Abstract
Accumulating evidence indicates the key role of α-calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (αCaMKII) in synaptic plasticity and learning, but it remains unclear how this kinase participates in the processing of memory extinction. Here, we investigated the mechanism by which αCaMKII may mediate extinction by using heterozygous knock-in mice with a targeted T286A mutation that prevents the autophosphorylation of this kinase (αCaMKIIT286A+/–). Remarkably, partial reduction of αCaMKII function due to the T286A+/– mutation prevented the development of extinction without interfering with initial hippocampus-dependent memory formation as assessed by contextual fear conditioning and the Morris water maze. It is hypothesized that the mechanism of extinction may differ depending on the interval at which extinction training is started, being more akin to “new learning” at longer intervals and “unlearning” or “erasure” at shorter intervals. Consistent with this hypothesis, we found that extinction conducted 24 h, but not 15 min, after contextual fear training showed spontaneous recovery (reappearance of extinguished freezing responses) 21 d following the extinction, representing behavioral evidence for new learning and unlearning mechanisms underlying extinction 24 h and 15 min post-training, respectively. Importantly, the αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutation blocked new learning of contextual fear memory extinction, whereas it did not interfere with unlearning processes. Our results demonstrate a genetic dissociation of new learning and unlearning mechanisms of extinction, and suggest that αCaMKII is responsible for extinguishing memories specifically through new learning mechanisms.
A growing body of evidence suggests that memory retrieval is a dynamic process that modifies stored information following initial learning. For example, if animals are given repeated or prolonged re-exposure to the conditioned stimulus (CS) without the unconditioned stimulus (US) after training with CS–US pairing(s) in Pavlovian fear conditioning, they exhibit memory extinction as measured by a decline in conditioned fear responses. Extinction is thought to be an active learning process resulting in new memories that compete with and suppress the expression of original memory (“new learning”) (Myers and Davis 2002, 2007). Alternatively, extinction may simply reflect forgetting or erasing mechanisms of the original memory trace (“unlearning”) (Myers and Davis 2002, 2007). Interestingly, recent studies have begun to reveal that these different mechanisms that underlie extinction occur depending on the length of time following fear acquisition (Cain et al. 2005; Mao et al. 2006; Myers et al. 2006). Extinction initiated shortly after acquisition preferentially engages unlearning processing, while extinction initiated at longer delays recruits a new learning mechanism as evidenced by the re-emergence of extinguished fear responses with the passage of time (spontaneous recovery), following unsignaled presentation of the US (reinstatement) or when tested in a context different from the one where extinction training takes place (renewal) (Myers et al. 2006). Although some, but not all, components of the molecular machinery required for initial memory formation are suggested to be involved in its extinction (Myers and Davis 2002, 2007), it remains unclear how the learning-related signaling is responsible for the two distinct mechanisms of memory extinction.
α-Calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (αCaMKII) is an abundant synaptic protein that is central to memory formation (Irvine et al. 2006) and long-term potentiation (LTP), an activity-dependent strengthening of synapses that is thought to underlie learning and memory (Lisman et al. 2002). A broad range of genetic lesions of αCaMKII signaling has been demonstrated to block both learning and LTP (Elgersma et al. 2004). In particular, autophosphorylation of αCaMKII at threonine-286 in response to Ca2+ influx, which switches the kinase into a Ca2+/calmodulin-independent or autonomously active state, provides a crucial step (Miller and Kennedy 1986; Fukunaga et al. 1995; Tan and Liang 1996; Ouyang et al. 1997; Rodrigues et al. 2004), since knock-in mice with a targeted point mutation (T286A) that prevents autophosphorylation at this site exhibit deficits in hippocampal LTP and memory formation (Giese et al. 1998; Ohno et al. 2002, 2005, 2006; Need and Giese 2003; Irvine et al. 2005; Cooke et al. 2006). Although the mechanisms by which αCaMKII autophosphorylation contributes to learning have been well documented, its role in memory extinction remains to be determined. Two pharmacological investigations demonstrated that extinction of step-down passive avoidance performance was impaired when rats received the CaMKII inhibitor KN-62 before the first extinction exposure (Szapiro et al. 2003; Bevilaqua et al. 2006), but the precise mechanism by which this kinase mediates memory extinction is poorly understood. To address this question, the present study was designed to test the impacts of the αCaMKIIT286A point mutation on the extinction of hippocampus-dependent memory in contextual fear conditioning and Morris water maze paradigms, focusing on new learning and unlearning mechanisms. Although αCaMKIIT286A mice provide a valuable tool to study αCaMKII function in vivo, the homozygous T286A mutation significantly impairs hippocampal learning and precludes the analysis of extinction processes that follow the establishment of memory. Therefore, we investigated mice heterozygous for a T286A point mutation (αCaMKIIT286A+/–) that have partially reduced levels of αCaMKII function but retain normal learning abilities (Ohno et al. 2001), thus allowing us to specifically test their phenotypes during memory extinction.
