Abstract
When Pat Goldman-Rakic described the circuitry and function of primate prefrontal cortex in her influential 1987 monograph (Goldman-Rakic, 1987), she included only a few short paragraphs on the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). That year, there were only nine papers published containing the term “orbitofrontal,” an average of less than one paper per month. Twenty years later, this rate has increased to 32 papers per month. This explosive growth is partly attributable to the remarkable similarities that exist in structure and function across species. These similarities suggest that OFC function can be usefully modeled in nonhuman and even nonprimate species. Here, we review some of these similarities.
Keywords: conditioned, conditioning, human, learning, prefrontal cortex, primate, rat, reward
What do we know about orbitofrontal cortex from studies of reversal learning?
Historically, OFC has been linked to inflexible or inappropriate responding. This deficit is evident in Dr. John Harlow's account of the most famous orbitofrontal patient, Phineas Gage (Harlow, 1868), and it is most often demonstrated experimentally in reversal learning tasks. Reversal learning occurs when an animal must alter established response tendencies when outcome contingencies change, typically in discrimination tasks (Fig. 1A). Thus, the animal is taught that responding to one cue produces reward, whereas executing the same response to another cue produces nonreward or punishment. After the animal learns to respond correctly, the experimenter switches the cue–outcome associations, and the animal must learn to change its behavior. Rats, cats, mice, marmosets, monkeys, and humans with damage to OFC are slower to acquire reversals (Mishkin, 1964; Teitelbaum, 1964; Butter, 1969; Dias et al., 1996; Bechara et al., 1997; Ferry et al., 2000; Chudasama and Robbins, 2003; Fellows and Farah, 2003; Schoenbaum et al., 2003; Hornak et al., 2004; Izquierdo et al., 2004; Bissonette et al., 2005), and neural activity related to the cue–outcome learning across reversals is evident in OFC of rats, monkeys, and humans (Thorpe et al., 1983; Schoenbaum et al., 1999; O'Doherty et al., 2003b; Wallis and Miller, 2003; Hampton et al., 2006) (Fig. 1B,C). Similar results are obtained despite the methodological differences in these studies. For example, lesions can be neurotoxic or by aspiration, punishment can be explicit or consist of a time-out, the task can be go, no-go, or symmetrically rewarded, and the outcome contingencies may be all-or-nothing or probabilistic.
Thus OFC appears to be fundamentally critical to facilitating rapid reversal learning, but why? Although this deficit has often been attributed to OFC's role in inhibiting “prepotent” responding, typically animals are able to inhibit the same response before learning that they are unable to inhibit after reversal. Moreover, OFC lesions do not affect reversal of some naturalistic or innate response tendencies (Chudasama et al., 2007). This suggests that OFC is not critical for reversal learning because of a general role in response inhibition. Instead, there must be some deeper underlying function critical for guiding behavior when outcomes change. Insight into this function has come in the last several years through the use of reinforcer devaluation tasks.
What do we know about orbitofrontal cortex from studies of reinforcer devaluation?
Reinforcer devaluation refers to a procedure in which changes in a learned response are assessed after devaluation of the expected outcome. Typically, this is done with food by selectively satiating the animal on the outcome or by pairing it with illness (Holland and Straub, 1979). In contrast to reversal learning, in which the animal directly experiences pairing of the new outcome with the cues and responses, the effects of reinforcer devaluation do not involve a change in the actual outcome and can be observed under extinction conditions. Under these conditions, the animal must use preexisting information, updated with the new outcome value, to appropriately guide behavior. This complex feat, which requires recall of the original associations and integration with the new outcome value, is easily accomplished by normal animals, who respond significantly less for a devalued than a nondevalued outcome. In contrast, rats and monkeys with OFC lesions fail to alter learned responding after devaluation (Gallagher et al., 1999; Izquierdo et al., 2004). This is true even when OFC lesions are made after acquisition of the stimulus–outcome associations or even after outcome devaluation (Pickens et al., 2003, 2005), suggesting that OFC plays a critical role in evoking the original learning and integrating it with the new value of the outcome to guide responding. Consistent with this proposal, neurons in OFC modify their responses to food items and to predictive cues to reflect changes in their current incentive value (Critchley and Rolls, 1996; Tremblay and Schultz, 1999; Hikosaka and Watanabe, 2000; Gottfried et al., 2003; Scott et al., 2005). Thus, OFC is critical for signaling the current value of the expected outcome.
Questions for the next 20 years
Although the last 20 years have brought remarkable progress, this work has also uncovered interesting questions; here we highlight several.
