Abstract
Cognitive deficits are a core feature of established psychotic illnesses. However, the association between cognition and emerging psychosis is less understood. While there is some evidence that cognitive deficits are present prior to the onset of psychosis, findings are not consistent. In this article we provide an overview of the more general cognitive findings available from genetic high-risk studies, retrospective studies, and birth cohort studies. We then focus the review on neuropsychological performance in clinically “at-risk” groups. Overall, general cognitive ability as assessed by established batteries appears to remain relatively intact in these ultra-high risk cohorts and is a poor predictor close to illness onset relative to other vulnerability factors. Further decline may occur with illness progression, more consistent with state relative to trait factors. In addition, most established cognitive tasks involve several relatively discrete cognitive subprocesses, where findings from general batteries of subtests may mask specific deficits. In this context, our review suggests that relatively specific olfactory identification and spatial working memory deficits exist prior to illness onset and may be more potent trait markers for psychosis than cognitively dense tasks such as verbal memory. Suggestions for further research address the importance of standardization of inclusion criteria and the maintenance of basic neuropsychological assessment to allow better comparison of findings across centers. Further, in order to better understand the aetiopathology of cognitive dysfunction in psychosis, more experimental, hypothesis-driven measures of discrete cognitive processes are required. Delineation of the relationship between specific cognitive ability and symptoms from data-driven approaches may improve our understanding of the role of cognition during psychosis onset.
Keywords: schizophrenia, high risk, first-episode psychosis, neurodevelopmental, prodrome, cognition
Introduction
Cognitive deficits are viewed as central to the underlying pathophysiology of established psychotic disorders, particularly schizophrenia,1 with the most consistent findings being problems in attention, memory, and higher-order executive function.2–4 These deficits are present at the first onset,5–8 are largely unrelated to positive symptomatology,9–13 and do not change over time.9,14 In other studies a proportion of patients appear to undergo a decline in general intellectual function at some point either prior to or around the first onset of psychosis,15,16 while other findings suggest that deficits improve.17 These inconsistent findings may be attributed to variability in cohort inclusion criteria, duration of untreated psychosis, and utilization of large cognitive assessment batteries, where measures of performance reflect multiple cognitive subprocesses and summaries of the same may mask intra- and inter-individual heterogeneity. Others have suggested that some cognitive deficits are state dependent and therefore fluctuate with psychopathology,12 while other deficits reflect a stable, possibly neurodevelopmental, condition.18 For example, Cornblatt and colleagues19 suggest that cognitive deficits may be developmentally differentiable, with some deficits specifically predicting psychosis, while others form a more general vulnerability core for psychiatric illness and related disability.12
Overall, research to date suggests that at least some deficits are trait-related and are unlikely to be explained by the effects of medication or the accumulation of toxic biopsychosocial sequelae of illness progression. Such deficits may be considered “de facto” biological markers for illness onset, although they may not necessarily occur in people with the illness.
The Importance of Prediction
Recent studies have emphasized the need for early detection and treatment of the prodrome in order to avoid a full psychotic episode, to weight treatment resources toward those at greater risk for more severe illness, to reduce symptoms, including comorbidity such as depression and substance use,20–23 and to improve outcome.24–26 In addition, early detection may shorten the overtly psychotic period, help establish good therapeutic alliance, and improve quality of life and long-term outcome by minimizing severity and disability.24,27,28 Finally, such paradigms allow the prospective study of the transition process.21–23
Overview of Approaches and Rationale for Limits of Current Review
Several approaches address the nature of cognitive deficits prior to illness onset and whether such deficits are predictive of either transition to psychosis or functional outcome. One approach is the genetic high-risk approach, in which cognitive functioning of family members of individuals with psychosis, usually offspring,29–32 or individuals with psychosis spectrum disorders, eg schizotaxia33 or schizotypy,34 is assessed. While this strategy arguably focuses on a core potential biological marker for schizophrenia, sample representativeness of the population who present for first-episode psychosis is limited, as not all people with a genetic risk for schizophrenia develop the illness35 and as little as 10% of first-episode psychosis cohorts have a positive family history.
However, such approaches do provide clues regarding domains of cognition that may be compromised premorbidly as either a general or potentially more specific vulnerability marker, while recognizing that neither is necessary or sufficient for psychosis onset. Apart from findings on generalized batteries of tests (eg, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children [WISC],36 Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test37),29,38–40 relatively more specific domains of vulnerability include visuo-motor development,41,42 perceptual problems, conceptual sorting, language processing, and possibly mental tracking, sustained attention, short-term memory, working memory, and verbal recognition and recall.38,43–48 These deficits are moderate,49 and in cross-sectional studies they have been reported to be greater in relatives with a higher genetic loading.50,51 Finally, these cognitive domains remain relatively complex; verbal recognition, for example, could be parsed into relatively discrete subprocesses, including sustained attention, verbal working memory, and efficient storage and retrieval, all or some of which may be differentially compromised.
Another approach to identifying high-risk cohorts is the retrospective study of patients with established psychosis, which also provides clues regarding the nature of more general cognitive deficits many years prior to the onset of illness. Findings suggest compromised general IQ was already present during childhood,52 though this was minimal and had poor predictive validity. However, significant linear increases in risk for adult schizophrenia across decreasing tertiles of the distributions of general intellectual functioning at the 11- and 15-year assessments have been reported.53,54 Similar results from other studies suggest low educational achievement is common,55,56 along with language disturbance.57 However, as with genetic high-risk studies, these retrospective approaches are limited in that they generally allow no specific, a priori, hypothesis-driven paradigms regarding the nature and timing of onset of cognitive deficits or, indeed, of relatively more specific domains of cognitive function.
A further strategy is to follow large birth cohorts over time, which invariably incorporates early, broad assessment of individuals who eventually develop schizophreniform disorders (eg, the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study40,58). However, such approaches are inefficient, particularly regarding the length of follow-up period and the degree of assessment detail that can be applied to test formal hypotheses. Moreover, the number of individuals who are identified with schizophrenia is small relative to the degree of resources invested in screening large numbers of people.59
Despite these limitations, general domains of cognition that have been found to be vulnerable early in the development of people who eventually develop schizophrenia include nonverbal and verbal educational achievement and organizational and reading ability.15,35,60–62 These domains include relatively more specific problems in mathematical and vocabulary skills and in mechanical knowledge.52,61,62
More recent approaches of identifying premorbid cohorts have focused on positive symptomseg,22 or the German approach of self-recognized “basic symptoms”63 (for review21,64). Strategies predominantly focus on “help-seeking” adolescents, who by definition may already be manifesting established signs of attenuated onset of psychosis, as these state-based criteria are thought to identify an at-risk mental state.65,66 These approaches reflect a “close-in” or “multiple gate screening” approach to identifying a high-risk cohort,67 where an individual must meet a number of criteria to be included in the high-risk group. These incorporate behavioral difficulties in adolescence, in an attempt to improve the accuracy of identifying the high-risk group further. However, inclusion criteria do not yet incorporate factors reflecting cognitive risk specifically.
Studies using this approach have been referred to as “ultra-high risk” (UHR) or clinical high-risk studies to differentiate them from traditional genetic high-risk studies that rely on family history as the primary inclusion criterion. The terms “at-risk mental state” or “ultra-high risk” do not imply that a full-threshold psychotic illness, such as schizophrenia, is inevitable but suggest that an individual is at increased risk of developing a psychotic disorder by virtue of his or her mental state. These risks arguably reflect a more representative sample of the first-episode cohort than the studies described above that rely solely on genetic risk. All studies described below are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1.