Results
The αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutation blocks extinction of contextual fear memory
Our previous study demonstrated that mice homozygous for an αCaMKIIT286A point mutation show impairments in hippocampus-dependent contextual fear conditioning, in which mice learn to associate a distinct context (CS) with an aversive foot shock (US) (Ohno et al. 2005). In contrast, heterozygous αCaMKIIT286A+/– mice were normal in contextual learning, since they exhibited a robust contextual freezing response (the absence of all but respiratory movements) that was comparable to wild-type controls when tested 24 h after training with one CS–US pairing (F(1,58) = 0.24, P > 0.05) (Fig. 1). Furthermore, no difference was observed between αCaMKIIT286A+/– and wild-type mice in levels of freezing before or after the foot shock during training sessions (data not shown), suggesting that αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutants were also normal in movements and fear expression. During extinction, mice were repeatedly re-exposed to the conditioning chamber without a foot shock for 6 min per day for five consecutive days. Wild-type control mice showed a significant reduction in contextual freezing levels during the extinction sessions (F(4,170) = 7.57, P < 0.05). In contrast, repeated 6-min exposures to the conditioning chamber for 5 d did not affect contextual memory in αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutants (F(4,120) = 0.26, P > 0.05) (Fig. 1A), and freezing levels of αCaMKIIT286A+/– mice observed on Day 5 were significantly higher than those of wild-type littermate mice (F(1,58) = 14.58, P < 0.05) (Fig. 1B). Together, the αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutation specifically blocked extinction of contextually conditioned fear memory without affecting initial memory formation.
αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutation blocks extinction of spatial memory
To address the role of αCaMKII signaling in extinguishing the different types of memory, we next tested αCaMKIIT286A+/– mice in the Morris water maze (Fig. 2). In the hidden platform version of the water maze, animals learn to locate a submerged platform in a pool filled with opaque water. This hippocampus-dependent spatial navigation performance is profoundly impaired in homozygous αCaMKIIT286A mutant mice (Giese et al. 1998; Need and Giese 2003; Ohno et al. 2005). In contrast, αCaMKIIT286A+/– mice were normal in spatial learning in the water maze, since both the heterozygotes (F(4,30) = 2.88, P < 0.05) and wild-type controls (F(4,70) = 10.62, P < 0.05) showed similar reductions in their latencies to find the escape platform during acquisition (three sessions of two trials per day for 5 d) (Fig. 2A). Twenty-four hours after the last training trial, mice received four consecutive probe trials (30-min intervals) in which the platform was removed and mice were allowed to search for it for 60 sec, and the effects of the αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutation on the course of spatial memory extinction were examined. On the first probe test, both αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutant (F(3,24) = 3.45, P < 0.05) and wild-type control (F(3,56) = 3.71, P < 0.05) mice spent a significantly longer time in the target quadrant where the hidden platform was during spatial training, as compared with the other three quadrants. No significant difference in the search performance on the first probe trial (F(1,20) = 0.74, P > 0.05) demonstrated that αCaMKIIT286A+/– mice had normal spatial memory function (Fig. 2B).
Consistent with previous results (Lattal et al. 2003; Suzuki et al. 2004), repeatedly placing mice into the pool in the absence of the platform resulted in a decreased preference for the target quadrant in wild-type controls (F(3,56) = 3.48, P < 0.05) (Fig. 2B). In contrast, αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutants showed no reduction in the percentage of time spent in the target quadrant through four probe trials (F(3,24) = 0.15, P > 0.05), and their extinction rate of spatial memory was significantly lower than that of wild-type littermate controls (F(1,20) = 21.31, P < 0.05). Using another measure of spatial memory (Fig. 2C), we confirmed that extinction occurred in wild-type mice, as assessed by significant increases in proximity to the accurate platform position (F(3,56) = 3.45, P < 0.05), whereas αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutants showed no change in proximity through four consecutive probe trials (F(3,24) = 0.36, P > 0.05) and extinction was significantly prohibited in this mutant (F(1,20) = 12.04, P < 0.05). During the extinction trials, neither significant floating behavior nor reductions in the average swim speed were observed in αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutants (F(3,24) = 2.00, P > 0.05) or wild-type control mice (F(3,56) = 1.98, P > 0.05), and there was no difference in swim speed between both groups (F(1,20) = 1.71, P > 0.05) (Fig. 2D). Therefore, the αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutation specifically interfered with extinction of spatial memory without affecting swim performance by itself in the water maze.
αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutation blocks new learning but not unlearning mechanisms of fear memory extinction
Different mechanisms of extinction are suggested to occur depending upon the length of intervals between initial learning and extinction sessions: Extinction after longer intervals may more preferentially trigger new learning mechanisms, while shorter intervals may result in unlearning processes (Cain et al. 2005; Mao et al. 2006; Myers et al. 2006). To address the roles of αCaMKII signaling in the two distinct processes of extinction, mice were subjected to a single but longer re-exposure to the conditioning context in the absence of foot shock, which has been reported to favor the development of rapid and dramatic extinction of contextually conditioned fear (Suzuki et al. 2004; Isiegas et al. 2006). Consistent with these findings, we also found that a single 30-min re-exposure of wild-type control mice to the chamber 24 h after training with one CS–US pairing resulted in a significant reduction of contextual freezing within the long extinction session (F(4,105) = 6.99, P < 0.05) (Fig. 3A). A form of new learning that underlies extinction is accounted for by the fact that spontaneous recovery of extinguished fear responses occurs with the passage of time following extinction in the absence of any further training (Myers and Davis 2002, 2007). Importantly, while wild-type mice showed significantly lower levels of freezing during the last 6-min period of extinction session as compared with the first 6-min period, comparable contextual freezing reappeared in a test for spontaneous recovery given 21 d after extinction (F(2,63) = 11.30, P < 0.05) (Fig. 3B). Since the original fear memory remained intact, this experimental protocol for extinction initiated 24 h after contextual conditioning engaged new learning of inhibition.
αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutant mice exhibited robust contextual freezing during the first 6 min of extinction that was indistinguishable from that of wild-type controls (F(1,39) = 0.28, P > 0.05), but they showed no significant reduction in freezing through the 30-min extinction session conducted 24 h after conditioning (F(4,90) = 1.04, P > 0.05), presenting a contrast to the performance of wild-type mice (Fig. 3A). Notably, freezing levels of αCaMKIIT286A+/– mice observed during the final 6-min period of the extinction session were significantly higher than those of wild-type littermate mice (F(1,39) = 5.70, P < 0.05) (Fig. 3B) and were not different from levels of freezing in αCaMKIIT286A+/– mice during the initial 6 min of extinction or during the test for spontaneous recovery conducted 21 d later (F(2,54) = 1.39, P > 0.05). Taken together, these results showed that the αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutation interfered with new learning mechanisms underlying extinction of contextual fear memory without affecting initial contextual learning.
On the other hand, neither wild-type mice (F(4,95) = 1.56, P > 0.05) nor αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutants (F(4,115) = 0.79, P > 0.05) showed a significant within-session reduction in contextual freezing when the 30-min extinction session was given 15 min after training (Fig. 4A). Since similar levels of freezing were observed throughout the extinction session, including the first 6-min period in αCaMKIIT286A+/– and wild-type control groups (Fig. 4A,C), we tested a subset of mice 1 d later to clarify whether conditioned fear memory was extinguished (Fig. 4B). Significantly lower levels of freezing during this post-extinction test as compared with the first 6 min of the extinction session were observed in wild-type control mice (F(2,57) = 8.72, P < 0.05) and αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutants (F(2,69) = 13.78, P < 0.05), and, more importantly, the freezing levels of both groups remained significantly lower 21 d after the extinction session, representing no spontaneous recovery of extinguished memory. However, it is possible that the additional exposure to the conditioning chamber 1 d after extinction prevented spontaneous recovery. To address this possibility, we tested the other set of mice for spontaneous recovery 21 d after the 30-min extinction session without any sessions intervening between them (Fig. 4D). While no within-session extinction was observed again in wild-type controls (F(4,55) = 0.68, P > 0.05) or αCaMKIIT286A+/– mice (F(4,75) = 0.69, P > 0.05) during 30-min exposure to the chamber given 15 min post-training (Fig. 4C), their freezing levels 21 d later were significantly lower than those of the first 6-min period in wild-type mice (F(1,22) = 10.98, P < 0.05) and αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutants (F(1,30) = 24.43, P < 0.05) (Fig. 4D). Thus, we concluded that extinction initiated 15 min after training was devoid of spontaneous recovery of contextual memory in both wild-type control and αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutant mice.
