Abstract
Temporal control of brain and behavioral states emerges as a consequence of the interaction between circadian and homeostatic neural circuits. This interaction permits the daily rhythm of sleep and wake, regulated in parallel by circadian cues originating from the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) and arousal-promoting signals arising from the orexin-containing neurons in the tuberal hypothalamus (TH). Intriguingly, the SCN circadian clock can be reset by arousal-promoting stimuli while activation of orexin/hypocretin neurons is believed to be under circadian control, suggesting the existence of a reciprocal relationship. Unfortunately, since orexin neurons are themselves activated by locomotor promoting cues, it is unclear how these two systems interact to regulate behavioral rhythms. Here mice were placed in conditions of constant light, which suppressed locomotor activity, but also revealed a highly pronounced circadian pattern in orexin neuronal activation. Significantly, activation of orexin neurons in the medial and lateral TH occurred prior to the onset of sustained wheel-running activity. Moreover, exposure to a 6 h dark pulse during the subjective day, a stimulus that promotes arousal and phase advances behavioral rhythms, activated neurons in the medial and lateral TH including those containing orexin. Concurrently, this stimulus suppressed SCN activity while activating cells in the median raphe. In contrast, dark pulse exposure during the subjective night did not reset SCN-controlled behavioral rhythms and caused a transient suppression of neuronal activation in the TH. Collectively these results demonstrate, for the first time, pronounced circadian control of orexin neuron activation and implicate recruitment of orexin cells in dark pulse resetting of the SCN circadian clock.
Background
The mammalian hypothalamus plays a fundamental role in the control of numerous critical brain and behavior states. For example, within the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN), site of the brain's dominant circadian clock, neurons orchestrate daily rhythms responsible for a wide range of physiological and neural processes [1,2]. This SCN circadian clock is synchronized (entrained) by exogenous time cues (zeitgebers) such as varying levels of environmental light (photic stimuli), as well as by so-called non-photic cues that promote behavioral arousal [3,4]. Photic information is conveyed to the SCN via the retinohypothalamic tract [5,6], while non-photic stimuli activate pathways originating in the thalamic intergeniculate leaflet (IGL) and the median raphe (MRN) of the brain stem [7,8]. Unfortunately, our understanding of how neural substrates integrate circadian and arousal information is far from complete.
Recently, significant progress was made in identifying the neurochemical basis of arousal with the isolation and characterization of the orexin/hypocretin neuropeptides (referred to as orexins in this study) [9,10]. There are two bioactive forms, orexin-A (OXA) and orexin-B, which are cleaved from the common precursor prepro-orexin. The orexins are mainly excitatory and mediate their biological actions through two G-protein coupled receptors, OXR1 and OXR2. In mammals, neurons expressing orexins are mostly limited to the lateral hypothalamic area and dorsomedial nucleus of the tuberal hypothalamus (TH). Immunohistochemical studies have established that these orexin-containing neurons have extensive neuronal projections and innervate key circadian structures including the SCN, IGL and MRN [11-13].
SCN efferents project to the TH [14] and may directly contact orexin-containing cells [15]. Transgenic and pharmacological impairment of orexin-OXR1/R2 signaling implicates this neuropeptide system in the promotion of arousal and wakefulness [16]. Indeed, orexin neurons demonstrate diurnal variation in activity/activation, with highest levels in nocturnal animals occurring during the night [17,18]. Since orexin neuronal activation corresponds with forward locomotor activity [19,20], and because such behavior increases at night, it is unclear whether SCN-derived signals regulate the nighttime activation of orexin neurons.
In this study, using c-Fos as a marker of cellular activation, we demonstrate that under constant light (LL), a condition that suppresses the amplitude of wheel-running rhythms, TH cells including OXA-immunoreactive (OXA-ir) neurons are much more active during the subjective night than the subjective day. We also show that activation of OXA-ir neurons in the medial TH occurs prior to the nocturnal onset of vigorous wheel-running. Transient exposure to 6 h of darkness during the subjective day promotes arousal, suppresses c-Fos expression in the SCN and activates OXA-ir neurons, particularly those in the medial TH. Such changes in arousal and cellular activity accompany the resetting actions of this dark pulse stimulus at this time. These results implicate SCN-derived signals in exerting temporal control over TH cells including OXA-ir neurons and highlight extra-SCN actions of an arousal-promoting phase-resetting stimulus.
Results
Behavioral Results
Effects of Constant Light (LL)
Under LD conditions, all singly housed male C57BL/6J mice (n = 92) showed robust diurnal rhythms in wheel-running activity with most of their locomotor activity occurring during the lights-off phase (Figure 1). With the transition from LD to LL, all mice demonstrated an immediate suppression of wheel-running activity. Under LD conditions the average number of wheel revolutions in the 24 h preceding this transition was 15193 ± 109 (mean ± SEM), and this was significantly reduced by 81% to 2839 ± 43 revolutions during the first 24 h in LL (p < 0.001). With prolonged exposure to LL, the period of wheel-running (tau) significantly lengthened from ~24 h to ~24.8 h (p < 0.001) and the duration of the active phase (alpha) also significantly increased from ~11.8 h to ~13.1 h (p < 0.05) (Table 1). The intensity of wheel-running during alpha was greatly suppressed from ~19.7 in LD to ~5.3 revolutions/min in LL (p < 0.001) and the percentage of variance explained by the dominant period of the running-wheel rhythm (PVE) was also significantly reduced from ~67% in LD to ~27% in LL (p < 0.001), indicating that the strength of the locomotor rhythm diminished in these conditions (Table 1).
Table 1.