Program Investigators | N | Age | Clinical High-Risk Subject Group: Diagnostic and Risk Characteristics | Follow-up Duration/Transition Rate | Cognitive Assessments | Cognitive Battery Raw Scores | Main Findings |
PACE (Yung et al. 1996) | |||||||
Brewer et al. 1996 (Abs)69 | UHR = 17 CTL = 22 |
N/A | N/A | Baseline only | COGMAPS UPSIT |
N/A | Deficits in olfactory identification |
Brewer et al. 1998 (Abs)70 | UHR = 65 CTL = 24 |
N/A | N/A | 18/12 follow-up / 21 (32%) UHR-P | COGMAPS UPSIT |
N/A | Deficits in Current IQ, attention, memory, and executive function, and olfactory identification at baseline; Mental arithmetic poorer in UHR-P compared to UHR-NP |
Brewer et al 200374 | UHR = 81 CTL = 31 |
19.9 (4.1) P 20.4 (3.2) NP 21.1 (3.9) 14–30 years |
Attenuated = 48.1% BLIPS = 11.1% T&S = 13.6% Attenuated/BLIP = 1.2% Attenuated/T&S = 11.1% BLIPS/T&S = 12.3% All 3 = 2.5% Remaining 10 UHR-P not Scz: Dep with psychotic features (n = 2); Schizaff- Dep (n = 1); Bipolar (n = 3); Psychotic disorder NOS (n = 3); Substance- induced psychosis (n = 1) |
18/12 follow-up / 22 (27.2%) UHR-P, of whom 12 developed Scz | NART UPSIT |
NART IQ: UHR-P = 99.9 (Scz) = 96.8 (Spectrum) = 103.6 UHR-NP = 100.5 CTL = 108.5 UPSIT: UHR-P = 31.2 (Scz) = 29.8 (Spectrum) = 32.9 UHR-NP = 32.2 CTL = 33.4 |
Deficit in olfactory identification in UHR-P |
Wood et al. 200378 | UHR = 38 CTL = 49 |
18.3 (3.2) P 19.7 (2.8) NP 20.3 (2.7) 14–30 years |
Attenuated = 36.8% BLIPS = 2.6% T&S = 26.3% Attenuated/BLIPS = 2.6% Attenuated/T&S = 31.6% |
12-24/12 follow-up / 9 (23.7%) UHR-P | NART CANTAB |
NART IQ: UHR-P = 92.8 UHR-NP = 101.7 CTL = 100.3 |
UHR impaired on Spatial Span, SWM, and DMTS; NS trend for UHR-P poorer on SWM; WM linked to negative symptoms in UHR group generally |
Brewer et al. 200579 | UHR = 98 CTL = 37 |
19.4 (4.0) P 20.0 (3.6) NP 20.7 (4.3) 15–29 years |
Attenuated = 44.9% BLIPS = 13.3% T&S = 15.3% Attenuated/BLIPS = 1.0% Attenuated/T&S = 11.2% BLIPS/T&S = 11.2% All 3 = 3.1% Remaining 16 UHR-P not Scz: Dep with psychotic features (n = 5); Schizaff (n = 1); Bipolar (n = 4); Psychotic disorder NOS (n = 3); Substance-induced psychosis (n = 1); Brief psychotic disorder (n = 2) UHR-NP: Dep (n = 4); GAD (n = 2); OCD (n = 1); Social phobia (n = 2); Dysthymia (n = 2); Adjustment disorder (n = 1); PTSD (n = 1) |
12/12→follow-up / 34 (34.7%) UHR-P WMS-R Vis Rep |
NART 7 Subtest WAIS-R WMS-R VMI 3-trial RAVLT Trails A & B COWAT Stroop118 |
NART IQ: UHR-P = 99.3 UHR-NP = 100.4 CTL = 108.1 WAIS-R VIQ: UHR-P = 99.2 UHR-NP = 98.5 CTL = 103.8 WAIS-R PIQ: UHR-P = 100.6 UHR-NP = 102.4 CTL = 111.6 WMS-R Vis Rep Raw: UHR-P = 32.1 UHR-NP = 35.3 CTL = 36.0 WMS-R VMI: UHR-P = 82.2 UHR-NP = 91.1 CTL = 96.7 RAVLT 3-trials: UHR-P = 9.6 UHR-NP = 9.6 CTL = 9.6 Trails A/B Seconds: UHR-P = 26.8/70.3 UHR-NP = 28.0/70.8 CTL = 23.4/61.0 COWAT: UHR-P = 35.4 UHR-NP = 37.2 CTL = 33.7 Stroop interference D/B: UHR-P = 0.46 UHR-NP = 0.49 CTL = 0.46 |
UHR deficits: VIQ, Block Design, Vis Rep UHR-P deficits: VMI (Logical Memory), Vis Rep UHR-NP deficits: Digit Symbol UHR did not improve in IQ between premorbid and current as did CTL |
Francey et al. 200581 | UHR = 70 CTL = 51 |
20.9 P 19.9 NP 23.3 14–30 years |
Attenuated = 42.9% BLIPS = 15.7% T&S = 14.3% Attenuated/BLIPS = 14.3% Attenuated/T&S = 12.9% Remaining 12 UHR-P not Scz: Dep with psychotic features (n = 3); Schizaff (n = 1); Bipolar (n = 3); Psychotic disorder NOS (n = 3); Substance-induced psychosis (n = 1); Brief psychotic disorder (n = 1) UHR-NP: None (n = 28) Dep (n = 4); GAD (n = 1); Panic disorder (n = 2); Social phobia (n = 1); Dysthymia (n = 4); Adjustment disorder (n = 1) |
12/12 follow-up / 20 (28.6%) UHR-P (n = 21 UHR received low-dose neuroleptics and therapy) |
NART CPT-IP |
NART IQ N/A CPT-IP Raw Scores N/A |
UHR CPT deficits compared to CTL; however, of those who developed psychosis, there were no differences to those who did not develop psychosis. |
Koutsouradis et al. 200586 | UHR = 16 CTL = 17 |
N/A | N/A | 12-18/12 follow-up / 7 (43.8%) UHR-P | COGMAPS | N/A | Visual reproduction, verbal fluency, and Trails B all showed significant declines over the transition to psychosis, while cognitive performance for UHR-NP group remained stable or improved |
RAP | |||||||
Lencz et al. 200512 | CHR = 38 CTL = 39 |
16.5 (2.2) 15.8 (2.6) |
n = 23 (60.5%) neuroleptic-free | 6/12→follow-up / 12 (31.6%) CHR-P | WRAT-III WISC-III/WAIS-R Vocab Block Design Digit Span WMS-R Log Mem I/II Vis Rep I/II CVLT WCST COWAT TRAILS A/B Ruff Figural Fluency119 Letter-Number Span CPT-IP Finger Tap120 Groove Pegboard121 Line Judgment122 Boston Naming Test123 |
WRAT IQ: UHR = 101.0 CTL = 108.0 WAIS-R FSIQ (Prorated): UHR = 97.5 CTL = 110.0 Further individual scores not reported Cognitive Domains: (z-score deficits compared to CTL): Cognitive Domains: (z-score deficits compared to CTL): Verbal Memory z = 1.8 Executive/WM z = 1.6 Language z = 1.3 Motor z = 1.2 Attention z = 1.0 Visuo-spatial z = 0.75 Verbal Memory: CHR-P z = 2.8 CHR-NP z = 1.2 CPT Raw Scores N/A |
CHR impaired on global cognition, along with verbal memory and executive function/working memory, while visuo-spatial relatively spared; CHR-P had lower verbal memory at baseline. |
Smith et al. 200694 | CHR = 8 CTL = 10 |
16.3 (2.6) 16.6 (2.9) |
Comorbidity: Anxiety (n = 4); Dep (n = 2); ADHD (n = 2) 10 low-risk CTL (1 year extra education). |
Baseline only | WISC-III/WAIS-R Vocab Block Design Computerized SWM task |
Prorated IQ: CHR = 108.0 CTL = 111.9 |
SWM deficits in CHR but not on a non-WM-demanding spatial control task. |
PRIME | |||||||
Hawkins et al. 200498 | CHR = 36 CTL = comparable published norms from Test Manuals and Goldberg's124 discordant twin samples |
19.8 (4.7) 16–45 years |
Not reported | Not reported | WAIS-R 4 subtest CPT-IP 450 Letter/Number Sequencing Dot location Trails A/B Stroop Finger tapping CVLT WMS-R Vis Rep I/II COWAT Ruff Figural Fluency |
WAIS-R Vocab SS: UHR = 10.8 Norm = 10.0 WAIS-R Info SS: UHR = 9.8 Norm = 10.0 WAIS-R Block Design SS: UHR = 9.8 Norm = 10.0 WAIS-R Digit Symbol SS: UHR = 8.9 Norm = 10.0 WMS-R Vis Rep I Raw Score: UHR = 33.0 Norm = 34.0 WMS-R Vis Rep II Raw Score: UHR = 31.9 Norm = 31.5 CVLT: UHR = 50.5 Norm = 55.9 Trails A/B Seconds: UHR = 28.6/76.5 Norm = 26.6/54.3 COWAT: UHR = 32.5 Norm = 43.7 Stroop: UHR = 39.8 Norm = 49.8 Letter/Number Sequencing: UHR = 13.8 Norm = 15.7 Dot Location: UHR = 0.63 Norm = 1.27 Ruff Figural Fluency: UHR = 83.7 Norm = 107.5 CPT 450 D': UHR = 1.2 Norm = 0.8 |
UHR poorer on digit symbol, Vocab, CPT, Letter/Number Sequencing, Dot Location, Trails B, CVLT, COWAT, and Figural Fluency; normal on Trails A, WMS-R Vis Rep I/II, and CVLT Total Recall. |