How (When?) does OFC facilitate flexible behavior?
OFC is important for outcome-guided behavior (devaluation) and also for facilitating changes in that behavior in the face of unexpected outcomes (reversal). Are these functions dissociable, or do they derive from a common “precursor” function? The involvement of OFC in reversal learning may reflect its critical role in signaling expected outcomes, which may contribute to teaching signals early in reversal (Stalnaker et al., 2007). Alternatively, reversal learning may rely on rules regarding the occurrence rather than the value of reward per se (Murray and Izquierdo, 2007), suggesting different underlying mechanisms underlying OFC's role in the two tasks. Consistent with this idea, neurons in OFC clearly encode abstract rules that predict the occurrence of reward (Ramus and Eichenbaum, 2000; Wallis et al., 2001), and preliminary data suggest a possible dissociation of devaluation and reversal abilities within subregions of OFC (Kazama and Bachevalier, 2006). However, value signals in human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex are modulated by the rules or structure of reversal tasks (Hampton et al., 2006). Disentangling these functions may prove difficult.
How does OFC represent apples versus oranges … and footshock?
Research on OFC function is converging with the rapidly developing field of neuroeconomics. A central tenet of neuroeconomics is that the brain has a common currency in which to represent the value of goods, so that decisions between dissimilar items, like apples and oranges, can be made efficiently. This role has been assigned to OFC (Montague and Berns, 2002); OFC neurons are activated by rewards and cues that predict rewards in a way that reflects their incentive value, typically independent of the predictive cue or associated response (Tremblay and Schultz, 1999; Arana et al., 2003; O'Doherty et al., 2003b; Wallis and Miller, 2003; Roesch and Olson, 2005). Consistent with this proposal, OFC neurons in monkeys that are trained to perform a complex decision-making task for differently valued rewards seem to provide a pure, linear representation of the juices' value (Padoa-Schioppa and Assad, 2006). These data show that OFC can signal apparent economic value. However, a number of issues remain to be addressed. For example, how specific is this result to OFC and to this particular setting? Is a similar signal observed in other brain areas or when decisions must be made spontaneously, in a novel context or between novel goods? Or is such pure value encoding only observed in OFC in expert decision makers? Data from rats learning to make decisions suggest that economically relevant variables, in this case response direction as well as time to and size of reward, are represented independently within OFC (Roesch et al., 2006). Although this may be a species difference, it may also be that circumstance and familiarity are critical in determining how tightly encoding in OFC integrates disparate information bearing on value. It will also be critical to know how aversive outcomes are integrated into this common currency. OFC represents bad outcomes as well as good ones (O'Doherty et al., 2001; Gottfried et al., 2002; O'Doherty et al., 2003a; Morrison and Salzman, 2006).
Does OFC discriminate between pavlovian (stimulus-outcome) and instrumental (response-outcome) associations?
A role for OFC in signaling expected outcomes has been demonstrated thus far only in pavlovian settings. This includes the explicit pavlovian reinforcer devaluation task described above (Gallagher et al., 1999) as well as operant tasks built on object discrimination learning (Izquierdo et al., 2004), in which devaluation likely operates on pavlovian associations between the cues and the differently preferred food rewards. Whether OFC also plays a role in response-outcome or “goal-directed” instrumental learning is less clear. Recently, one of us has shown that blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) activity in human OFC during the selection of actions leading to different outcomes is modulated as a result of reinforcer devaluation (Valentin et al., 2007). Similarly, two different laboratories have shown that neurons in rat OFC fire differentially for left and right responses, even when they lead to the same reward (Feierstein et al., 2006; Roesch et al., 2006). However, large lesions of rat OFC fail to disrupt changes in instrumental responding after reinforcer devaluation (Ostlund and Balleine, 2007). Several accounts may reconcile these findings. First, although information linking responses and outcomes may be present in OFC, it may not be essential for behavior in instrumental settings. Apparent response–outcome correlates may also reflect sensory attributes of different responses (e.g., space). Alternatively, portions of rat and primate “medial” OFC may be more closely related to medial prefrontal areas already implicated in guiding behavior based on action–outcome associations (Rushworth et al., 2007). Additional study will be needed to evaluate these proposals.
What is OFC anyway?
