Skip to main content
American Journal of Public Health logoLink to American Journal of Public Health
. 2008 Dec;98(12):2237–2243. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2007.109488

Childhood Mental Ability and Adult Alcohol Intake and Alcohol Problems: The 1970 British Cohort Study

G David Batty 1,, Ian J Deary 1, Ingrid Schoon 1, Carol Emslie 1, Kate Hunt 1, Catharine R Gale 1
PMCID: PMC2636535  PMID: 18235070

Abstract

Objectives. We examined the potential relation of mental ability test scores at age 10 years with alcohol problems and alcohol intake at age 30 years.

Methods. We used data from a prospective observational study involving 8170 members of a birth cohort from Great Britain born in 1970. Data included mental ability scores at age 10 years and responses to inquiries about alcohol intake and problems at age 30 years.

Results. After adjustment for potential mediating and confounding factors, cohort members with higher childhood mental ability scores had an increased prevalence of problem drinking in adulthood. This association was stronger among women (odds ratio [OR]1 SD increase in ability = 1.38; 95% confidence interval [CI] = 1.16, 1.64) than men (OR1 SD increase in ability = 1.17; CI = 1.04, 1.28; P for interaction = .004). Childhood mental ability was also related to a higher average intake of alcohol and to drinking more frequently. Again, these gradients were stronger among women than among men.

Conclusions. In this large-scale cohort study, higher childhood mental ability was related to alcohol problems and higher alcohol intake in adult life. These unexpected results warrant examination in other studies.


A widely accepted definition of intelligence (IQ) or mental ability is the ability to learn, reason, and solve problems.1 Mental ability may influence health and related behaviors because of the potential for variable interpretation of health promotion advice by persons with differing mental ability.2 If skills captured by mental ability tests (such as verbal comprehension and reasoning) are shown to be important in the successful management of a person's health behaviors, public health implications may include consideration of cognitive ability in the design of health promotion materials and in health professional–client interactions.3

In recent studies, mental ability has been linked to selected health behaviors. In extended follow-up of child cohorts, high childhood mental ability scores were associated with increased smoking cessation rates2 and more health-favorable levels of smoking initiation,4,5 physical activity,6 and food intake in later life.6,7 With alcohol intake as the outcome of interest, mental ability has been shown to have an inverse association with age at onset of use,8 a U-shaped association with heavy intake (21 drinks/wk),9 and a null relation with hazardous levels of intake (500 g of pure alcohol/mo)10 and total amount.11 These apparently discrepant findings may be partially explained by small sample sizes,10,11 highly selected samples,8,11 and differing definitions of outcomes.

Using data from the Aberdeen “Children of the 1950s” cohort, Batty et al. were able to examine, for the first time, the relation between mental ability and alcohol binge drinking, a pattern of consumption defined by peak intake on a single occasion as indexed by self-reported hangovers.3 They found that higher mental ability–scoring children had a reduced likelihood of alcohol-induced hangovers in middle age,3 an observation that is consistent with an inverse association between IQ and alcohol-related mortality seen in a group of Swedish military conscripts.12 Given that alcohol problems are characterized by patterns of intense drinking,13 we hypothesized that higher mental ability scores would reveal a similar inverse relation with alcohol drinking problems. We used data from the 1970 British Cohort Study to test this previously unexamined hypothesis. These data also afforded us the opportunity to contribute to the sparse literature on mental ability and alcohol consumption.

METHODS

The 1970 British Cohort Study is an ongoing longitudinal study involving participants from 17 198 live births occurring to parents who resided in Great Britain between April 5 and 11, 1970.14 We used data from 1980 to 1981, when study participants completed cognitive ability tests at age 10 years,15 and from 1999 to 2000 when, at age 30 years, they responded to enquiries about their alcohol consumption and other health behaviors.16

Data Collected at the 10-Year Follow-Up

Testing of the children's mental ability took place in schools when the children were age 10 years; a teacher-adapted version of the British Ability Scales17 was used. There were 4 subscales: word definitions, word similarities, recall of digits, and matrices. A detailed description of the tests is provided elsewhere.6,7 Test results were scored by trained coders. Reliability of coding was monitored throughout the survey and found to be high.18 In keeping with previous reports on this data set,57 we carried out a principal components analysis of these 4 tests to establish the presence of a general cognitive ability factor (also known as g).19 For ease of interpretation, we transformed this score into an IQ-type score, with a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15 (our index of mental ability). Information about current occupation and the educational attainment by both parents was collected during the interview with the child's parents. Social class of each parent was derived from current occupation using 4 categories (professional/managerial [highest], skilled nonmanual, skilled manual, and semiskilled/unskilled [lowest]), according to the 1980 Registrar General's Classification of Occupations.20