FEPSY | |||||||
Gschwandtner et al. 200399 | CHR = 32 CTL = 32 |
26.5 (8.8) 25.5 (4.4) |
Not reported | Baseline only | WCST Twr of Hanoi125 TAP CPT |
WCST Perseverations (%): CHR = 26.6 CTL = 15.6 Twr of Hanoi (seconds): CHR = 468 CTL = 299 TAP GoNoGo R/T (ms): CHR = 585 CTL = 488 TAP WM R/T (ms): CHR = 784 CTL = 550 TAP WM (missed): CHR = 4.0 CTL = 1.3 CPT R Time (ms): CHR = 494 CTL = 399 |
UHR higher perseveration and prolonged reaction times in Twr of Hanoi; GoNoGo, WM, and CPT reaction also deficit. |
Gschwandtner et al. 2005100 | CHR = 40 CTL = 42 |
27.4 (9.1) 25.9 (5.2) |
Not reported | Baseline only | WCST Twr of Hanoi TAP CPT |
Verbal IQ: CHR = 105.2 CTL = 119.0 Nonverbal IQ: CHR = 111.1 CTL = 118.8 WCST Perseverations: CHR = −0.08 CTL = 0.23 WCST Perseverative Error: CHR = −0.13 CTL = 0.31 Twr of Hanoi (moves): CHR = −0.05 CTL = −0.09 TAP WM False Alarm: CHR = −0.34 CTL = 0.34 TAP WM Missing: CHR = −0.44 CTL = 0.39 TAP GoNoGo False Alarm: CHR = −0.24 CTL = 0.13 TAP GoNoGo Missing: CHR = −0.38 CTL = 0.12 CPT Missing: CHR = −0.32 CTL = 0.34 CPT False Alarm: CHR = −0.39 CTL = 0.38 |
UHR deficits in sustained attention, WM, and perseveration. |
PAS | |||||||
Silverstein et al. 2006107 | UHR = 70 CTL = 24 |
17.4 (3.6) 20.7 (4.4) |
Trait = GAF drop of 30 points (n = 11); Attenuated (n = 38); BLIPS (n = 21) | 18-24/12 follow-up / 49 assessed, of whom 24 (49.0%) UHR-P | NART Computerized Perceptual Organization Task |
No differences between UHR and CTL | |
Schall et al. 2003109 | UHR = 103 | N/A | 12-36/12 follow-up / 62 (55.3%) of original UHR (n = 112) UHR-P | WCST Stroop Trails B Verbal Recall |
N/A | UHR higher error rates on WCST, Stroop, and Trails B; UHR-P poorer in Verbal Memory at baseline | |
CARE | |||||||
Shafer et al. 2003 | UHR = 27 CTL = 17 |
N/A | N/A | Baseline only | CPT-IP | N/A | UHR attentional deficits intermediate to CTL and FEP |
University of Drebecen | |||||||
Bartok et al. 200524 | CHR = 11 | 25.0 (5.0) 19–40 years |
12/12 follow-up / 9 (81.82%) CHR-P | CANTAB | CHR impaired on PAL, SRM, RVP, and SWM compared to CTL; No difference between UHR-P and UHR-NP | ||
FETZ | |||||||
Hambrecht et al. 2002112 | CHR = 29 CTL = 29 |
23.1 (4.4) 24.0 (3.0) 15–31 years |
Not reported | 15/12 follow-up / 5 (9.8%) of larger cohort (n = 51), 29 of which received cognitive assessment | Matched by Verbal IQ, measured by 37 series of 4 nonwords, and 1 word has to be identified Visual BM CPT- IP SWM (DR) Task RAVLT COWAT (+ category) Rey Figure WCST Perseverative Errors; |
Visual BM (% hits): UHR = 84.7 CTL = 86.9 Attention (% hits): UHR = 72.7 CTL = 81.5 RAVLT: UHR = 11.2 CTL = 12.1 Recognition: UHR = 14.0 CTL = 14.1 COWAT: UHR = 17.6 CTL = 21.6 Visual Recall (Rey copy less delay): UHR = 12.4 CTL = 8.9 WCST (% perseveration): UHR = 11.4 CTL = 10.8 |
Self perceived deficits in perception, cognition, and stress reactivity; CHR worse on verbal recall, verbal fluency, attention, and visual memory, though only fluency after Bonferroni corrections; CTL had higher premorbid IQ |
Note: Abbreviations—Abs: Abstract; ADHD: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; BLIPS: Brief Limited Intermittent Psychotic Symptoms22; BM: Backward Masking; CANTAB: Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery77; CARE: Cognitive Assessment and Risk Evaluation Program, University of California, San Diego; CHR: Clinical High Risk; CHR-NP: Clinical High Risk-Non Psychotic; CHR-P: Clinical High Risk-Psychotic; COGMAPS: Cognitively Graded Mental Health Research Institute Assessment Protocol for Schizophrenia72; COWAT: Controlled Oral Word Association Test87; CPT-IP: Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs version126; CTL: Control Group; CVLT: California Verbal Learning Test127; Dep: Depression; DR: Delayed Response (CANTAB); DMTS: Delayed Matching to Sample; FEP: First-Episode Psychosis; FEPSY: Früherkennung von Psychosen, Basel, Germany; FETZ: Früherkennungs- und Therapiezentrum für psychotische Krisen; FSIQ: Full Scale Intelligence Quotient; GAD: Generalised Anxiety Disorder; GAF: Global Assessment Form; Log Mem: Logical Memory subtest from the WMS-R80; ms: milliseconds; N/A: Not available; NART: National Adult Reading Test128; NOS: Not otherwise specified; NS: Non-significant; OCD: Obessessive Compulsive Disorder; PACE: Personal Assessment and Crisis Evaluation Clinic, Melbourne, Australia; PAS: Psychological Assistance Service, Newcastle, Australia; PAL: Paired Associate Learning (WMS-R); PIQ: Performance IQ (WAIS); PRIME: Prevention through Risk Identification, Management, and Education Clinic, Yale University; PTSD: Post Traumatic Stress Disorder; R: Revised; RAP: Recognition and Prevention Program, New York; RAVLT: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test129,130; R/T: Reaction Time; RVP: Rapid Visual Processing (CANTAB); Scz: Schizophrenia; Schizaff: Schizo Affective Disorder; SRM: Spatial Recognition Memory (CANTAB); SS: Scaled Score; SWM: Spatial Working Memory (CANTAB); TAP: Testbattreie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprufung103; T&S: Trait/State; Twr: Tower; UHR: Ultra-High Risk; UHR-NP: Ultra-High Risk—Nonpsychotic; UHR-P: Ultra-High Risk—Psychotic transition; UPSIT: University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test73; Vis Rep: Visual Reproduction subtest (WMS-R); VIQ: Verbal IQ (WAIS)131; VMI: Verbal Memory Index (WMS-R); WAIS: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale131; WAIS-R: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised; WCST: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test102; WISC: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children36; WM: Working Memory; WMS-R: Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised80; WRAT: Wide Range Achievement Test132
The Personal Assessment and Crisis Evaluation (PACE) Clinic
The Personal Assessment and Crisis Evaluation (PACE) Clinic, established in Melbourne, Australia, in 1994, was the first to specifically target adolescents in a putative prodromal phase.22 The strategy focuses on individuals in the peak age range of risk for onset of psychotic disorder—adolescents and young adults.68 In addition, further criteria regarding potential risk factors developed by the PACE Clinic include attenuated psychotic symptoms, self-resolving psychotic symptoms, a trait risk factor (a schizotypal personality disorder in the young person or a family history of a psychotic disorder in a first-degree relative), and functional decline.