It should be noted that the short-interval group of wild-type mice produced poorer extinction (Fig. 4), as compared with the long-interval group that received the same 30-min extinction session 24 h after contextual training and exhibited a robust within-session extinction (Fig. 3A). However, this did not appear to account for the lack of recovery in the short-interval extinction group, since poorer extinction was not associated with poorer retention of extinction but rather enduring extinction. In the first set of experiments (Fig. 4B), αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutants showed significantly lower levels of freezing as compared to wild-type controls during the memory testing 1 d (F(1,42) = 29.14, P < 0.05) and 21 d (F(1,42) = 11.42, P < 0.05) after mice were exposed to a 30-min extinction session 15 min post-training. However, this was likely due to slightly lower levels of freezing during the first 6 min of extinction in αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutants (although statistically not significant; F(1,42) = 3.54, P = 0.07), and no difference in freezing was observed between αCaMKIIT286A+/– and wild-type mice during the first 6 min of the extinction session (F(1,26) = 0.45, P = 0.51) or during memory testing 21 d post-extinction (F(1,26) = 0.12, P = 0.74) in the second set of experiments (Fig. 4D). Obviously, in contrast to the impacts on new learning mechanisms of delayed extinction 24 h post-training that were followed by spontaneous recovery (Fig. 3), the αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutation did not inhibit the short-interval extinction, which was initiated 15 min post-training and showed no signs of spontaneous recovery (Fig. 4). Collectively, our results strongly supported the idea that different mechanisms are recruited during memory extinction at shorter and longer time points after training (Cain et al. 2005; Mao et al. 2006; Myers et al. 2006).
Discussion
Extinction appears to rely on similar molecular pathways that are responsible for initial learning processes (Myers and Davis 2002, 2007), while evidence is also emerging that extinction may engage different mechanisms (Lattal and Abel 2001; Cain et al. 2002; Marsicano et al. 2002; Isiegas et al. 2006). We investigated the role of αCaMKII, a key molecular determinant of memory formation (Irvine et al. 2006), in extinction of contextual fear and spatial memories by testing how a heterozygous T286A point mutation at the autophosphorylation site of this kinase (αCaMKIIT286A+/–) affects the mechanisms that extinguish memories. Although mice that are homozygous for the αCaMKIIT286A mutation and deficient in autophosphorylation exhibit impairments in contextual fear conditioning and spatial learning in the water maze (Giese et al. 1998; Ohno et al. 2002, 2005, 2006; Need and Giese 2003; Irvine et al. 2005), αCaMKIIT286A+/– mice were normal in the formation of contextual and spatial memories tested 24 h after training. Remarkably, the αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutation prevented extinction of contextual fear and spatial memories when the mice were repeatedly subjected to the contextual CS-alone sessions or exposed to the water maze pool in the absence of the platform. Therefore, these findings indicate that both initial learning and the development of memory extinction require the activation of αCaMKII through its autophosphorylation, but extinction processes are more susceptible to partial disruption of αCaMKII signaling due to the heterozygous T286A mutation and place higher demands on intact αCaMKII function as compared with memory formation.
It is hypothesized that the length of time between the initial training and extinction sessions may be a determinant of neural mechanisms involved in extinguishing original memory (Cain et al. 2005; Mao et al. 2006; Myers et al. 2006; for review, see Myers and Davis 2007). In particular, Myers et al. (2006) recently tested this hypothesis by giving different groups of rats an extinction session at different time points ranging from 10 min to 72 h after acquisition and evaluated their susceptibility to recovery of conditioned fear through reinstatement, renewal, and spontaneous recovery 21 d later. They demonstrated that rats extinguished 24–72 h following conditioning exhibit moderate to strong recovery that provides evidence for new learning accounts of extinction, while rats extinguished 10 min to 1 h after acquisition exhibit little or no recovery, representing erasure mechanisms of conditioned fear. New learning accounts of extinction involve the formation of a second, inhibitory association whose effect is opposite to the excitatory association between representations of the CS and US during acquisition and renders the CS no longer capable of evoking fear responses. It is conceivable that this inhibitory association is generally more labile or subject to disruption than initial excitatory associations and thus may be lost during the recovery processes of fear memory following extinction (Myers and Davis 2002, 2007). Consistent with these views, we found that wild-type control mice, re-exposed to the chamber for 30 min 24 h after acquisition, exhibit new learning mechanisms of extinction, since they showed spontaneous recovery of contextually conditioned fear responses that were measured 21 d after extinction.