Data Collected at the 30-Year Follow-Up

Information was collected by interview in the adult participant's home. As part of a series of questions on health, enquiries were made about alcohol consumption and drinking problems. Participants were asked how frequently they had an alcoholic drink of any kind. Participants who said that they currently drank were asked to provide details of their alcohol intake during the last 7 days. All participants, apart from those who said they were lifelong teetotalers, were asked to complete 4 items from the CAGE (cut down, annoyed, guilty, eye-opener) questionnaire about possible lifetime alcohol-related problems.21 These items are used to create a drinking problem scale, with each positive response given a score of 1: Have you ever felt you ought to cut down on drinking? Have people annoyed you by criticizing your drinking? Have you ever felt bad or guilty about your drinking? Have you ever had a drink first thing in the morning (eye-opener) to steady your hands? The Cronbach α for the scale formed from these 4 items was 0.61, indicating moderate internal consistency for the CAGE questionnaire's total score. Scores of 2 or more positively endorsed items on this scale have been shown in community samples to indicate alcohol problems.22,23 Participants were also asked about their highest academic or vocational qualifications; these were subsequently coded into 6 categories (Table 1). Social class was derived from current occupation using the same 4 categories as for parental social class (Table 1). Participants were asked to provide information on their gross and net earnings from employment.

TABLE 1.

Characteristics and Childhood Mental Ability Score of Study Participants, by Gender: 1970 British Cohort Study, 1980–1981 and 1999–2000

Men
Women
No (%) Mental Ability Score, Mean (SD) No. (%) Mental Ability Score, Mean (SD)
Parental social classa
    Professional/managerial 1067 (27.0) 107.3 (14.1) 1177 (27.9) 106.0 (13.7)
    Skilled nonmanual 378 (9.6) 104.6 (14.7) 373 (8.8) 101.3 (13.2)
    Skilled manual 1495 (37.9) 97.7 (14.3) 1587 (37.6) 96.9 (13.9)
    Semiskilled/unskilled 577 (14.6) 95.9 (14.8) 628 (14.9) 93.4 (14.1)
    Unknown 431 (10.9) 98.3** (15.9) 457 (10.8) 97.3** (15.3)
Current social class
    Professional/managerial 1526 (38.6) 107.3 (14.2) 1442 (34.2) 106.1 (13.5)
    Skilled nonmanual 486 (12.3) 102.5 (13.9) 1616 (38.3) 98.1 (13.1)
    Skilled manual 1272 (32.2) 96.2 (13.5) 377 (8.9) 94.7 (14.6)
    Semiskilled/unskilled 576 (14.6) 92.8 (14.5) 698 (16.5) 92.4 (14.3)
    Unknown 88 (2.2) 96.7** (17.8) 89 (2.1) 85.6** (18.1)
Academic/vocational qualifications
    No qualifications 368 (9.3) 89.4 (14.1) 327 (7.7) 85.7 (13.7)
    CSE or equivalent NVQ 1 313 (7.9) 92.0 (13.3) 334 (7.9) 90.9 (13.5)
    O-level or equivalent NVQ 2 980 (24.8) 97.3 (13.7) 1340 (31.7) 96.7 (12.6)
    A-level or equivalent NVQ 3 944 (23.9) 100.6 (13.7) 765 (18.1) 99.7 (13.3)
    Degree or diploma or equivalent NVQ 4 1181 (29.9) 107.6 (14.1) 1312 (31.0) 105.8 (13.5)
    Higher degree or NVQ 5 162 (4.1) 114.9** (12.3) 144 (3.4) 114.3** (12.3)
Annual gross earnings, £ per y, men/women
    ≤14 350 / ≤ 7790 786 (25.3) 95.0 (15.6) 711 (24.6) 95.2 (13.6)
    14 351–19 200 / 7791–13 188 758 (24.4) 100.9 (14.5) 736 (25.4) 97.9 (13.6)
    19 201– 26 000 / 13 189–19 200 799 (25.7) 102.8 (14.4) 746 (25.8) 101.6 (13.3)
    >26 000 / > 19 200 761 (24.5) 107.8** (13.8) 699 (24.2) 108.2** (12.9)
Marital status
    Married 1534 (39.2) 100.7 (14.7) 2068 (49.0) 99.6 (14.3)
    Cohabiting 965 (24.6) 101.0 (14.5) 907 (21.5) 99.7 (14.4)
    Single, never married 1278 (32.6) 101.0 (16.4) 1021 (28.3) 98.3 (16.0)
    Separated/divorced 138 (3.5) 97.7 (15.4) 215 (5.1) 99.2 (14.1)
    Widowed 7 (2.8) 105.6 (15.6)
Has children
    Yes 1293 (32.9) 98.1 (14.2) 2284 (54.0) 96.6 (14.3)
    No 2637 (67.1) 102.0** (15.6) 1942 (45.9) 102.5** (14.6)

Note. CSE = Certificate of Secondary Education; NVQ = National Vocational Qualification. N = 8170 for all analyses except those including marital status (N = 8133), parenthood (N = 8156), and income (N = 5996).

a

Derived from mother's occupation if no father present, otherwise derived from father's occupation.