Early reports of cognitive ability from this group demonstrated performance profiles intermediate to control and first-episode psychosis cohorts across domains of attention, memory, and executive ability.69–71 These approaches utilized general neuropsychological assessment protocols (Cognitively Graded Mental Health Research Institute Assessment Protocol for Schizophrenia [COGMAPS]72). Hypothesis testing concerning relatively more specific cognitive processes was also incorporated into the baseline assessment protocol of this cohort, where it was predicted that olfactory identification (utilizing the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test [UPSIT]73) deficits, implicating neurodevelopmental compromise of prefrontal (orbitofrontal) neural regions, would be present prior to illness onset. Olfactory identification function of 81 UHR individuals was compared with the functioning of 31 healthy comparison subjects.74 Twelve of the UHR cohort were diagnosed with a schizophrenia spectrum disorder within 12 months of baseline assessment. Olfactory identification deficits (OID) were specific to the UHR subgroup who developed schizophrenia relative to the healthy comparison group nonpsychotic UHR subgroup, as well as UHR individuals who developed a psychotic disorder that was not within the schizophrenia spectrum (eg, bipolar disorder or substance-induced psychosis). This finding suggests that compromised OID may be a premorbid marker of transition to schizophrenia but is not predictive of psychosis generally. Further, findings of OID have previously been associated with negative symptoms,71,75 suggesting that reliance on precursors for negative symptoms rather than positive symptoms alone may be a useful focus for inclusion criteria to clinical high-risk paradigms. Current work is underway to confirm the hypotheses that OID in this cohort reflects a neurodevelopmental lag of prefrontal aspects of limbic-prefrontal pathways, that OID is likely to be more associated with emerging negative symptoms and treatment resistance and, further, that OID is likely to reflect stable trait markers.75,76 Finally, we are working on parsing olfactory identification ability further into sensation, detection, and identification, for example, to examine even more discrete neuropsychological subprocesses that are involved in successful performance on this task.76
Utilization of more experimental challenges of relatively specific components of cognition, as measured by the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB77) was reflected in the paradigm reported by Wood and colleagues.78 Here, working memory (WM) was examined in 38 UHR PACE patients, of whom 9 later developed psychosis. These were compared with 49 healthy control subjects. Both spatial working memory (SWM) and delayed matching-to-sample performance were significantly poorer in the UHR group relative to controls. The subgroup that later developed psychosis also performed more poorly than those who did not, although for the most part these differences were nonsignificant. However, consistent with the olfactory findings above, a significant association between SWM errors and negative symptoms was seen in the later psychotic group. This suggests that the relationship between measures of prefrontal function and negative symptoms may be a useful predictive tool.
In the largest study of UHR cohorts conducted to date, Brewer and colleagues79 examined 98 UHR young people, of whom 34 later developed psychosis, compared with 37 healthy controls. A striking finding was the overall lack of clinically significant cognitive impairment in the UHR group, suggesting that global cognitive deficits found after psychosis onset may not be key features of premorbid vulnerability. However, UHR subjects did have significantly lower Performance IQ than the comparison subjects, along with lower premorbid functioning and aspects of visual and verbal learning. Further impairments specific to the UHR patients who developed psychosis were also found in the visual reproduction subtest and the verbal memory index of the Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised (WMS-R).80 This last impairment was completely explained by lower Logical Memory scores, with no deficit in Paired Associated Learning. No other memory, attentional, or executive tasks discriminated between any of the groups, and there were no differences in performance related to different psychotic diagnoses. The findings suggest that visuo-spatial processing impairment and some prefrontally mediated, rather than medial-temporal-mediated, memory deficits were apparent before the full expression of psychotic illness. Cognitive performance on more complex tasks requiring rapid registration and efficient recall may be compromised before the development of psychosis; however, further examination of the subprocesses involved in performance of these tasks is required.
These findings contrast with those of Francey and colleagues,81 who examined attention with the Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs version (CPT-IP)82 in 70 UHR patients relative to 51 normal control subjects and 32 first-episode psychosis (FEP) patients. While the UHR group exhibited performance deficits relative to the comparison group (similar to the FEP group), those who developed psychosis did not differ from those who did not. These results suggest that sustained attention was an indicator of vulnerability for psychosis but did not predict transition. Consistent with this notion, impaired performance on the CPT-IP has been found in people with schizophrenia, their first-degree relatives, and people with schizophrenia spectrum personality disorders.83–85
In a small longitudinal study of 16 UHR patients (7 of whom developed psychosis), Koutsouradis and colleagues86 found that visual reproduction scores from the WMS-R, verbal fluency (Controlled Oral Word Association Test [COWAT]87), and Trail Making Test Form B88 all showed significant declines over the transition to psychosis, while cognitive performance for the nonpsychotic UHR group remained stable or improved. These data indicate that the onset of psychotic disorder is associated with additional impairment in visuo-spatial and executive abilities.
As highlighted in the approach of genetic high-risk cohorts above, use of subtests from IQ and memory batteries arguably involve a number of cognitive subdomains or elements; in the case of the Logical Memory subtest from the WMS-R, sustained attention, strategic processing, verbal working memory, and retrieval are relatively more discrete cognitive processes that may be challenged for successful performance.89,90 Experimental paradigms that parse these processes more clearly to determine the relative weighting that each process holds for successful performance are recommended. Results from such experimental paradigms that utilize nonstandardized tasks also need to be reported in the context of more general IQ and memory batteries in order to allow standardized comparability of patient cohorts across centers. Further, this approach acknowledges that variation in general IQ summaries (premorbid and current), which in the normal population are usually also related to memory ability, no doubt mask individual performance differences on subtests of general batteries or, further, confound performance on experimental cognitive tasks that are designed to challenge parsed components of subtests/indexes such as “verbal memory.”
In summary, the findings from the PACE UHR group are consistent with findings from the genetic high-risk paradigms. Results from large batteries of standardized tests indicate that UHR patients perform at a level that is intermediate to that displayed by FEP and control samples. In addition, decomposing generalized tests of cognitive function into relatively more specific cognitive abilities such as olfactory identification and spatial working memory may be useful trait markers specific to the schizophrenia diagnosis per se, although further longitudinal follow-up of this cohort is required. It is feasible that potential trait markers, such as deficits in sustained attention, are confounds within assessments that utilize generalized or more neuropsychologically “blunt” cognitive batteries, and this should be considered in future experimental designs.
Recognition and Prevention (RAP) Program
The Recognition and Prevention (RAP) Program further elaborated the PACE ultra-high risk criteria for identifying young people thought to be at high risk of psychosis.19 Adolescents between the ages of 12 and 22 (with a primary focus on ages 14–18) are recruited to this program if they are seeking treatment for attenuated positive psychotic symptoms (referred to as the clinical high risk—positive group [CHR-P]) or if they display specific combinations of cognitive, academic, and social impairments and disorganization/odd behavior. This second cohort is referred to as the clinical high risk—negative group (CHR-N). It is hypothesized that the developmental course of schizophrenia follows a progression from CHR-N to CHR-P to “schizophrenia-like psychosis” (essentially schizophreniform/brief psychotic disorder) to schizophrenia. The transition rate from CHR-P status to psychotic disorder, using both the PACE and Prevention through Risk Identification, Management, and Education (PRIME; see below) clinics' definitions of psychosis, was 26.5% (9 of 34 patients) within 6 months,91 a rate similar to the 6-month transition rate in the PACE Clinic.66 The transition rate to schizophrenia in the schizophrenia-like psychosis group was 33%.92 This approach of further refining and characterizing subclasses of CHR states facilitates the implementation of more specific hypothesis-driven experimental paradigms, particularly in the interests of predicting relatively discrete or parsed cognitive processes that may be differentially related to CHR subclasses.
A further advantage of the RAP program is that it has focused specifically on cognitive impairments that may precede the onset of acute psychosis.12,92,93 Lencz and colleagues12 examined the performance on a neuropsychological battery of 38 clinical HR patients and 39 age- and sex-matched healthy controls. Clinical follow-up of at least 6 months duration was available on 33 patients, of whom 12 had developed nonaffective psychotic disorders. Similar to the findings of Brewer et al.79 discussed above, the CHR patients showed significant impairments of global intelligence, although these were not large enough to be clinically meaningful. Furthermore, measures of verbal memory and executive functioning/working memory were generally more impaired, while visuo-spatial functioning was relatively spared. High-risk patients who later developed psychosis had significantly lower verbal memory scores at baseline compared with those patients who did not. These findings suggest that verbal memory deficits (assessed using nonhippocampal specific tasks, such as story recall and list learning) might be an important risk factor for the development of schizophrenia spectrum psychotic disorders, possibly implicating prefrontal-hippocampal neurodevelopmental abnormality.