A common theme in many of the questions discussed above is how to define OFC: what are the critical anatomical features that describe an area as orbitofrontal, both within and across species, and are there specialized areas within OFC that mediate particular functions? It will be increasingly critical in future studies to be precise in defining subregions within OFC. For example, future work will likely uncover differences in OFC function that reflect evolutionary specializations between species. One region of particular interest in this regard may be the rostral portion of OFC in primates, which is characterized by a granular cell layer that is not evident in rats (Preuss, 1995). Another open question is the nature of the functional relationship between rostral parts of OFC and adjacent regions of frontopolar cortex implicated in exploratory decision making (Daw et al., 2006). In addition, future studies should address the way in which autonomic signals affect processing within OFC (Roberts, 2006). Determining how regions within OFC promote flexibility, represent potential positive and negative consequences of choices, and guide selection of the most advantageous choices will be a highlight of the coming 20 years. This work will help elucidate the neural underpinnings of adaptive behavior and may also shed light on unique features of the human character.
References
- Arana FS, Parkinson JA, Hinton E, Holland AJ, Owen AM, Roberts AC. Dissociable contributions of the human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex to incentive motivation and goal selection. J Neurosci. 2003;23:9632–9638. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-29-09632.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997;275:1293–1294. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bissonette GB, Martins G, Lande MD, Schoenbaum G, Powell EM. Assessing prefrontal cortical function in mice. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 2005;31:66–19. [Google Scholar]
- Butter CM. Perseveration and extinction in discrimination reversal tasks following selective frontal ablations in Macaca mulatta. Physiol Behav. 1969;4:163–171. [Google Scholar]
- Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. Dissociable contributions of the orbitofrontal and infralimbic cortex to pavlovian autoshaping and discrimination reversal learning: further evidence for the functional heterogeneity of the rodent frontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2003;23:8771–8780. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-25-08771.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chudasama Y, Kralik JD, Murray EA. Rhesus monkeys with orbital prefrontal cortex lesions can learn to inhibit prepotent responses in the reversed reward contingency task. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:1154–1159. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley HD, Rolls ET. Hunger and satiety modify the responses of olfactory and visual neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1996;75:1673–1686. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.4.1673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daw ND, O'Doherty JP, Dayan P, Seymour B, Dolan RJ. Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature. 2006;441:876–879. doi: 10.1038/nature04766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dias R, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature. 1996;380:69–72. doi: 10.1038/380069a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feierstein CE, Quirk MC, Uchida N, Sosulski DL, Mainen ZF. Representation of spatial goals in rat orbitofrontal cortex. Neuron. 2006;51:495–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fellows LK, Farah MJ. Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. Brain. 2003;126:1830–1837. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferry AT, Lu XC, Price JL. Effects of excitotoxic lesions in the ventral striatopallidal–thalamocortical pathway on odor reversal learning: inability to extinguish an incorrect response. Exp Brain Res. 2000;131:320–335. doi: 10.1007/s002219900240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher M, McMahan RW, Schoenbaum G. Orbitofrontal cortex and representation of incentive value in associative learning. J Neurosci. 1999;19:6610–6614. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-15-06610.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman-Rakic PS. Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behavior by representational memory. In: Mountcastle VB, Plum F, Geiger SR, editors. Handbook of physiology: the nervous system. Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society; 1987. pp. 373–417. [Google Scholar]
- Gottfried JA, O'Doherty J, Dolan RJ. Appetitive and aversive olfactory learning in humans studied using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci. 2002;22:10829–10837. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10829.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gottfried JA, O'Doherty J, Dolan RJ. Encoding predictive reward value in human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Science. 2003;301:1104–1107. doi: 10.1126/science.1087919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampton AN, Bossaerts P, O'Doherty JP. The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in abstract state-based inference during decision-making in humans. J Neurosci. 2006;26:8360–8367. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1010-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harlow JM. Recovery after passage of an iron bar through the head. Publ Mass Med Soc. 1868;2:329–346. [Google Scholar]
- Hikosaka K, Watanabe M. Delay activity of orbital and lateral prefrontal neurons of the monkey varying with different rewards. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:263–271. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland PC, Straub JJ. Differential effects of two ways of devaluing the unconditioned stimulus after pavlovian appetitive conditioning. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1979;5:65–78. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.5.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hornak J, O'Doherty J, Bramham J, Rolls ET, Morris RG, Bullock PR, Polkey CE. Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbito-frontal or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16:463–478. doi: 10.1162/089892904322926791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izquierdo AD, Suda RK, Murray EA. Bilateral orbital prefrontal cortex lesions in rhesus monkeys disrupt choices guided by both reward value and reward contingency. J Neurosci. 2004;24:7540–7548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1921-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kazama AM, Bachevalier J. Selective aspiration or neurotoxic lesions of the orbital frontal areas 11 and 13 spared monkeys' performance on the object discrimination reversal task. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 2006;32:670–25. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4655-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mishkin M. Perseveration of central sets after frontal lesions in monkeys. In: Warren JM, Akert K, editors. The frontal granular cortex and behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1964. [Google Scholar]