Circadian Parameter | Light-Dark (LD) | Constant Light (LL) |
tau (h) | 24.08 ± 0.03 h | 24.84 ± 0.04 h* |
Percentage of Variance Explained by the Dominant Rhythmic Component (PV%) | 66.6 ± 1.47 | 26.6 ± 0.98*** |
Alpha Duration (h) | 11.8 ± 0.15 | 13.1 ± 0.27* |
Motor Activity (RPM) | 19.7 ± 1.19 | 5.3 ± 0.51*** |
***P < 0.001; *P < 0.05
Dark Pulse Phase-Response Curve
Twenty-five mice, from the above 92, received dark pulses of 6 h duration at various points in the circadian cycle. Dark pulses centred between mid-subjective day and early subjective night significantly advanced circadian phase (Figures 1, 2). More specifically, pulses initiated between CT3–7 (n = 16) elicited phase shifts with an average magnitude of ~6.17 h. Dark pulses starting between CT7–11 (n = 9) elicited smaller but still significant phase shifts with an average magnitude of ~1.38 h (mean ± 95% confidence intervals (CI) values did not overlap zero). Dark pulses applied at other circadian times, failed to elicit significant phase shifts (mean ± CI values overlapped zero) (Figure 2). In addition to the resetting actions, dark pulse exposure acutely increased wheel-running by ~2.3 km on the day of the pulse. However, this induced activity did not correlate with the resultant phase shift magnitude (Pearson correlation, p > 0.05; data not shown), suggesting that the dark pulse elicited increase in wheel-running is not a key factor influencing phase shift magnitude to this stimulus. Interestingly, on the days following the dark pulse, the strength of the wheel-running rhythm was significantly reduced (PVE reduced from 31.1% to 28.6%; p < 0.05). Further, although tau and the duration of alpha were not affected by the dark pulse, the intensity of wheel-running during alpha was significantly reduced from ~5.7 to ~4.7 revolutions/min (p < 0.05). Taken together, these data (Table 2) indicate that in addition to phasic resetting, dark pulses have prolonged effects on murine behavioral rhythms.
Table 2.
Circadian Parameter | Pre-Dark Pulse | Post-Dark Pulse |
tau (h) | 24.77 ± 0.05 | 24.67 ± 0.04 |
Percentage of Variance Explained by the Dominant Rhythmic Component (PV%) | 31.1 ± 1.6 | 28.6 ± 1.6* |
Alpha Duration (h) | 12.6 ± 0.5 | 12.4 ± 0.4 |
Motor Activity (RPM) | 5.71 ± 0.9 | 4.7 ± 0.73* |
*p < 0.05
Immunohistochemistry
Circadian variation in expression and activation of OXA neurons
Consistent with previous reports in other rodent species [11-13,20,21], OXA-ir was detected within the cytoplasm and processes of a population of hypothalamic neurons. OXA-ir neurons were mostly restricted to the TH, with the lateral TH containing more immunopositive cells than the medial TH. OXA-ir fibres and terminals were found in many brain areas including the PeriSCN region and within the IGL, MRN, and dorsal raphe (DRN) (Additional file 1). Using c-Fos-ir as a marker of neuronal activation, we assessed the numbers of activated cells in the medial and lateral TH as well as within key structures of the neural circadian system. Sections from animals under the LL condition were used as controls against those examined during and after dark pulse exposure. Single-labeled OXA-containing neurons, single-labeled c-Fos-ir, and those OXA-ir neurons double-labeled with c-Fos-ir were counted. In the medial TH, one-way ANOVA indicated a highly significant effect of circadian time on neuronal activation (F6,23 = 8.47, p < 0.001). Planned contrast comparison of the mean c-Fos-ir expression level during subjective day (collapsed across CT5, 6, and 9), with the mean c-Fos-ir expression during subjective night (collapsed across CT16, 17, and 20), revealed a highly significant increase in c-Fos-ir during subjective night (p < 0.001; Figure 3A). The number of OXA-ir neurons did not vary as a function of circadian time (p > 0.05; Figure 3C), but there was a highly significant effect of circadian time on the number of OXA-ir cells that co-expressed c-Fos-ir (F6,23 = 4.63, p < 0.01); with more OXA-ir neurons activated during the subjective night than the subjective day (p < 0.0001; Figure 3E). A separate comparison of the number of c-Fos-ir OXA neurons at CT9 (1.2 ± 0.5) and CT12 (7.4 ± 2.3) revealed a significant increase (p < 0.05) at the transition to subjective night, indicating that activation of OXA cells in the medial TH begins prior to the onset of vigorous wheel-running. c-Fos-ir in non-OXA neurons did not show this pattern of activation (p > 0.05).
Similar trends were observed in the lateral TH. Here c-Fos-ir expression varied significantly across the circadian cycle (F6,23 = 4.44, p < 0.01) with substantially more activated cells visualized from subjective night time points than from the subjective day (p < 0.001; Figure 3B). The number of OXA-ir neurons did not vary across the circadian cycle in LL (p > 0.05; Figure 3D). However, the number of OXA-ir neurons co-expressing c-Fos-ir varied across the circadian cycle (F6,23 = 4.91, p < 0.01), with significantly more double-labeled cells detected during the subjective night compared with the subjective day (p < 0.0001; Figure 3F). A separate comparison of the number of c-Fos-ir OXA neurons at CT9 (4.6 ± 2.5) and CT12 (19.7 ± 5.1) revealed a significant increase (p < 0.05) at the transition to subjective night, indicating that activation of OXA cells in the lateral TH begins prior to the onset of vigorous wheel-running. Again, c-Fos-ir in non-OXA neurons did not show this pattern of activation (p > 0.05).
From these data, it is clear that in the lateral and medial TH, cellular activation displays a pronounced circadian profile (including neurons containing OXA). Parametrically, it appears that the lateral TH has more activated OXA-ir neurons at subjective night than in the medial TH. However, when the data are normalized, the medial TH OXA-ir population shows the greater activation, with double-labeled OXA-ir cells rising from ~9.5% during the subjective day to ~60% during the subjective night. In the lateral TH, activated OXA-ir cells rose less dramatically, from ~6.4% of OXA-ir neurons during the subjective day to ~37% during the subjective night. Moreover, OXA neurons in the medial and lateral TH (but not non-OXA neurons) express significantly more c-Fos-ir at CT12. Since detection of c-Fos-ir in neurons typically occurs >60 min following an appropriate stimulus [22], these increases cannot be attributable to the commencement of wheel-running at CT12 and instead must be due to a circadian signal. Activity in the SCN of nocturnal mice is high during the behaviorally quiescent subjective day and low during the active subjective night. Available data indicate that SCN output signals suppress locomotor activity during the day, with the decline of this output at late day/early subjective night permitting locomotor activity. Therefore, the maximal levels of c-Fos/OXA co-expression seen around the mid-subjective night are presumably due to the combined actions of circadian disinhibition and feedback from behavioral arousal including forward locomotion.