In a small study of spatial working memory in this CHR cohort (using the Delayed Response Task), Smith et al.94 compared 8 patients with 10 controls matched for age, gender, education, IQ, and socioeconomic status. Consistent with findings from the PACE clinic,78 the CHR group was impaired on this task. This task is a good example of attempts to parse more generalized cognitive abilities into subcomponent processes; however, the lack of accompanying general IQ or memory data limits the comparability of this cohort to those of other centers. There is also no context for determining the functional significance of the results.
Prevention through Risk Identification, Management, and Education (PRIME) Clinic
The ultra-high-risk concept and ‘close-in’ strategy has also been adopted at Yale University's Prevention through Risk Identification, Management, and Education (PRIME) Clinic, which began in 1997. Young people between 12 and 45 years old who are thought to be experiencing the onset phase of a first psychotic episode are recruited to the clinic. The PRIME group has coined the term Criteria of Prodromal Syndromes, or COPS, to describe its intake criteria, which are very similar to the ultra-high risk criteria developed earlier at PACE. The PRIME group has also developed a semistructured interview, originally influenced by the Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States (CAARMS)95 from the PACE clinic and the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS),96 called the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes (SIPS).97 Although it has been suggested that this approach is the most specific and methodologically stringent in the field so far,64 the high number of refusers (86%) and the severity of symptoms in those who participate in the research indicate that the sample is more ill (although not more likely to become psychotic) than samples from other centers. Only 1 study of cognition has been reported by this group,98 which assessed 36 UHR patients on a broad neuropsychological battery. Although there was no control group (comparisons were with population norms), the results suggest that the UHR group were performing below population norms.
Früherkennung von Psychosen (FEPSY)
The Basel Early Detection of Psychosis Study was initiated in 1999 and has yet to report the operationalization of the at-risk state. In 2 overlapping reports,99,100 data from 32 UHR and 32 control subjects (increased to 40 and 42 in the later paper) were reported. In the earlier (smaller subgroup) study, UHR patients performed significantly worse on measures of executive function (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [WCST]101,102), CPT (but it is unclear whether this is the CPT-IP), and Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprufung (TAP,103 a test of working memory). However, no transition rates are given (even in the later paper), and the members of the UHR group are much older than those seen by PACE, PRIME, or RAP (average age). In the later study, data were no longer reported as means, and instead z-scores were presented, making direct comparison difficult. However, it appears that the results from the former study were maintained. Utilization of tasks such as the WCST traditionally may reflect “prefrontal” functioning; however, the number of cognitive subprocesses required to perform this task suggests that it remains a relatively blunt probe of prefrontal functioning, particularly as hippocampal integrity may also be challenged during performance on this task.104
Psychological Assistance Service (PAS)
In 1997 the Psychological Assistance Service (PAS) opened in Newcastle, Australia, as a clinical service for the assessment and treatment of young people at high risk of psychosis and those experiencing a first psychotic episode. The high-risk criteria are based on those of PACE but also allow inclusion if a young person has a second-degree relative with a history of psychotic disorder in conjunction with a significant decline in functioning.105 The transition rate to psychosis after a 12-month period was 50%.106
In a further study,107 70 UHR patients were compared with FEP patients and 24 comparable, subjects using an adapted task with known sensitivity to perceptual organization deficits in schizophrenia (adapted from108) and whose scores have predicted long-term outcome and disorganized symptomatology. There was no difference between the groups on this task. The data were found to be consistent with the notion that preattentive processes in schizophrenia are not impaired and that perceptual organization problems are not a general feature of the UHR group. A further unpublished study reporting data from a larger cognitive battery suggests that conversion to schizophrenia was predicted by impairments in verbal memory and executive function.109
Cognitive Assessment and Risk Evaluation (CARE) Program
The Cognitive Assessment and Risk Evaluation (CARE) program at the University of California, San Diego, was established in 2000, and only preliminary information is available.110 However, the criteria used are comparable to other UHR studies, and the research includes a control group. Unpublished data from this study generally support the findings of Francey and colleagues81—that UHR patients have some impairment of attentional processing intermediate to controls and first-episode psychosis patients.
University of Debrecen
Bartok and colleagues24 assessed young Hungarian adults referred to an outpatient clinic of the Department of Psychiatry at the University of Debrecen. They were referred for secondary care by general practitioners after help-seeking for nonspecific emotional or behavioral abnormalities. All had specific, isolated, mild, nonaffective psychotic symptoms and were diagnosed as being in the pre-psychotic phase following International Classification of Diseases ICD-10 based psychiatric interview (9 developed psychosis by the 12-month follow-up). However, this cohort is not adequately characterized by objective measures of psychosis threshold or entry criteria. Results from baseline assessment utilizing the CANTAB suggested that prepsychotic patients had deficits in attentional, frontal, and prefrontal cognitive function, when compared with CANTAB normative data from the United Kingdom (n = 3000), although examination of a local control group would be useful in extending our understanding of these findings.
Basic Symptoms: An Alternative Approach to Clinical High Risk
The German approach to early detection and intervention in schizophrenia follows the long-standing observation that cognitive, affective, and social disturbances often occur years before the first psychotic episode and are often recognized by the person affected at this early stage. For example, social deficits appeared as early as 2–4 years before the first hospital admission and, thus, in the prodromal phase about a year before the onset of the first psychotic symptom.111 A new instrument to assess basic symptoms was recently developed—the Schizophrenia Prediction Instrument, Adult version (SPI-A). Preliminary results have indicated that 25 of 147 individuals (17%) who have reported experiencing at least 1 basic symptom developed schizophrenia within an average of 12 months of assessment (SD = 7.6; range 2–33)21. Although preliminary, this result reveals that the basic symptom approach is compatible with short follow-up intervals.
As 20% of individuals who report experiencing basic symptoms do not also report experiencing subthreshold psychotic symptoms, it is suggested that basic symptoms either describe an additional at-risk group to that identified using ultra-high risk criteria or—more likely—that basic symptoms occur earlier in the course of the illness.21
Früherkennungs- und Therapiezentrum für psychotische Krisen (FETZ)
Hambrecht and colleagues112 examined the role of subjective and objective neuropsychological deficits in 29 basic symptoms patients and compared them with 29 healthy control subjects. These groups differed significantly in premorbid IQ, and the basic symptoms patients performed poorly on attention, verbal memory (free recall), verbal fluency, and visual memory tasks. No differences were found in visual backward masking, spatial working memory, verbal recognition memory, and WCST performance. However, when Bonferroni adjustments were made, verbal fluency was the only task indicating significant deficit. None of the cognitive variables were predictive.
General Discussion
Overall, clinical high-risk studies using the “close-in” approach are still in their infancy. No center has been operating for more than 12 years, and only the PACE clinic has published more than 2 studies. Nevertheless, early indications are that, depending on the matching characteristics of comparison groups and of the generalized test batteries utilized, CHR cohorts perform relatively normally, though intermediate in performance to normal controls and patients with first-episode psychosis. Further, they demonstrate specific areas of cognitive impairment prior to psychosis onset, where more discrete functions such as olfactory identification and spatial working memory may have predictive value. While some measures of verbal memory are also useful, they are neuropsychologically complex, and it is only those measures that require prefrontal processes for successful performance that appear to be predictive.
Comparison between centers is limited by the variability in the operationalization of inclusion criteria, although most use criteria based on those originally PACE criteria. Further refined characterization of clinical risk profiles can only advance our understanding of this transition period (eg, RAP and PAS). In contrast, characterization based on cognitive profiles is in its infancy, and no center has yet formally incorporated cognitive vulnerability into its inclusion criteria. Transition rates differ, as does the composition of sample subgroups based on clinical characteristics. Age at intake varies greatly between the UHR and the basic symptoms approach, although not in the expected direction (the mean age of the basic symptoms patients tends to be 8 or 9 years older than that of UHR patients). This has implications for interpretation, where a confounding factor that has not been adequately addressed to date concerns the likely incomplete maturation of executive function that parallels prefrontal development.76 Further, both the UHR and basic symptoms approaches rely on help-seeking people rather than a whole population, epidemiological approach. This limits the generalizability of the findings and makes comparison with genetic- and population-risk approaches difficult.