Importantly, our results demonstrated that the αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutation significantly and specifically impairs the new learning processing of fear memory extinction tested 24 h after training with a single CS–US pairing without affecting initial fear learning, suggesting a critical role of αCaMKII signaling in extinguishing memories through new learning mechanisms. It should be noted that a homozygous αCaMKIIT286A mutation abolishes NMDA-receptor-dependent LTP at hippocampal Schaffer collateral-CA1 synapses (Giese et al. 1998; Ohno et al. 2002; Cooke et al. 2006) and impairs contextual conditioning after a single training trial (Irvine et al. 2005; Ohno et al. 2005), while LTP is only attenuated, and considerable levels of LTP remain in the heterozygotes (Ohno et al. 2002). It is reasonable to suppose that partial reductions in LTP may impact new learning mechanisms of extinction but the residual level of LTP may be enough to sustain initial fear learning in αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutants, since the inhibitory association is formed by re-exposure to the context CS alone during extinction and thus, is more vulnerable to disruptions as compared with the excitatory association with CS–US pairing during fear memory acquisition.
On the other hand, a single 30-min re-exposure of wild-type control mice to the conditioning chamber 15 min after contextual training caused less extinction of fear memory compared with that given 24 h after conditioning, since it showed no within-session extinction and produced significant but only limited levels of reduction in freezing during a post-extinction test occurring 24 h later. Similar differences in the efficacy of short-interval versus long-interval extinction have been reported previously (Cain et al. 2005; Cammarota et al. 2005; Maren and Chang 2006; Myers et al. 2006). Of particular interest, Maren and Chang (2006) compared the 15-min and 24-h interval extinctions after tone fear conditioning in rats, and demonstrated that both extinction procedures acutely suppressed fear memories, but shorter-interval extinction was less effective in producing long-term suppression of conditioned fear (48 h after extinction). Therefore, the results suggest that recent fear memory is more resistant to extinction. However, this difference does not seem to reflect the conditioning–extinction interval per se, but rather is due to the higher levels of freezing at the outset of shorter-interval extinction training. In fact, the investigators demonstrated that reducing the number of conditioning trials and thus decreasing levels of fear at the time of extinction (15 min post-training) enabled animals to exhibit the long-term retention of extinction, which lasted at least for a week without spontaneous recovery. The present study similarly demonstrated that extinction of contextual fear memory at a shorter time point (15 min) after initial training, as compared with that at a longer time point (24 h), becomes less complete but more resistant to spontaneous recovery. Our results are also consistent with recent findings that animals that underwent extinction 10–60 min after conditioning lacked spontaneous recovery of memory, as measured by fear-potentiated acoustic startle responses, in contrast to the animals extinguished 24–72 h after training (Myers et al. 2006).