Do dark pulses activate orexin neurons?
We examined whether a stimulus, which promotes arousal and phase-dependent SCN resetting, could alter cellular activation within the medial and/or lateral TH. This was established by administering a 6 h dark pulse, initiated at either CT5 or 16, and determining the expression of c-Fos-ir in both OXA-ir positive and negative cells. At these timepoints, the SCN is most and least responsive respectively, to the phase-resetting actions of a dark pulse (see dark pulse PRC above). Mice dark pulsed at CT5 were sampled 1 h (CT6) and 4 h (CT9) following pulse onset, and 1 h after reinstatement of light (CT12). Animals dark pulsed at CT16 were sampled 1 h (CT17) and 4 h (CT20) following the onset of dark. All sets of tissue were compared to time-matched unpulsed LL controls. Representative photomicrographs showing c-Fos-ir and OXA-ir the medial and lateral TH at CT6 and CT9 under unpulsed and pulsed conditions are shown in Figure 4. In all samples, the number of detectable OXA-ir cells did not change significantly between animals or lighting condition (data not shown).
In the medial TH, dark pulse exposure at CT5, significantly elevated c-Fos-ir 4 h (CT9) into the pulse (p < 0.05; Figure 5A), but not at other timepoints (CT6 and CT12) examined. In the lateral TH, dark pulse exposure had no significant effects on c-Fos-ir at these timepoints (Figure 5B). In comparison, in medial and lateral TH, the number of OXA-ir neurons expressing c-Fos increased significantly at both the CT6 and CT9 analysis time-points (medial TH: p < 0.01 and p < 0.05 respectively and lateral TH: both p < 0.05 respectively; Figures. 4C–J and 5C, D). Further, the extent of co-localization differed modestly between areas. More than half the total medial OXA-ir cells (~58%) became activated during the pulse, whereas ~40% of lateral TH OXA-ir cells were activated. Additionally, the finding that non-OXA-ir cells in medial TH become activated by this stimulus, establishes that dark pulse activation is a feature exhibited by many cells types in this part of the hypothalamus.
For animals that were exposed to a dark pulse beginning at CT16, no significant change in total c-Fos-ir was detected in the medial TH at any time-point sampled. However, there was a significant suppression of OXA-ir activated cells at 1 h (CT17) after pulse initiation (p < 0.01; Figure 5C) with the proportion of activated OXA-ir cells reducing from ~60% to ~52%. Conversely, lateral TH expression of c-Fos-ir was significantly reduced 1 h (CT17) but not 4 h (CT20) into the pulse (p < 0.05; Figure 5B). There was no effect on activated OXA cells at either CT (Figure 5D). Overall, a dark pulse centered around the end of the subjective night modestly, and transiently, suppressed OXA activation in the medial TH, while reducing non-OXA cell activation levels in the lateral TH.
Effect of a dark pulse on c-FOS-ir within the SCN, IGL, MRN and DRN structures
Since OXA-ir neurons of the TH are anatomically linked to many components of the neural circadian system, including the SCN, IGL, MRN, and DRN, we assessed if the activational state of cells in these regions varied in response to a dark pulse beginning at CT5. In unpulsed LL controls at CT6 and CT9, c-Fos-ir was spontaneously expressed throughout all levels of the SCN. At 4 h, but not 1 h, into the 6 h dark pulse, c-Fos-ir in the SCN was significantly reduced (unpaired t-test, p < 0.01 Figure 6A). Qualitatively, the area most prominently suppressed by the dark pulse appeared to be the rostral SCN and preliminary inspections indicated both 'core' and 'shell' regions [23] of the mid-rostrocaudal SCN were similarly affected by the dark pulse (data not shown). In both the IGL and DRN, small but non-significant increases in c-Fos-ir were coincident with dark pulse exposure (Figure 6B, C). More robust changes in cellular activation were observed within the MRN, where the number of c-Fos-ir cells became significantly elevated 4 h into the dark pulse (unpaired t-test, p < 0.01; Figure 6D). Collectively, these data indicate that a dark pulse given during the subjective day significantly and differentially affects cell activation in the neural circadian system, specifically suppressing SCN cells while activating MRN cells.
Discussion
This study demonstrates unequivocally that orexin neuronal activation is under pronounced circadian control and further that orexin neurons are selectively activated by an SCN-resetting arousal-promoting stimulus. We also highlight potential differences in the activation state of orexin neurons in the medial and lateral TH. These findings reveal new complexities of SCN-TH interactions in the regulation of brain states and behavior.
Consistent with studies in Syrian hamster and Swiss mouse, we found that a 6 h dark pulse robustly phase-advances C57BL/6J wheel-running rhythms when initiated during the mid- to late subjective day [24-30]. Further, our observation that the magnitude of dark pulse-evoked wheel-running activity does not necessarily correlate with the magnitude of the phase shift is also in broad agreement with other studies [31-33]. Previously, dark pulses given during the late subjective night/early subjective day were found to elicit phase-delays in Syrian hamster and Swiss mouse wheel-running rhythms [24-26]. Although we did observe that some C57BL/6J mice phase-delayed to dark pulses given at this time of the circadian cycle (see Figures 2 and 3), delays were not consistently evoked. This discrepancy is likely to be due to a bona fide species-related difference, as we found that Syrian hamsters housed under identical conditions invariably show significant phase-delays to 6 h dark pulses beginning around CT16 and extending to early subjective day [26]. Collectively, these data suggest that the C57BL/6J mouse is more readily phase-advanced than phase-delayed by 6 h dark pulses.
