: 10.1038/nature04258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Lachaux JP, et al. Neuroimage. 2005;25:491. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Fries P. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005;9:474. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Fries P. Science. 2005;308:111. doi: 10.1126/science.1107027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Izhikevich EM. Neural Netw. 2001;14:883. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(01)00078-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Bragin A, et al. J. Neurosci. 1995;15:47. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-01-00047.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Buzsaki G, et al. Neuroscience. 2003;116:201. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00669-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Siapas AG, Lubenov EV, Wilson MA. Neuron. 2005;46:141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lee H, Simpson GV, Logothetis NK, Rainer G. Neuron. 2005;45:147. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Chrobak JJ, Buzsaki G. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:388. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-01-00388.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lakatos P, et al. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;94:1904. doi: 10.1152/jn.00263.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ekstrom AD, et al. Hippocampus. 2005;15:881. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sederberg PB, Kahana MJ, Howard MW, Donner EJ, Madsen JR. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:10809. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-34-10809.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Onton J, Delorme A, Makeig S. Neuroimage. 2005;27:341. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Schack B, Vath N, Petsche H, Geissler HG, Moller E. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2002;44:143. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(01)00199-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Palva JM, Palva S, Kaila K. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:3962. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4250-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Nunez PL, Srinivasan RS. Electric Fields of the Brain: The Neurophysics of EEG. Oxford Univ. Press; Oxford: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Crone NE, Miglioretti DL, Gordon B, Lesser RP. Brain. 1998;121:2301. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.12.2301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Edwards E, Soltani M, Deouell LY, Berger MS, Knight RT. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;94:4269. doi: 10.1152/jn.00324.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Logothetis NK, Pauls J, Augath M, Trinath T, Oeltermann A. Nature. 2001;412:150. doi: 10.1038/35084005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Mukamel R, et al. Science. 2005;309:951. doi: 10.1126/science.1110913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Niessing J, et al. Science. 2005;309:948. doi: 10.1126/science.1110948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Materials and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online.
  • 26.Kahana MJ, Seelig D, Madsen JR. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2001;11:739. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(01)00278-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Miller R. Cortico-Hippocampal Interplay and the Representation of Contexts in the Brain. Springer-Verlag; New York: 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lin SC, Gervasoni D, Nicolelis MAL. J. Neurophysiol. 2006 doi: 10.1152/jn.00524.2006. doi: 10.1152/jn.00524.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. This work was supported by the Rauch family, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke grant NS21135, NSF Fellowship 2004016118, and National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders grant F31DC006762. The authors thank P. Garcia and D. Filippi of the Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, for their help on electrical stimulation mapping and localizing epileptic tissue.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary

RESOURCES