Genetic and birth cohort studies imply that cognitive abnormalities are present from early development, but the data reported here demonstrate that they are much more consequences of illness stage and progression relative to other vulnerability factors. Importantly, most UHR patients perform normally in many domains. Several studies have found that some schizophrenia patients can be characterized as neuropsychologically normal.113–115 However, others have suggested that, when examined more closely, such patients have either previously performed at a higher level and have thus deteriorated to average, normal, or functional IQ89,116 or they have isolated subtle impairments in executive and sensorimotor function against a background of normal performance on a wide range of other tasks.117 In either case, this suggests that in the UHR population, vulnerability may only be unmasked by high demand tasks, not routine tests. Finally, vulnerability does not always progress, so the key is to prevent vulnerability from progressing to transition and further deterioration (from early change to full decompensation).
Conclusions and Future Directions
High-risk research has revealed promising cognitive indicators of the future development of psychosis in the context of generally normal function. Future work needs to address the inconsistencies in intake criteria and to engage in longitudinal studies at predefined intervals. Furthermore, specific research hypotheses need to be generated that address core vulnerability markers, particularly those that involve parsing more complex cognitive processes into relatively discrete elements. General batteries are neuropsychologically dense, though they hold the advantage of obtaining reliable IQ measures or memory indexes, whereas experimental approaches that allow task performance on subtests of such batteries to be parsed into even more basic psychological processes allow refined appreciation of the neural abnormalities underlying the at-risk mental state.89 Likewise, popular tasks such as the WCST and the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure, which reflect more general domains of executive ability and which provide gross indications of ability in relatively broad cognitive domains, can also be parsed into subprocesses. As they stand, such tasks reflect relatively gross challenges of cognitive ability, where they could be supplemented by computerized tasks that parse subprocesses more clearly. The CANTAB and Delayed Response Paradigms represent superior experimental examples in this regard. In addition to reporting patient mean performance on cognitive tasks, more sophisticated analysis of potential subgroups of patients based on cognitive profiling is recommended, as normal performance in some patients may mask significant deficits in subgroups. Moreover, characterization of general current and premorbid cognitive IQs via utilization of reliable and valid test batteries such as the Wechsler scales should remain a standard approach. This not only affords a reliable point of comparison across centers but also provides a functional context for reports of nonstandardized experimental results of “parsed” cognitive processes, which are likely confounded by more general intellectual ability. Incorporation of matched control groups, particularly with regard to measures of premorbid IQ (including years of education) reflects superior experimental design, relative to reliance on published norms of popular neuropsychological tasks. Finally, as has historically occurred with symptom ratings, the use of cluster analysis and factor analytic techniques focused on cognitive scores to characterize potential subgroups of subjects would be useful. Cognitive profiles can then be more meaningfully associated with clinical profiles for the purpose of incorporating the same into inclusion criteria for clinical high risk for psychosis.
Acknowledgments
This research and the structure of the PACE Clinic has been supported by grants from the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia (IDs 145627, 350241, 970598, 970391, 350241), the Stanley Foundation, the Victorian Health Promotion Foundation, and an unrestricted research grant from Janssen-Cilag Pharmaceuticals. Dr Wood is supported by an NHMRC Clinical Career Development Award and a National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (NARSAD) Young Investigator Award.
References
- 1.Pantelis C, Maruff P. The cognitive neuropsychiatric approach to investigating the neurobiology of schizophrenia and other disorders. J Psychosom Res. 2002;53(2):655–664. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00434-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Saykin AJ, Gur RC, Gur RE, et al. Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48:618–624. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810310036007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hutton SB, Puri BK, Duncan LJ, Robbins TW, Barnes TR, Joyce EM. Executive function in first-episode schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1998;28:463–473. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797006041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology. 1998;12:426–445. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Nopoulos P, Flashman L, Flaum M, Arndt S, Andreasen N. Stability of cognitive functioning early in the course of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1994;14:29–37. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(94)90006-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bilder RM, Lipschutz-Broch L, Reiter G, Geisler S, Mayerhoff D, Lieberman JA. Neuropsychological deficits in the early course of first episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1991;5:198–199. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(91)90071-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bilder RM, Goldman RS, Robinson D, et al. Neuropsychology of first-episode schizophrenia: initial characterization and clinical correlates. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:549–559. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.4.549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Saykin AJ, Shtasel DL, Gur RE, et al. Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51:124–131. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950020048005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rund BR, Zeiner P, Sundet K, Oie M, Bryhn G. No vigilance deficit found in either young schizophrenic or ADHD subjects. Scand J Psychol. 1998;39(2):101–107. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lucas S, Fitzgerald D, Redoblado-Hodge MA, et al. Neuropsychological correlates of symptom profiles in first episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;71:323–330. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Cornblatt B, Obuchowski M, Schnur DB, O'Brien JD. Attention and clinical symptoms in schizophrenia. Psychiatr Q. 1997;68:343–359. doi: 10.1023/a:1025495030997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Lencz T, Smith CW, McLaughlin D, et al. Generalized and specific neurocognitive deficits in prodromal schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:863–871. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rhinewine JP, Lencz T, Thaden EP, et al. Neurocognitive profile in adolescents with early-onset schizophrenia: clinical correlates. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58:705–712. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hoff AL, Sakuma M, Wieneke M, Horon R, Kushner M, DeLisi LE. Longitudinal neuropsychological follow-up study of patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:1336–1341. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.9.1336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kremen WS, Buka SL, Seidman LJ, Goldstein JM, Koren D, Tsuang MT. IQ decline during childhood and adult psychotic symptoms in a community sample: a 19-year longitudinal study. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:672–677. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.5.672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Weickert TW, Goldberg TE, Gold JM, Bigelow LB, Egan MF, Weinberger DR. Cognitive impairments in patients with schizophrenia displaying preserved and compromised intellect. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:907–913. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.9.907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Addington J, Saeedi H, Addington D. The course of cognitive functioning in first episode psychosis: changes over time and impact on outcome. Schizophr Res. 2005;78:35–43. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Davies N, Russell A, Jones P, Murray RM. Which characteristics of schizophrenia predate psychosis? J Psychiatr Res. 1998;32(3–4):121–131. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(97)00027-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Cornblatt BA, Lencz T, Smith CW, Correll CU, Auther AM, Nakayama E. The schizophrenia prodrome revisited: a neurodevelopmental perspective. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29:633–651. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Good KP, Brewer WJ, Kopala LC. The benefits of incorporating research into clinical programs. In: Ehmann T, MacEwan GW, Honer WG, editors. Best Care in Early Psychosis Intervention: Global Perspectives. London: Taylor and Francis; 2004. pp. 175–182. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Phillips LJ, McGorry PD, Yung AR, McGlashan TH, Cornblatt B, Klosterkotter J. Prepsychotic phase of schizophrenia and related disorders: recent progress and future opportunities. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 2005;48:S33–44. doi: 10.1192/bjp.187.48.s33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Yung AR, McGorry PD. The prodromal phase of first-episode psychosis: past and current conceptualizations. Schizophr Bull. 1996;22:353–370. doi: 10.1093/schbul/22.2.353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Yung AR, McGorry PD, McFarlane CA, Jackson HJ, Patton GC, Rakkar A. Monitoring and care of young people at incipient risk of psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 1996;22:283–303. doi: 10.1093/schbul/22.2.283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bartok E, Berecz R, Glaub T, Degrell I. Cognitive functions in prepsychotic patients. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2005;29:621–625. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hafner H, Maurer K, Ruhrmann S, et al. Early detection and secondary prevention of psychosis: facts and visions. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2004;254(2):117–128. doi: 10.1007/s00406-004-0508-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ruhrmann S, Schultze-Lutter F, Klosterkotter J. Early detection and intervention in the initial prodromal phase of schizophrenia. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2003;36(suppl3):S162–167. doi: 10.1055/s-2003-45125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Corcoran C, Malaspina D, Hercher L. Prodromal interventions for schizophrenia vulnerability: the risks of being “at risk.”. Schizophr Res. 2005;73:173–184. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.05.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Larsen TK, Friis S, Haahr U, et al. Early detection and intervention in first-episode schizophrenia: a critical review. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2001;103:323–334. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2001.00131.