Our study complements and extends earlier research in which temporal variation in orexin cell activation was determined primarily under diurnal conditions [20,34,35] or limited (two time points) circadian sampling frequencies [17]. Under LL conditions in which the amplitude and intensity of murine wheel-running are greatly suppressed, we detected a highly significant circadian profile in the levels of activated of OXA-ir and non-OXA-ir neurons in the medial and lateral TH. Here, neurons demonstrated substantially higher levels of c-Fos-ir during the subjective night phase. Interestingly, we detected significant (but sub-maximal) increases in OXA-ir/c-Fos-ir double-labeled neurons in both the medial and lateral TH at CT12 compared to CT9. Because detection of c-Fos-ir in neurons typically occurs ~60 min following an appropriate stimulus, this is evidence that OXA cells are not simply reactive to the initiation of locomotor activity at CT12 and are subject to genuine circadian control. However, because maximal levels of OXA activation are not seen until the mid subjective night (when locomotor activity is well established) (see Figure 5C, D) it is likely that behavioral feedback mechanisms further activate OXA cells. This circadian pattern of orexin neuron activation is consistent with the circadian profile of orexin detected in the cerebrospinal fluid of nocturnal rodents; the amplitude of which is suppressed by exposure to LL and abolished in SCN-lesioned animals [36,37]. This indicates that c-Fos-labelling as a marker of neuronal activation corresponds with the release of OXA.
The effects of dark exposure during the mid-subjective day on c-Fos-ir expression in the circadian system support earlier findings from Syrian hamster [27]. A significant suppression of c-Fos-ir in the SCN was detected during the daytime dark pulse. This was accompanied by activation of MRN neurons, OXA-ir and non-OXA-ir neurons in the medial TH, and OXA-ir neurons in the lateral TH. Projections from cells in the IGL and MRN to the SCN are both important for the phase-advancing actions of various non-photic stimuli [3,8,38]. However, we observed that unlike the MRN, daytime dark pulses did not robustly activate neurons in the IGL and DRN. This suggests that cells in MRN are more involved in dark pulse evoked phase shifts than those of the IGL or DRN. Consistent with this interpretation, Coogan and Piggins [27] did not observe significant increases in c-Fos-ir labeling in the hamster IGL following a daytime dark pulse. Similarly, Harrington and Rusak [28] found that IGL lesions do not abolish the resetting actions of dark pulses in Syrian hamsters. Although other non-photic stimuli, such as running in a novel wheel, can induce c-Fos-ir in the hamster IGL [39,40], activation of the geniculo-hypothalamic tract (GHT) does not appear to be critical for dark-pulse evoked phase advances. Electrical stimulation of GABA and serotonin containing-neurons in the MRN reduces light-evoked c-Fos-ir in the SCN and similar effects have been documented with exogenous application of serotonergic or GABAergic agonists into the SCN [8,41-43]. Moreover, analogous to daytime dark pulses, serotonergic agonists given during the subjective day phase advance rodent wheel-running rhythms and suppress clock gene expression in the rodent SCN [44,45]. Further, serotonergic agonists given during the subjective day can potentiate dark pulse evoked phase advances, supporting the contention that the MRN-SCN pathway is involved dark pulse resetting of circadian rhythms [45].
Within the TH, our results suggest functional differences in the circadian and photic regulation of both orexin and non-orexin neuronal groups. Cells in the TH demonstrated circadian variation in c-Fos-ir expression, and in the degree of activated OXA-ir neurons. In the medial and lateral TH, the basal level of OXA-ir activation during the subjective day is similar (~10% and ~7% respectively), but across subjective night, the medial TH OXA-ir cells show a larger activation, with ~60% containing c-Fos-ir while ~42% of lateral OXA are activated. When exposed to a 6 h dark pulse during the subjective day, it is also the medial TH that expresses the largest increase in the proportion of OXA-ir neurons co-expressing c-Fos-ir. Complementary findings have been published recently by Webb et al. [40] who report that hamsters confined to running wheels (an arousal-inducing procedure) demonstrate a greater proportion of OXA-ir/c-Fos-ir double-labeled cells in medial TH areas compared with lateral TH areas.
Intriguingly, dark pulse exposure at subjective night causes a small but significant decrease in OXA-ir/c-Fos-ir co-localization in the medial TH, without affecting the activational state of non-OXA-ir cells. A different pattern is seen in the lateral TH; here it is the non-OXA-ir cells whose activation is suppressed, while the OXA-ir neurons are not significantly affected. These data support suggestions that different populations of orexin neurons undergo differential tonic regulation and respond differently to various stimuli [17,46,47]. Here, activation of OXA neurons in the medial TH appears more tightly linked to circadian regulation of behavioral arousal than those located laterally. This may be a reflection of the extensive innervation of medial hypothalamic sites by SCN efferents and/or their proximity to the third ventricle, in which cerebrospinal fluid borne SCN-derived signals can be communicated [48]. Additionally, SCN-controlled peripheral signals such as corticosterone can potentially alter orexin cellular activity [49].
Orexin neurons are ideally positioned to integrate arousal and circadian cues. The results of the present study, as well as those of other researchers, indicates that the pronounced difference in the activation state of orexin neurons from the quiescent subjective day to the behaviorally active night, is likely caused by both circadian influences and arousal/behavioral feedback. During the subjective day, SCN neuronal activity is maximal and, as the SCN output signal is believed to be predominantly inhibitory, acts to suppress other brain areas including the TH (Figure 7A, E). When neuronal activity in the SCN declines, as seen before subjective night, there is a significant rise in c-fos expression in orexin cells which occurs at least 1 h prior to the onset of vigorous subjective nighttime wheel-running (Figure 7B, E). Together, the alteration in SCN input results in a disinhibition of orexin neurons leading to arousal and priming of the animals behavior for waking. Further sustained activation of orexin neurons during the subjective night arises through behavioral feedback and activation of IGL-SCN and MRN-SCN pathways, which in turn, facilitate sustained suppression of the SCN (Figure 7C, E). Accordingly, the cessation of subjective night develops through increased SCN neuronal output through the late night and early subjective day.