- Montague PR, Berns GS. Neural economics and the biological substrates of valuation. Neuron. 2002;36:265–284. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00974-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morrison SE, Salzman CD. Representation of stimulus value in primate orbitofrontal cortex during reinforcement learning. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 2006;32:164–165. [Google Scholar]
- Murray EA, Izquierdo AD. Orbitofrontal and amygdala contributions to affect and action in primates. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2007 doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.021. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Doherty J, Kringelback ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C. Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:95–102. doi: 10.1038/82959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Doherty J, Winston J, Critchley H, Perrett D, Burt DM, Dolan RJ. Beauty in a smile: the role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia. 2003a;41:147–155. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00145-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Doherty J, Critchley HD, Deichmann R, Dolan RJ. Dissociating valence of outcome from behavioral control in human orbital and ventral prefrontal cortices. J Neurosci. 2003b;23:7931–7939. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-21-07931.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostlund SB, Balleine BW. Orbitofrontal cortex mediates outcome encoding in pavlovian but not instrumental learning. J Neurosci. 2007 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5443-06.2007. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Padoa-Schioppa C, Assad JA. Neurons in orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature. 2006;441:223–226. doi: 10.1038/nature04676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickens CL, Setlow B, Saddoris MP, Gallagher M, Holland PC, Schoenbaum G. Different roles for orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in a reinforcer devaluation task. J Neurosci. 2003;23:11078–11084. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-35-11078.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickens CL, Saddoris MP, Gallagher M, Holland PC. Orbitofrontal lesions impair use of cue-outcome associations in a devaluation task. Behav Neurosci. 2005;119:317–322. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preuss TM. Do rats have prefrontal cortex? The Rose-Woolsey-Akert program reconsidered. J Comp Neurol. 1995;7:1–24. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramus SJ, Eichenbaum H. Neural correlates of olfactory recognition memory in the rat orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2000;20:8199–8208. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-21-08199.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts AC. Primate orbitofrontal cortex and adaptive behavior. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roesch MR, Olson CR. Neuronal activity in primate orbitofrontal cortex reflects the value of time. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:2457–2471. doi: 10.1152/jn.00373.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roesch MR, Taylor AR, Schoenbaum G. Encoding of time-discounted rewards in orbitofrontal cortex is independent of value representation. Neuron. 2006;51:509–520. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rushworth MF, Behrens TE, Rudebeck PH, Walton ME. Contrasting roles for cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex in decisions and social behaviour. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:168–176. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Chiba AA, Gallagher M. Neural encoding in orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala during olfactory discrimination learning. J Neurosci. 1999;19:1876–1884. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-05-01876.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Setlow B, Nugent SL, Saddoris MP, Gallagher M. Lesions of orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala complex disrupt acquisition of odor-guided discriminations and reversals. Learn Mem. 2003;10:129–140. doi: 10.1101/lm.55203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott TR, Edwards EM, Smith CA, Hilgert KG, Schwartz GJ, Pritchard TC. Medial orbitofrontal cortex: its role in mediating satiety in the macaque. Chem Senses. 2005;30(Suppl 1):il90. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stalnaker TA, Franz TM, Singh T, Schoenbaum G. Basolateral amygdala lesions abolish orbitofrontal-dependent reversal impairments. Neuron. 2007 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.014. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teitelbaum H. A comparison of effects of orbitofrontal and hippocampal lesions upon discrimination learning and reversal in the cat. Exp Neurology. 1964;9:452–462. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(64)90053-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorpe SJ, Rolls ET, Maddison S. The orbitofrontal cortex: neuronal activity in the behaving monkey. Exp Brain Res. 1983;49:93–115. doi: 10.1007/BF00235545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tremblay L, Schultz W. Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal cortex. Nature. 1999;398:704–708. doi: 10.1038/19525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valentin VV, Dickinson A, O'Doherty JP. Determining the neural substrates of goal-directed learning in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2007;27:4019–4026. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0564-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallis JD, Miller EK. Neuronal activity in primate dorsolateral and orbital prefrontal cortex during performance of a reward preference task. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18:2069–2081. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02922.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallis JD, Anderson KC, Miller EK. Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules. Nature. 2001;411:953–956. doi: 10.1038/35082081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]