What mechanisms may account for the short-interval extinction, which is less susceptible to spontaneous recovery of original memories than is long-interval extinction? Psychological theories have described extinction as “unlearning,” in which the excitatory association between CS and US mediating conditioned responses is weakened and ultimately lost (Rescorla and Wagner 1972; Wagner and Rescorla 1972; for review, see Myers and Davis 2007). More recent evidence points to the importance of the timing of extinction relative to conditioning in determining the nature of extinction. Given that recovery effects serve as the impetus for new learning accounts of extinction (Myers and Davis 2002, 2007), the absence of spontaneous recovery implies the possibility that erasure may be a plausible mechanism underlying short-interval extinction (Myers et al. 2006). Importantly, this idea is strongly supported by recent neurobiological observations (Cain et al. 2005; Mao et al. 2006). For example, fear extinction initiated immediately after acquisition is insensitive to the L-type voltage-gated calcium channel (L-VGCC) blocker nifedipine, whereas delayed extinction is impaired by nifedipine (Cain et al. 2005). Therefore, the L-VGCC dependence of extinction is related to the maturity or consolidation state of the acquired memory. The investigators suggested that extinction training may act to weaken the excitatory CS–US association formed during acquisition in an L-VGCC-independent manner when fear memory has not yet fully consolidated at shorter intervals after acquisition, probably via a mechanism corresponding to depotentiation or long-term depression (LTD). Consistent with this hypothesis, extinction training applied 1 h after acquisition reversed the fear conditioning-induced synaptic insertion of AMPA-type glutamate receptors (a putative molecular mechanism for erasure of fear memories), whereas this reversal did not occur when extinction was started 24 h after training (Mao et al. 2006). The present study demonstrated that the αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutation does not affect short-interval extinction, which is initiated 15 min after contextual conditioning, as opposed to interfering with new learning mechanisms during delayed extinction 24 h post-training. Therefore, our observations further support the view that distinct molecular mechanisms underlie short- and long-interval extinction of memory. It seems reasonable to speculate that the incomplete consolidation state 15 min after training may be preferentially subjected to an αCaMKII-independent unlearning processing of fear memory (e.g., depotentiation or LTD) rather than new learning of inhibition, which proceeds via αCaMKII-mediated mechanisms and is crucial for extinguishing well-consolidated memory 24 h after conditioning.
Taken collectively, our results suggest that αCaMKII-dependent signaling is specifically involved in mediating a form of new learning that counteracts the expression of original memories during long-interval extinction, whereas αCaMKII function does not contribute to erasure processing of memories at a labile state during short-interval extinction. The data presented here favor the notion that the molecular machinery may differentially participate in extinguishing memories depending on the timing of extinction, although further studies will be required to fully understand the neurobiological basis of both types of memory extinction.
Materials and Methods
Mice
The generation of the αCaMKIIT286A mutant mice was described previously (Giese et al. 1998). The heterozygous αCaMKIIT286A population was backcrossed more than 15 generations into the C57BL/6N background from the original 129/B6 background. The resultant αCaMKIIT286A+/– mutants at 3–5 mo of age were used for the experiments, and wild-type littermate mice served as controls. At 3–4 wk postnatally, the mice were weaned and genotyping was performed by PCR analysis of tail DNA. All experiments were done blind with respect to the genotype of the mice and were conducted with the approval of the Nathan Kline Institute Animal Care and Use Committee.
Fear conditioning
Contextual fear conditioning was tested as described previously (Ohno et al. 2001, 2005). The experiments were performed using four standard conditioning chambers, each of which was housed in a soundproof isolation cubicle and equipped with a stainless-steel grid floor connected to a solid-state shock scrambler. Each scrambler was connected to an electronic constant-current shock source that was controlled via an interface connected to a Windows XP computer running FreezeFrame software (Coulbourn Instruments). A digital camera was mounted on the steel ceiling of each chamber, and video signals were sent to the same computer for analysis. During training, all mice that were going to be subjected to extinction later were placed in the conditioning chamber for 3 min and then exposed to a foot shock (1.0 mA, 2 sec). After the shock delivery, mice were left in the chamber for another 30 sec. Contextual fear memory was measured by scoring freezing behavior (the absence of all but respiratory movement) when the mice were placed back into the same conditioning chamber. We also assessed freezing before and after foot shock was delivered during training. The automated FreezeFrame system digitizes the video signal at 4 Hz and compares movement frame by frame to score the amount of freezing.
During the repeated extinction sessions, mice were re-exposed to the conditioning chamber in the absence of foot shock for 6 min per day for five consecutive days, staring 24 h after contextual fear training, and levels of freezing were assessed. To investigate new learning and unlearning mechanisms of fear memory extinction, different sets of mice received a single 30-min re-exposure to the chamber without foot shock 24 h and 15 min after contextual conditioning, respectively. Since significant within-session reductions in contextual freezing were not observed in the short-interval extinction groups, extinction was evaluated by comparing the freezing when these mice were re-exposed to the conditioning chamber for 3 min, 24 h after the 30-min extinction session. As a control, a subset of mice that were subjected to 30-min extinction 15 min after training did not receive the additional 3-min exposure to the chamber 24 h post-extinction. Twenty-one days after extinction, all mice were re-exposed to the training context for 3 min to test spontaneous recovery of extinguished fear memory.