Phase-shifting actions of dark pulses can also be explained by similar mechanisms. Application of a daytime dark pulse causes an interruption of the excitatory photic input to the SCN, which results in reduced inhibitory output drive from the SCN. The consequence of this is disinhibition and activation of the orexin cells, and other neuronal populations, to promote arousal and behavioral feedback (Figure 7D, E). The latter effect activates the MRN-SCN pathway, leading to further suppression of SCN neurons. Termination of the dark pulse restores the excitatory light input to the SCN, activating SCN cells and elevating inhibitory output signals to many brain regions, ending neuronal activation in orexin and non-orexin cells. Based on our present data, we predict that MRN-SCN projections are more important than IGL-SCN projections under these conditions. This is in contrast to other arousal promoting non-photic stimuli, such as novelty induced locomotion, for which the GHT projection between the IGL and the SCN appears critical for clock resetting [32].
An additional contributing factor to the phase resetting actions of daytime dark pulses is sleep deprivation. In Syrian hamsters, it is reported that it is sleep deprivation occurring during the dark pulse rather than wheel-running activity that determines phase-resetting to this stimulus [33]. Here, in our study on mice, wheel-running activity during the pulse does not correlate with the magnitude of the phase shift. However, in a small cohort of mice (n = 4) visually monitored with infra-red goggles during the dark pulse given CT5–11, we found that the mice rested a lot less and engaged in many more active behaviors (general exploration, cage-top climbing, etc) than had been recorded in these same animals over CT5–11 in LL conditions (Marston and Piggins, unpublished observations). Since orexin release is enhanced by sleep deprivation [50], this indicates another mechanism via which dark pulses can regulate these and other TH cells.
OXA may also have other direct and indirect effects on SCN neuronal activity. For example, in addition to innervating the IGL and MRN, OXA-ir fibres project to the Peri-SCN region [12,51] and OX1R protein is detectable within the rat SCN [52]. This raises the possibility that locally released OXA may act on the SCN. Indeed, we recently showed that OXA and OXB can alter rat SCN neuronal activity in vitro, with some actions involving recruitment of GABAergic mechanisms [53]. Such actions of locally released orexins on the SCN may feedback to modulate TH cellular activity since one study has described SCN projections to OXA neurons [15].
Conclusion
Collectively, our findings raise the possibility that activation of OXA neurons is involved in resetting of the SCN circadian clock and further allude to the pathway from the MRN to the SCN being functionally more important than the GHT for dark pulse-induced phase-shifting. Moreover, our findings identify at least two separate populations of OXA neurons that are differentially activated by endogenous and exogenous cues, thereby enabling the orexin system to collectively 'multitask' and coordinate appropriate physiological and behavioral states.
Methods
Adult male C57BL/6J mice were purchased from Harlan (Loughborough, UK). All procedures were carried out in accordance the Animal (Scientific Procedures) act, 1986 (UK). Animals were initially group housed (6–8 animals per cage) for 1–2 weeks under a 12 h:12 h light/dark cycle (LD; lights-on 07:00) before being individually housed in running wheel-equipped (stainless steel, 16 cm diameter, North Kent Plastics, Kent, UK) polycarbon cages (overall dimensions 45 cm × 21 cm × 19 cm). Individually housed mice were kept under 12 h:12 h LD cycle for a further 10 days and then released into constant light (LL; light intensity at cage level ~100 lux) to establish stable free-running activity rhythms for at least 10 circadian cycles prior to experimental procedures. Food (B&K Universal, Hull, UK) and water were available ad libitum in all conditions.
Each revolution of the running wheel depressed an externally mounted microswitch, the activation of which was recorded and counted via a PC using the collect module of the Chronobiology Kit software suite (Stanford Software Systems, Santa Cruz, CA, USA). Actograms of wheel-running rhythms and wheel revolution counts were produced with the Analyze 9 module (Stanford Software Systems). Changes in endogenous period (tau) under LD and LL conditions were estimated by the chi-square periodogram. Wheel-running activity rhythms were visualized offline as double-plotted actograms. The El Temps programme (Dr. Noguerra, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain) was used to measure a number of key properties of the wheel-running rhythm and to determine the effects of exposure to LL and dark pulses (see [26] for further details).
Dark Pulse Phase-Response Curve
To generate a phase-response curve (PRC) to dark pulses, mice (n = 25) were given a 6 h dark pulse every 10–16 days at various points across the circadian cycle. Dark pulses were initiated by turning off the room lights for 6 h and terminated by reinstatement of light. Animals remained undisturbed in their home cages throughout the dark pulse. All animals received at least one dark pulse, with some animals receiving up to 3 pulses over 60 circadian cycles.
Steady state phase shifts were independently assessed by three experienced researchers (two blind to experimental conditions) using the line of best-fit method [25,27,54]. Briefly, two lines of best fit were drawn through the onsets of wheel-running activity (CT12 by convention). The first line delineated activity onset (CT12) over the 7 cycles prior to the dark pulse. This line was extrapolated to predict CT12 on the first cycle following the pulse. The second line identified activity onset over the 7–10 cycles following the dark pulse (for the purposes of line-fitting, circadian cycles containing transients were discounted from this process). Interpolation of the second line also allowed CT12 to be predicted on the first cycle following the dark pulse. The difference (in min) between the CT12 values obtained by extrapolation and interpolation was taken to be the steady state phase shift elicited by the dark pulse. Animals with unstable behavioral rhythms were excluded from analysis.
Steady state phase shifts in response to dark pulses across the circadian cycle were initially graphed on a scatterplot to aid visualization. For analysis, phase shift data were grouped into 4 h bins (e.g. dark pulses centered between CT2–6, 6–10 etc). Each bin contained at least 6 data points (range n = 6–16) with no animal contributing more than a single data point to any individual CT bin. Where an animal received more than a single pulse within a specific 4 h period the values were averaged to provide a single value. The relationship between intensity of wheel-running activity induced by a subjective day dark pulse and the magnitude of resulting phase shifts was examined by parametric correlation.