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Byrne M, Hodges A, Grant E, Owens DC, Johnstone EC. Neuropsychological assessment of young people at high genetic risk for developing schizophrenia compared with controls: preliminary findings of the Edinburgh High Risk Study (EHRS) Psychol Med. 1999;29:1161–1173. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799001002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Cannon TD, Mednick SA. The schizophrenia high-risk project in Copenhagen: three decades of progress. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1993;370:33–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1993.tb05359.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Erlenmeyer-Kimling L, Squires-Wheeler E, Adamo UH, et al. The New York High-Risk Project: psychoses and cluster A personality disorders in offspring of schizophrenic parents at 23 years of follow-up. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52:857–865. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950220067013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Snitz BE, Macdonald AW, 3rd, Carter CS. Cognitive deficits in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: a meta-analytic review of putative endophenotypes. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32:179–194. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Tsuang MT, Stone WS, Tarbox SI, Faraone SV. An integration of schizophrenia with schizotypy: identification of schizotaxia and implications for research on treatment and prevention. Schizophr Res. 2002;54:169–175. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00364-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Cadenhead KS, Braff DL. Endophenotyping schizotypy: a prelude to genetic studies within the schizophrenia spectrum. Schizophr Res. 2002;54:47–57. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00351-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cannon TD, Bearden CE, Hollister JM, Rosso IM, Sanchez LE, Hadley T. Childhood cognitive functioning in schizophrenia patients and their unaffected siblings: a prospective cohort study. Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:379–393. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition. San Antonio, Tex: Psychological Corporation; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wilson BA, Cockburn J, Baddely A. The Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test. Gaylord, Mich: National Rehabilitation Services; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Marcus J, Hans SL, Auerbach JG, Auerbach AG. Children at risk for schizophrenia: the Jerusalem Infant Development Study. II. neurobehavioral deficits at school age. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:797–809. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820220053006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Carter JW, Schulsinger F, Parnas J, Cannon T, Mednick SA. A multivariate prediction model of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2002;28:649–682. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Cannon M, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, et al. Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:449–456. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.5.449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Fish B. Neurobiologic antecedents of schizophrenia in children. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1977;34:1297–1313. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1977.01770230039002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Fish B, Marcus J, Hans SL, Auerbach JG, Perdue S. Infants at risk for schizophrenia: sequelae of a genetic neurointegrative defect. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49:221–235. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820030053007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Wolf LE, Cornblatt BA. Neuropsychological functioning in children at risk for schizophrenia. In: Pantelis C, Nelson HE, Barnes TRE, editors. Schizophrenia: A Neuropsychological Perspective. London: Wiley; 1996. pp. 161–182. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hodges A, Byrne M, Grant E, Johnstone E. People at risk of schizophrenia: sample characteristics of the first 100 cases in the Edinburgh High-Risk Study. Br J Psychiatry. 1999;174:547–553. doi: 10.1192/bjp.174.6.547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Whyte M, Brett C, Harrison LK, et al. Neuropsychological performance over time in people at high risk for developing schizophrenia and controls. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:730–739. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Erlenmeyer-Kimling L, Rock D, Roberts SA, et al. Attention, memory, and motor skills as childhood predictors of schizophrenia-related psychoses: the New York High-Risk Project. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1416–1422. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.9.1416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Johnstone EC, Abukmeil SS, Byrne M, et al. Schizophr Res. Vol. 46. 2000. Edinburgh High-Risk Study—findings after four years: demographic, attainment and psychopathological issues; pp. 1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Johnstone EC, Ebmeier KP, Miller P, Owens DG, Lawrie SM. Predicting schizophrenia: findings from the Edinburgh High-Risk Study. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;186:18–25. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.1.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Sitskoorn MM, Aleman A, Ebisch SJ, Appels MC, Kahn RS. Cognitive deficits in relatives of patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2004;71:285–295. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Byrne M, Clafferty BA, Cosway R, et al. Neuropsychology, genetic liability, and psychotic symptoms in those at high risk of schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2003;112:38–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Faraone SV, Seidman LJ, Kremen WS, Toomey R, Pepple JR, Tsuang MT. Neuropsychologic functioning among the nonpsychotic relatives of schizophrenic patients: the effect of genetic loading. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48:120–126. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00263-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Jones P, Rodgers B, Murray R, Marmot M. Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet. 1994;344(8934):1398–1402. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(94)90569-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cannon TD, Rosso IM, Hollister JM, Bearden CE, Sanchez LE, Hadley T. A prospective cohort study of genetic and perinatal influences in the etiology of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:351–366. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Cannon TD, Huttunen MO, Lonnqvist J, et al. The inheritance of neuropsychological dysfunction in twins discordant for schizophrenia. Am J Hum Genet. 2000;67:369–382. doi: 10.1086/303006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Foerster A, Lewis SW, Owen MJ, Murray RM. Low birth weight and a family history of schizophrenia predict poor premorbid functioning in psychosis. Schizophr Res. 1991;5:13–20. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(91)90049-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Foerster A, Lewis S, Owen M, Murray R. Pre-morbid adjustment and personality in psychosis: effects of sex and diagnosis. Br J Psychiatry. 1991;158:171–176. doi: 10.1192/bjp.158.2.171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hollis C. Child and adolescent (juvenile onset) schizophrenia: a case control study of premorbid developmental impairments. Br J Psychiatry. 1995;166:489–495. doi: 10.1192/bjp.166.4.489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Poulton R, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Cannon M, Murray R, Harrington H. Children's self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: a 15-year longitudinal study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:1053–1058. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.11.1053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Isohanni I, Jarvelin MR, Jones P, Jokelainen J, Isohanni M. Can excellent school performance be a precursor of schizophrenia? a 28-year follow-up in the Northern Finland 1966 birth cohort. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1999;100:17–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb10909.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Cannon M, Jones P, Gilvarry C, et al. Premorbid social functioning in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: similarities and differences. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:1544–1550. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.11.1544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.David AS, Malmberg A, Brandt L, Allebeck P, Lewis G. IQ and risk for schizophrenia: a population-based cohort study. Psychol Med. 1997;27:1311–1323. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797005680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Davidson M, Reichenberg A, Rabinowitz J, Weiser M, Kaplan Z, Mark M. Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:1328–1335. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.9.1328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Huber G, Gross G, Schuttler R. Schizophrenie. Verlaufs- und sozialpsychiatrische Langzeituntersuchungen an den 1945–1959 in Bonn hospitalisierten schizophrenen Kranken. Bonn, Ger: Springer; 1979. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Olsen KR, Rosenboum B. Prospective investigations of the prodromal state of schizophrenia: review of studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005;113:247–272. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00697.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Yung AR, Phillips LJ, Yuen HP, McGorry PD. Risk factors for psychosis in an ultra high-risk group: psychopathology and clinical features. Schizophr Res. 2004;67:131–142. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00192-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Yung AR. Commentary: the schizophrenia prodrome: a high-risk concept. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29:859–865. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Bell RQ. Multiple-risk cohorts and segmenting risk as solutions to the problem of false positives in risk for the major psychoses. Psychiatry. 1992;55:370–381. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1992.11024610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Hafner H, Maurer K, Loffler W, et al. The epidemiology of early schizophrenia: influence of age and gender on onset and early course. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 1994;(23):29–38. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Brewer WJ, Francey S, Yung A, et al. Olfactory and neuropsychological deficits in high-risk and first-episode psychosis. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1996;6(Suppl3):136. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Brewer WJ, Francey S, Yung A, et al. Cognitive and olfactory deficits: course from high risk to first-episode psychosis. Schizophr Res. 1998;29:54. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Brewer WJ, Velakoulis D, Copolov D, et al. Cannabis use in never-medicated ultra high-risk and first-episode psychosis patients: relationship to cognition. Aust J Psychol. 2000;52(Suppl):127. [Google Scholar]
- 72.Brewer WJ. Olfactory and Neuropsychological Dysfunction: Neurobehavioral Patterns in Fronto-limbic Networks from High Risk to Chronic Schizophrenia [dissertation]. Melbourne, Aust: University of Melbourne; 1999.