Morris water maze
The basic water maze protocol and apparatus have been described previously (Bourtchuladze et al. 1994; Ohno et al. 2005). Before training, each mouse was handled for 2 min every day for 7–10 d. The pool was 1.2 m in diameter and was made of white plastic. The water was maintained at 24°C ± 1°C throughout the experiments and was made opaque with nontoxic white paint to hide the white and square escape platform (10 cm × 10 cm). During training, the escape platform was submerged (1 cm) below the water surface and remained in the same position in the pool for a particular mouse, but groups of animals were trained with different platform positions to avoid quadrant biases.
Every training trial started with the mouse on the platform for 30 sec. The mouse was placed into the water facing the wall of the pool and was allowed to search for the platform. The starting position varied among four locations in a pseudorandom manner for each trial. The mice received six trials per day for 5 d (three blocks of two trials; 1-min inter-trial intervals, 1-h inter-block intervals). The trial ended either when an animal climbed onto the platform or when a maximum of 60 sec elapsed. The mouse was placed on the platform for 30 sec at the end of each trial. Twenty-four hours after the last training trial, all mice were given four consecutive 60-sec probe trials (30-min intervals) in which they swam in the pool in the absence of the platform. The data collection and analysis were performed using a digital tracking device (HVS Image).
Statistical analysis
The significance of differences between the groups was determined by a one-way ANOVA or by a two-way ANOVA with repeated measures, and post-hoc Fisher’s PLSD tests were performed when appropriate.
Acknowledgment
This work was supported by a National Institute of Mental Health grant (R01 MH067251) to M.O.
Footnotes
Article is online at http://www.learnmem.org/cgi/doi/10.1101/lm.1049608.
References
- Bevilaqua L.R., Bonini J.S., Rossato J.I., Izquierdo L.A., Cammarota M., Izquierdo I. The entorhinal cortex plays a role in extinction. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2006;85:192–197. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2005.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourtchuladze R., Frenguelli B., Blendy J., Cioffi D., Schutz G., Silva A.J. Deficient long-term memory in mice with a targeted mutation of the cAMP-responsive element-binding protein. Cell. 1994;79:59–68. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90400-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cain C.K., Blouin A.M., Barad M. L-type voltage-gated calcium channels are required for extinction, but not for acquisition or expression, of conditional fear in mice. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:9113–9121. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-20-09113.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cain C.K., Godsil B.P., Jami S., Barad M. The L-type calcium channel blocker nifedipine impairs extinction, but not reduced contingency effects, in mice. Learn. Mem. 2005;12:277–284. doi: 10.1101/lm.88805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cammarota M., Bevilaqua L.R., Rossato J.I., Ramirez M., Medina J.H., Izquierdo I. Relationship between short- and long-term memory and short- and long-term extinction. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2005;84:25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2005.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooke S.F., Wu J., Plattner F., Errington M., Rowan M., Peters M., Hirano A., Bradshaw K.D., Anwyl R., Bliss T.V., et al. Autophosphorylation of αCaMKII is not a general requirement for NMDA receptor-dependent LTP in the adult mouse. J. Physiol. 2006;574:805–818. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.111559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elgersma Y., Sweatt J.D., Giese K.P. Mouse genetic approaches to investigating calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II function in plasticity and cognition. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:8410–8415. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3622-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fukunaga K., Muller D., Miyamoto E. Increased phosphorylation of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II and its endogenous substrates in the induction of long-term potentiation. J. Biol. Chem. 1995;270:6119–6124. doi: 10.1074/jbc.270.11.6119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giese K.P., Fedorov N.B., Filipkowski R.K., Silva A.J. Autophosphorylation at Thr286 of the α calcium-calmodulin kinase II in LTP and learning. Science. 1998;279:870–873. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5352.870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irvine E.E., Vernon J., Giese K.P. αCaMKII autophosphorylation contributes to rapid learning but is not necessary for memory. Nat. Neurosci. 2005;8:411–412. doi: 10.1038/nn1431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irvine E.E., von Hertzen L.S., Plattner F., Giese K.P. αCaMKII autophosphorylation: A fast track to memory. Trends Neurosci. 2006;29:459–465. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isiegas C., Park A., Kandel E.R., Abel T., Lattal K.M. Transgenic inhibition of neuronal protein kinase A activity facilitates fear extinction. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:12700–12707. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2743-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lattal K.M., Abel T. Different requirements for protein synthesis in acquisition and extinction of spatial preferences and context-evoked fear. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:5773–5780. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-15-05773.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lattal K.M., Mullen M.T., Abel T. Extinction, renewal, and spontaneous recovery of a spatial preference in the water maze. Behav. Neurosci. 2003;117:1017–1028. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.5.1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman J., Schulman H., Cline H. The molecular basis of CaMKII function in synaptic and behavioural memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3:175–190. doi: 10.1038/nrn753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mao S.C., Hsiao Y.H., Gean P.W. Extinction training in conjunction with a partial agonist of the glycine site on the NMDA receptor erases memory trace. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:8892–8899. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0365-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S., Chang C.H. Recent fear is resistant to extinction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2006;103:18020–18025. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608398103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsicano G., Wotjak C.T., Azad S.C., Bisogno T., Rammes G., Cascio M.G., Hermann H., Tang J., Hofmann C., Zieglgansberger W., et al. The endogenous cannabinoid system controls extinction of aversive memories. Nature. 2002;418:530–534. doi: 10.1038/nature00839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller S.G., Kennedy M.B. Regulation of brain type II Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase by autophosphorylation: A Ca2+-triggered molecular switch. Cell. 1986;44:861–870. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(86)90008-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers K.M., Davis M. Behavioral and neural analysis of extinction. Neuron. 2002;36:567–584. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01064-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers K.M., Davis M. Mechanisms of fear extinction. Mol. Psychiatry. 2007;12:120–150. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers K.M., Ressler K.J., Davis M. Different mechanisms of fear extinction dependent on length of time since fear acquisition. Learn. Mem. 2006;13:216–223. doi: 10.1101/lm.119806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Need A.C., Giese K.P. Handling and environmental enrichment do not rescue learning and memory impairments in αCamKIIT286A mutant mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2003;2:132–139. doi: 10.1034/j.1601-183x.2003.00020.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohno M., Frankland P.W., Chen A.P., Costa R.M., Silva A.J. Inducible, pharmacogenetic approaches to the study of learning and memory. Nat. Neurosci. 2001;4:1238–1243. doi: 10.1038/nn771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohno M., Frankland P.W., Silva A.J. A pharmacogenetic inducible approach to the study of NMDA/αCaMKII signaling in synaptic plasticity. Curr. Biol. 2002;12:654–656. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(02)00767-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohno M., Tseng W., Silva A.J., Disterhoft J.F. Trace eyeblink conditioning requires the hippocampus but not autophosphorylation of αCaMKII in mice. Learn. Mem. 2005;12:211–215. doi: 10.1101/lm.90205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohno M., Sametsky E.A., Silva A.J., Disterhoft J.F. Differential effects of αCaMKII mutation on hippocampal learning and changes in intrinsic neuronal excitability. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2006;23:2235–2240. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04746.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ouyang Y., Kantor D., Harris K.M., Schuman E.M., Kennedy M.B. Visualization of the distribution of autophosphorylated calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II after tetanic stimulation in the CA1 area of the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:5416–5427. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-14-05416.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla R.A., Wagner A.R. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Black A.H., Prokasy W.F., editors. Classical conditioning II. Appleton-Century-Crofts; New York: 1972. pp. 64–99. [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues S.M., Farb C.R., Bauer E.P., LeDoux J.E., Schafe G.E. Pavlovian fear conditioning regulates Thr286 autophosphorylation of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II at lateral amygdala synapses. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:3281–3288. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5303-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki A., Josselyn S.A., Frankland P.W., Masushige S., Silva A.J., Kida S. Memory reconsolidation and extinction have distinct temporal and biochemical signatures. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:4787–4795. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5491-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szapiro G., Vianna M.R., McGaugh J.L., Medina J.H., Izquierdo I. The role of NMDA glutamate receptors, PKA, MAPK, and CAMKII in the hippocampus in extinction of conditioned fear. Hippocampus. 2003;13:53–58. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan S.E., Liang K.C. Spatial learning alters hippocampal calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II activity in rats. Brain Res. 1996;711:234–240. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01411-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner A.R., Rescorla R.A. Inhibition in Pavlovian conditioning: Application of a theory. In: Boakes R.A., Halliday M.S., editors. Inhibition and learning. Academic Press; London: 1972. pp. 301–336. [Google Scholar]