Immunohistochemistry
In the immunolabeling experiment, mice (n = 67) were sampled either in LL (for use as time-matched unpulsed controls), or at 1 h or 4 h into a 6 h dark pulse (initiated at either CT5 or CT16). In addition, some mice were sampled 1 h after cessation of the CT5 dark pulse (CT12). Animals were culled by cervical dislocation and decapitation under halothane anesthesia. Brains were carefully removed from the skull and placed in fixative [4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB)] for 4 days at 4°C. Following cryoprotection (30% sucrose in 0.1 M PB solution), brains were rapidly frozen with crushed dry ice and mounted onto the cryostage of a sledge microtome (Series 8000, Bright Instruments Ltd., Huntingdon, UK) and cut into 30 μm thick coronal sections (four serial sets of sections per brain).
Free-floating sections were washed 3 × 10 min in 0.1 M PB then incubated for 20 min in 1.5% hydrogen peroxide made in 0.1 M PB (Sigma, Poole, UK) to remove endogenous peroxidase activity. Sections were washed (3 × 10 min in 0.1 M PB) and subsequently incubated for 60 min in a blocking solution of 5% normal donkey serum (NDS; Sigma, Poole, UK) in 0.1 M PB; then transferred into the primary antibody (made up in 5% NDS) for 36–48 h at 4°C. The primary antibodies used were polyclonal goat anti-orexin-A (1:1000; Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Santa Cruz, CA, USA) and polyclonal rabbit anti-c-Fos (1:8000; Santa Cruz Biotechnology).
After incubation the sections were returned to room temperature (22°C) and washed in 0.1 M PB (3 × 10 min) before addition for 90 min in an appropriate biotinylated IgG secondary antibody (1:400 donkey anti-goat or 1:400 donkey anti-rabbit in 0.1 M PB; Jackson Immunoresearch, West Grove, PA, USA). Sections were washed (3 × 10 min in 0.1 M PB) then transferred to avidin-biotin complex (ABC) solution (1:200; Vector Laboratories, Peterborough, UK) for 90 min. Slices were then prepared for enzymatic detection after two further 10 min 0.1 M PB washes. For c-Fos-ir development, sections were placed for 10 min in a 0.1 M sodium acetate solution (pH 6.0) and then exposed to a nickel-intensified diaminobenzidine chromagen (NiDAB). The reaction was catalyzed with 0.015% glucose oxidase (Sigma), yielding a blue/black reaction stain. For OXA-ir double-labeling, sections were returned to 1.5% hydrogen peroxide before being rerun in the previous steps with the corresponding primary and secondary antibody until enzymatic detection. Diaminobenzidine chromagen without nickel intensification (DAB), catalyzed with 0.015% hydrogen peroxide produced a red/brown reaction product indicating OXA detection. The reaction was quenched in dH2O. Stained sections were rinsed in ~0.005 M PB and mounted onto gelatin coated slides and air-dried. They were then dehydrated through a series of graded ethanol baths (70%, 95% and 2 × 100%), cleared in Histoclear (National Diagnostics, Hull, UK) and air-dried before coverslipping.
Sections were visually inspected under an Olympus BX-50 microscope and photographed with an attached digital camera (Olympus c-4000 z). High resolution digital images of representative rostral, intermediate and caudal SCN, IGL, MRN and DRN sections were taken and imported into Microsoft PowerPoint (XP edition; Microsoft, Seattle, WA, USA). The SCN, IGL, MRN and DRN were delineated from adjacent neuroanatomical structures with reference to a stereotaxic atlas [55] and a series of cresyl violet stained C57BL/6J brain sections (provided by Dr David Cutler). Another experienced researcher confirmed accuracy of neuroanatomical delineation. OXA-ir neurons and c-Fos-ir nuclei were counted directly under microscope examination. Two sections (both hemispheres) per rostral, mid- and caudal level of the TH were analyzed from each animal (6 sections/animal). Using a modification of the medial/lateral subdivisions described by other researchers [17,46], we divided the TH into two major divisions, medial and lateral, defined by two counting boxes (each 1.05 mm in height × 0.7 mm in width) corresponding to stereotaxic coordinates from [55] with the medial box: ~4.7–~5.75 mm height × ~0.7 mm from midline; and the lateral box: ~4.7–~5.75 mm height × ~0.7–~1.4 mm from midline) single-labeled and co-localized cells were counted (See additional file 2). All counts were conducted blind to experimental conditions and verified by an independent researcher experienced in cellular and nuclear counting techniques. Immunostaining is expressed as average values/animal/timepoint.
Statistical analysis
To evaluate the effects of constant light on key features of the wheel-running rhythms we used paired t-tests. To determine the significance of phase shifts to dark pulses, we collapsed the data into 4 h circadian time bins and compared the mean ± 95% confidence interval with 0 min shift and interpreted the absence of an overlap as significant. One way analysis of variance was used for evaluating the significance of circadian time on c-Fos and OXA cellular labeling, with planned comparisons of mean levels at day-night phases. Unpaired t-tests were used as described. The JMP version 6 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC) and Prism Graphpad version 4 (Graphpad Software Inc. San Diego, CA) software packages were used for statistics and construction of graphs.
Abbreviations
DRN: Dorsal raphe; GHT: geniculohypothalamic tract; IGL: intergeniculate leaflet; LD: light-dark; LL: constant light; MRN: median raphe; OXA: orexin A; PRC: phase-response curve; PVE: percentage of variance; SCN: suprachiasmatic nuclei; TH: tuberal hypothalamus.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors' contributions
This manuscript is based on doctoral research completed by OJM with additional analysis and interpretation by RHW. OJM, RHW, and RES did the immunohistochemistry and quantification of immunostaining, OJM and MMC analyzed the behavioral data, and NU advised and provided financial support for the study, while OJM and HDP performed the statistical tests. OJM, RHW, and HDP wrote the paper.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements
Supported by project grants from the BBSRC to HDP and by an MRC-GSK Industrial Collaborative Studentship to OJM. We thank Johan Oldekopp for technical assistance.
Contributor Information
Oliver J Marston, Email: om246@cam.ac.uk.
Rhîannan H Williams, Email: rhiannan.williams@postgrad.manchester.ac.uk.
Maria M Canal, Email: maria.canal@manchester.ac.uk.
Rayna E Samuels, Email: mqbssres@manchester.ac.ukom.