- 73.Doty RL, Shaman P, Dann W. Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Test: standardized micro-encapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiol Behav. 1984;32:489–502. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90269-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Brewer WJ, Wood SJ, McGorry PD, et al. Impairment of olfactory identification ability in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis who later develop schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1790–1794. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.10.1790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Brewer WJ, Pantelis C, Anderson V, et al. Stability of olfactory identification deficits in neuroleptic-naive patients with first-episode psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:107–115. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.1.107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Brewer WJ, Pantelis C, DeLuca C, Wood SJ. Olfactory processing and brain maturation. In: Brewer WJ, Castle D, Pantelis C, editors. Olfaction and the Brain. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; in press. [Google Scholar]
- 77.Owen AM, Downes JJ, Sahakian BJ, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia. 1990;28:1021–1034. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90137-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Wood SJ, Pantelis C, Proffitt T, et al. Spatial working memory ability is a marker of risk-for-psychosis. Psychol Med. 2003;33:1239–1247. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703008067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Brewer WJ, Francey SM, Wood SJ, et al. Memory impairments identified in people at ultra-high risk for psychosis who later develop first-episode psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:71–78. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Wechsler D. The Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised. Manual. San Antonio, Tex: Psychological Corporation; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- 81.Francey SM, Jackson HJ, Phillips LJ, Wood SJ, Yung AR, McGorry PD. Sustained attention in young people at high risk of psychosis does not predict transition to psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2005;79:127–136. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.06.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Cornblatt BA, Risch NJ, Faris G, Friedman D, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. The Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs version (CPT-IP): I. new findings about sustained attention in normal families. Psychiatr Res. 1988;26:223–238. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(88)90076-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Chen WJ, Liu SK, Chang CJ, Lien YJ, Chang YH, Hwu HG. Sustained attention deficit and schizotypal personality features in nonpsychotic relatives of schizophrenic patients. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:1214–1220. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.9.1214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Franke P, Maier W, Hardt J, Hain C, Cornblatt BA. Attentional abilities and measures of schizotypy: their variation and covariation in schizophrenic patients, their siblings, and normal control subjects. Psychiatry Res. 1994;54:259–272. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(94)90020-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Laurent A, Biloa-Tang M, Bougerol T, et al. Executive/attentional performance and measures of schizotypy in patients with schizophrenia and in their nonpsychotic first-degree relatives. Schizophr Res. 2000;46:269–283. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00232-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Koutsouradis P, Wood SJ, Yung AR, et al. Executive function deficits identified in people at ultra-high risk for psychosis: a pilot study. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2005;39(Suppl):A31–A32. [Google Scholar]
- 87.Benton AL, Hamsher K deS. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, Iowa: AJA Associates; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 88.Spreen O, Strauss E. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 89.Joyce E, Huddy V. Defining the cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2004;34:1151–1155. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704003472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Macdonald AW, Carter CS. Cognitive experimental approaches to investigating impaired cognition in schizophrenia: a paradigm shift. J Cogn Exp Neuropsychol. 2002;24:873–882. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.7.873.8386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Lencz T, Smith CW, Auther AM, Correll CU, Cornblatt BA. The assessment of “prodromal schizophrenia”: unresolved issues and future directions. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29:717–728. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Cornblatt B, Lencz T, Obuchowski M. The schizophrenia prodrome: treatment and high-risk perspectives. Schizophr Res. 2002;54:177–186. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00365-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Cornblatt BA. The New York High Risk Project to the Hillside Recognition and Prevention (RAP) Program. Am J Med Genet. 2002;114:956–966. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.10520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Smith CW, Park S, Cornblatt B. Spatial working memory deficits in adolescents at clinical high risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2006;81:211–215. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.09.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Yung AR, Yuen HP, McGorry PD, et al. Mapping the onset of psychosis: the Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2005;39:964–971. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2005.01714.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Woods SW, et al. Symptom assessment in schizophrenic prodromal states. Psychiatr Q. 1999;70(4):273–287. doi: 10.1023/a:1022034115078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Hawkins KA, Addington J, Keefe RS, et al. Neuropsychological status of subjects at high risk for a first episode of psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2004;67:115–122. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Gschwandtner U, Aston J, Borgwardt S, et al. Neuropsychological and neurophysiological findings in individuals suspected to be at risk for schizophrenia: preliminary results from the Basel early detection of psychosis study—Fruherkennung von Psychosen (FEPSY) Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2003;108:152–155. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.00157.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Gschwandtner U, Pfluger M, Aston J, et al. Fine motor function and neuropsychological deficits in individuals at risk for schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2005 Nov 14; doi: 10.1007/s00406-005-0626-2. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Berg EA. A simple objective treatment for measuring flexibility in thinking. J Gen Psychol. 1948;39:15–22. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1948.9918159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Heaton RK. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) Odessa, Fla: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- 103.Zimmerman P, Fimm B. Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprufung (TAP) Freiberg: Psytest; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 104.Corcoran R, Upton D. A role for the hippocampus in card sorting? Cortex. 1993;29:293–304. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80182-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Carr V, Halpin S, Lau N, O'Brien S, Beckmann J, Lewin T. A risk factor screening and assessment protocol for schizophrenia and related psychosis. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2000;34(Suppl):S170–80. doi: 10.1080/000486700240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Mason O, Startup M, Halpin S, Schall U, Conrad A, Carr V. Risk factors for transition to first episode psychosis among individuals with “at-risk mental states.”. Schizophr Res. 2004;71:227–237. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Silverstein S, Uhlhaas PJ, Essex B, Halpin S, Schall U, Carr V. Perceptual organization in first episode schizophrenia and ultra-high-risk states. Schizophr Res. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.01.003. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Banks WP, Prinzmetal W. Configural effects in visual information processing. Perception Psychophys. 1976;19:361–367. [Google Scholar]
- 109.Schall U, Halpin S, Hunt SA, et al. Neurocognitive profiles of young people at high risk versus first-episode psychosis: a follow-up study. Schizophr Res. 2003;60(Suppl):156. [Google Scholar]
- 110.Seeber K, Cadenhead KS. How does studying schizotypal personality disorder inform us about the prodrome of schizophrenia? Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2005;7:41–50. doi: 10.1007/s11920-005-0024-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Hafner H, an der Heiden W. The course of schizophrenia in the light of modern follow-up studies: the ABC and WHO studies. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;249(Suppl4):14–26. doi: 10.1007/pl00014180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Hambrecht M, Lammertink M, Klosterkotter J, Matuschek E, Pukrop R. Subjective and objective neuropsychological abnormalities in a psychosis prodrome clinic. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 2002;43:S30–37. doi: 10.1192/bjp.181.43.s30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Goldstein G. Neuropsychological heterogeneity in schizophrenia: a consideration of abstraction and problem-solving abilities. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1990;5:251–264. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Heinrichs RW, Awad AG. Neurocognitive subtypes of chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1993;9:49–58. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(93)90009-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Palmer BW, Heaton RK, Paulsen JS, et al. Is it possible to be schizophrenic yet neuropsychologically normal? Neuropsychology. 1997;11:437–446. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.11.3.437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Kremen WS, Seidman LJ, Faraone SV, Toomey R, Tsuang MT. The paradox of normal neuropsychological function in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109:743–752. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.109.4.743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Allen DN, Goldstein G, Warnick E. A consideration of neuropsychologically normal schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003;9:56–63. doi: 10.1017/s135561770391006x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18:643–662. [Google Scholar]
- 119.Ruff RM, Light RH, Evans RW. The Ruff Figural Fluency Test: a normative study with adults. Dev Neuropsychol. 1987;3:37–52. [Google Scholar]
- 120.Halstead WC. Brain and Intelligence. Chicago. Ill: University of Chicago Press; 1947. [Google Scholar]
- 121.Klove H. Clinical neuropsychology. Medical Clin North Am. 1963;47:1647–1658. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Benton AL, Hannay HJ, Varney NR. Visual perception of line direction in patients with unilateral brain disease. Neurology. 1975;25:907–910. doi: 10.1212/wnl.25.10.907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston Naming Test—Second Edition. Philadelphia, Pa: Lea and Febiger; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 124.Goldberg TE, Ragland JD, Torrey EF, Gold JM, Bigelow LB, Weinberger DR. Neuropsychological assessment of monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47:1066–1072. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810230082013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Shallice T. Specific impairments of planning. Philos Trans Royal Soc London. 1982;298:199–209. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1982.0082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Cornblatt BA, Lenzenweger MF, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. The Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs version: II. contrasting attentional profiles in schizophrenic and depressed patients. Psychiatry Res. 1989;29:65–85. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90188-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test, Form II (Research Edition) San Antonio: Tex: Psychological Corporation; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 128.Nelson HE, O'Connell A. Dementia: the estimation of premorbid intelligence levels using the National Adult Reading Test. Cortex. 1978;14:234–244. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(78)80049-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment—Third Edition. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 130.Rey A. L'examen clinique en psychologie. Paris, France: Presses Universitaires de France; 1964. [Google Scholar]
- 131.Wechsler D. WAIS-R Manual. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- 132.Jastak S, Wilkinson GS. Wide Range Achievement Test—Revised. Wilmington, Del: Jastak Assessment Systems; 1984. [Google Scholar]