Neil Upton, Email: neil.upton@gsk.com.
Hugh D Piggins, Email: hugh.piggins@manchester.ac.uk.
References
- Morin LP, Allen CN. The circadian visual system, 2005. Brain Res Rev. 2006;51:1–60. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rusak B, Zucker I. Neural regulation of circadian rhythms. Physiol Rev. 1979;59:449–526. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1979.59.3.449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mrosovsky N. Locomotor activity and non-photic influences on circadian clocks. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 1996;71:343–372. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1996.tb01278.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mistlberger RE, Skene DJ. Nonphotic entrainment in humans? J Biol Rhythms. 2005;20:339–352. doi: 10.1177/0748730405277982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hannibal J. Neurotransmitters of the retino-hypothalamic tract. Cell Tissue Res. 2002;309:73–88. doi: 10.1007/s00441-002-0574-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berson DM. Strange vision: ganglion cells as circadian photoreceptors. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:314–320. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00130-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrington ME. The ventral lateral geniculate nucleus and the intergeniculate leaflet: interrelated structures in the visual and circadian systems. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1997;21:705–727. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(96)00019-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morin LP. Serotonin and the regulation of mammalian circadian rhythmicity. Ann Med. 1999;31:12–33. doi: 10.3109/07853899909019259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Lecea L, Kilduff TS, Peyron C, Gao X, Foye PE, Danielson PE, Fukuhara C, Battenberg EL, Gautvik VT, Bartlett FS, 2nd, et al. The hypocretins: hypothalamus-specific peptides with neuroexcitatory activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:322–327. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.1.322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakurai T, Amemiya A, Ishii M, Matsuzaki I, Chemelli RM, Tanaka H, Williams SC, Richardson JA, Kozlowski GP, Wilson S, et al. Orexins and orexin receptors: a family of hypothalamic neuropeptides and G protein-coupled receptors that regulate feeding behavior. Cell. 1998;92:573–585. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)80949-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cutler DJ, Morris R, Sheridhar V, Wattam TA, Holmes S, Patel S, Arch JR, Wilson S, Buckingham RE, Evans ML, et al. Differential distribution of orexin-A and orexin-B immunoreactivity in the rat brain and spinal cord. Peptides. 1999;20:1455–1470. doi: 10.1016/S0196-9781(99)00157-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGranaghan PA, Piggins HD. Orexin A-like immunoreactivity in the hypothalamus and thalamus of the Syrian hamster (Mesocricetus auratus) and Siberian hamster (Phodopus sungorus), with special reference to circadian structures. Brain Res. 2001;904:234–244. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02463-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peyron C, Tighe DK, Pol AN van den, de Lecea L, Heller HC, Sutcliffe JG, Kilduff TS. Neurons containing hypocretin (orexin) project to multiple neuronal systems. J Neurosci. 1998;18:9996–10015. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-23-09996.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deurveilher S, Semba K. Indirect projections from the suprachiasmatic nucleus to major arousal-promoting cell groups in rat: implications for the circadian control of behavioural state. Neuroscience. 2005;130:165–183. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.08.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abrahamson EE, Leak RK, Moore RY. The suprachiasmatic nucleus projects to posterior hypothalamic arousal systems. Neuroreport. 2001;12:435–440. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200102120-00048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nishino S, Sakarai T, Eds. The orexin/hypocretin system: Physiology and pathophysiology. Totowa, New Jersey: Humana Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Estabrooke IV, McCarthy MT, Ko E, Chou TC, Chemelli RM, Yanagisawa M, Saper CB, Scammell TE. Fos expression in orexin neurons varies with behavioral state. J Neurosci. 2001;21:1656–1662. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-05-01656.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Espana RA, Scammell TE. Orexin and hypothalamic control of sleeping and waking. In: Nishino S, Sakurai T, editor. The Orexin/hypocretin system: Physiology and and pathophysiology. Totowa, New Jersey: Humana Press; 2006. pp. 189–207. [Google Scholar]
- Mileykovskiy BY, Kiyashchenko LI, Siegel JM. Behavioral correlates of activity in identified hypocretin/orexin neurons. Neuron. 2005;46:787–798. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nixon JP, Smale L. Individual differences in wheel-running rhythms are related to temporal and spatial patterns of activation of orexin A and B cells in a diurnal rodent (Arvicanthis niloticus) Neuroscience. 2004;127:25–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.04.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mintz EM, Pol AN van den, Casano AA, Albers HE. Distribution of hypocretin-(orexin) immunoreactivity in the central nervous system of Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) J Chem Neuroanat. 2001;21:225–238. doi: 10.1016/S0891-0618(01)00111-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharp FR, Sagar SM, Hicks K, Lowenstein D, Hisanaga K. c-fos mRNA, Fos, and Fos-related antigen induction by hypertonic saline and stress. J Neurosci. 1991;11:2321–2331. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-08-02321.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yan L, Silver R. Differential induction and localization of mPer1 and mPer2 during advancing and delaying phase shifts. Eur J Neurosci. 2002;16:1531–1540. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02224.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbacka-Surowiak G. Is the PRC for dark pulses in LL a mirror image of light pulses in DD in mice? Biol Rhythm Res. 2000;31:531–544. doi: 10.1076/brhm.31.5.531.5659. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Boulos Z, Rusak B. Circadian phase response curve for dark pulses in hamsters. J Comp Physiol [A] 1982;146:411–417. doi: 10.1007/BF00609437. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Canal MM, Piggins HD. Resetting of the hamster circadian system by dark pulses. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2006;290:R785–792. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00548.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coogan AN, Piggins HD. Dark pulse suppression of P-ERK and c-Fos in the hamster suprachiasmatic nuclei. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22:158–168. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04193.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrington ME, Rusak B. Lesions of the thalamic intergeniculate leaflet alter hamster circadian rhythms. J Biol Rhythms. 1986;1:309–325. doi: 10.1177/074873048600100405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis GB, McKlveen RE, Turek FW. Dark pulses affect the circadian rhythm of activity in hamsters kept in constant light. Am J Physiol. 1982;242:R44–50. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1982.242.1.R44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendoza JY, Dardente H, Escobar C, Pevet P, Challet E. Dark pulse resetting of the suprachiasmatic clock in Syrian hamsters: behavioral phase-shifts and clock gene expression. Neuroscience. 2004;127:529–537. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.05.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antle MC, Mistlberger RE. Circadian clock resetting by sleep deprivation without exercise in the Syrian hamster. J Neurosci. 2000;20:9326–9332. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-24-09326.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biello SM, Janik D, Mrosovsky N. Neuropeptide Y and behaviorally induced phase shifts. Neuroscience. 1994;62:273–279. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90331-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mistlberger RE, Belcourt J, Antle MC. Circadian clock resetting by sleep deprivation without exercise in Syrian hamsters: dark pulses revisited. J Biol Rhythms. 2002;17:227–237. doi: 10.1177/07430402017003006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Espana RA, Valentino RJ, Berridge CW. Fos immunoreactivity in hypocretin-synthesizing and hypocretin-1 receptor-expressing neurons: effects of diurnal and nocturnal spontaneous waking, stress and hypocretin-1 administration. Neuroscience. 2003;121:201–217. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00334-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez GS, Smale L, Nunez AA. Diurnal and nocturnal rodents show rhythms in orexinergic neurons. Brain Res. 2002;955:1–7. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)03264-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deboer T, Overeem S, Visser NA, Duindam H, Frolich M, Lammers GJ, Meijer JH. Convergence of circadian and sleep regulatory mechanisms on hypocretin-1. Neuroscience. 2004;129:727–732. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.07.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang S, Zeitzer JM, Yoshida Y, Wisor JP, Nishino S, Edgar DM, Mignot E. Lesions of the suprachiasmatic nucleus eliminate the daily rhythm of hypocretin-1 release. Sleep. 2004;27:619–627. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.4.619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marchant EG, Watson NV, Mistlberger RE. Both neuropeptide Y and serotonin are necessary for entrainment of circadian rhythms in mice by daily treadmill running schedules. J Neurosci. 1997;17:7974–7987. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-20-07974.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikkelsen JD, Vrang N, Mrosovsky N. Expression of Fos in the circadian system following nonphotic stimulation. Brain Res Bull. 1998;47:367–376. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(98)00121-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Webb IC, Patton DF, Hamson DK, Mistlberger RE. Neural correlates of arousal-induced circadian clock resetting: hypocretin/orexin and the intergeniculate leaflet. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;27:828–835. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06074.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillespie CF, Beek EM Van Der, Mintz EM, Mickley NC, Jasnow AM, Huhman KL, Albers HE. GABAergic regulation of light-induced c-Fos immunoreactivity within the suprachiasmatic nucleus. J Comp Neurol. 1999;411:683–692. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990906)411:4<683::AID-CNE12>3.0.CO;2-J. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morin LP, Meyer-Bernstein EL. The ascending serotonergic system in the hamster: comparison with projections of the dorsal and median raphe nuclei. Neuroscience. 1999;91:81–105. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(98)00585-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickard GE, Rea MA. Serotonergic innervation of the hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nucleus and photic regulation of circadian rhythms. Biol Cell. 1997;89:513–523. doi: 10.1016/S0248-4900(98)80007-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horikawa K, Yokota S, Fuji K, Akiyama M, Moriya T, Okamura H, Shibata S. Nonphotic entrainment by 5-HT1A/7 receptor agonists accompanied by reduced Per1 and Per2 mRNA levels in the suprachiasmatic nuclei. J Neurosci. 2000;20:5867–5873. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-15-05867.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendoza J, Clesse D, Pevet P, Challet E. Serotonergic potentiation of dark pulse-induced phase-shifting effects at midday in hamsters. J Neurochem. 2008;106:1404–1414. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05493.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fadel J, Bubser M, Deutch AY. Differential activation of orexin neurons by antipsychotic drugs associated with weight gain. J Neurosci. 2002;22:6742–6746. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-15-06742.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Satoh S, Matsumura H, Nakajima T, Nakahama K, Kanbayashi T, Nishino S, Yoneda H, Shigeyoshi Y. Inhibition of rostral basal forebrain neurons promotes wakefulness and induces FOS in orexin neurons. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:1635–1645. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02577.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver R, LeSauter J, Tresco PA, Lehman MN. A diffusible coupling signal from the transplanted suprachiasmatic nucleus controlling circadian locomotor rhythms. Nature. 1996;382:810–813. doi: 10.1038/382810a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ford GK, Al-Barazanji KA, Wilson S, Jones DN, Harbuz MS, Jessop DS. Orexin expression and function: glucocorticoid manipulation, stress, and feeding studies. Endocrinology. 2005;146:3724–3731. doi: 10.1210/en.2005-0496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoshida Y, Fujiki N, Nakajima T, Ripley B, Matsumura H, Yoneda H, Mignot E, Nishino S. Fluctuation of extracellular hypocretin-1 (orexin A) levels in the rat in relation to the light-dark cycle and sleep-wake activities. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;14:1075–1081. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01725.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Date Y, Ueta Y, Yamashita H, Yamaguchi H, Matsukura S, Kangawa K, Sakurai T, Yanagisawa M, Nakazato M. Orexins, orexigenic hypothalamic peptides, interact with autonomic, neuroendocrine and neuroregulatory systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1999;96:748–753. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.2.748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Backberg M, Hervieu G, Wilson S, Meister B. Orexin receptor-1 (OX-R1) immunoreactivity in chemically identified neurons of the hypothalamus: focus on orexin targets involved in control of food and water intake. Eur J Neurosci. 2002;15:315–328. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01859.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown TM, Coogan AN, Cutler DJ, Hughes AT, Piggins HD. Electrophysiological actions of orexins on rat suprachiasmatic neurons in vitro. Neurosci Lett. 2008;448:273–278. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.10.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daan S, Pittendrigh CS. A functional analysis of circadian pacemakers in nocturnal rodents. II. The variability of phase response curves. J Comp Physiol [A] 1976;106:253–266. doi: 10.1007/BF01417857. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G, Franklin KB. The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego: Academic Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.