**

P < .001, for heterogeneity between groups.

Statistical Analysis

Comparison of cohort members with and without complete data for potential confounding or mediating variables showed the relations of childhood mental ability score with adult alcohol intake and alcohol problems to be similar in the 2 groups. However, among men only, the association between childhood mental ability score and risk of alcohol problems tended to be somewhat stronger among those with complete social class data, both in childhood and currently, than among those with incomplete data. Therefore, to avoid the possibility of selection bias, we retained cohort members with incomplete data in the analysis by using mother's social class in place of father's social class if no father was present, and by creating an extra category for missing data within parental and current social class.

We used analysis of variance (ANOVA) and the χ2 test to examine the relation between mental ability score and characteristics of the participants. ANOVA was also used to examine mean mental ability score according to frequency of drinking, alcohol units per week, and responses to the CAGE items. Alcohol units per week were categorized for men and women separately using cutpoints that reflected Great Britain guidelines during the period of data collection (> 14 units/wk for women; >21 units/wk for men).24

Ordinal logistic regression is a method of analyzing categorical data with more than 2 categories, where the categories are ordered in a natural way, such as “most of the time, sometimes, rarely, never.” It makes use of information about the ordering, producing a cumulative odds and assuming that the odds for each “cut-off” category are proportional (confirmed herein). We used ordinal logistic regression25 to examine the relation of mental ability score at age 10 years and risk of drinking alcohol more frequently and having a higher weekly alcohol intake. We used binary logistic regression to examine the relation of childhood mental ability score and risk of ever having an alcohol problem according to the CAGE questionnaire (yes vs no). Throughout our analyses, odds ratios are expressed per 1 SD advantage (increase) in mental ability score and are shown for varying levels of adjustment. In these analyses, we conceptualized parental social class as a potential confounding variable, whereas current social class, educational attainment, being a parent by age 30 years, and income were regarded as potential mediating factors in the relation between mental ability and the alcohol outcomes. At the 30-year follow-up survey, 11 205 participants provided data on frequency of alcohol consumption, of whom 8170 (73%) also had data on mental ability score at the age of 10 years and were therefore included in our analyses.

RESULTS

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the 8170 participants (3948 men; 4222 women) according to their mental ability scores at age 10 years. As anticipated, mental ability scores at age 10 years were strongly associated with all markers of socioeconomic position across the life course. Mean IQ scores were higher in those participants whose parents were from more-advantaged social classes, in those who were currently in professional or managerial jobs themselves, in those who had attained higher-level academic or vocational qualifications, and in those with greater annual earnings. There was no statistically significant relation between mental ability score and current marital status among men (P = .11) or women (P = .13), but participants who had children by the age of 30 years had a lower mean mental ability.

Table 2 shows mean mental ability score according to categories of the alcohol outcomes: frequency of drinking, alcohol units consumed per week, and problem drinking as indexed by CAGE. Of the 3895 men and 4148 women with a history of drinking alcohol who were asked the CAGE questions, 20.4% of men and 8.1% of women scored at least 2 points on the 4-item CAGE scale, suggesting that they had experienced alcohol problems. Men and women who gave positive responses to the CAGE questions cut down and guilty tended to have a higher mean childhood mental ability score than those who gave negative responses to those items. Mean childhood mental ability score was higher among those whose aggregated CAGE responses were indicative of alcohol problems. A greater difference was evident among women (4.5 mental ability points) than among men (1.7 mental ability points).

TABLE 2.

Childhood Mental Ability Score and Adult Alcohol Outcomes (Including CAGE Questions), by Gender: 1970 British Cohort Study, 1980–1981 and 1999–2000

Men
Women
No. (%) Mental Ability Score, Mean (SD) No. (%) Mental Ability Score, Mean (SD)
Cut down
    No 2596 (66.6) 99.3 (15.1) 3426 (82.6) 98.5 (14.6)
    Yes 1299 (33.4) 103.9 (15.0)** 722 (17.4) 104.1 (14.1)**
Annoyed
    No 3313 (85.0) 101.0 (15.2) 3878 (93.4) 99.5 (14.7)
    Yes 583 (15.0) 100.2 (15.4) 271 (7.7) 99.6 (13.9)
Guilty
    No 3256 (83.6) 100.5 (15.2) 3828 (92.3) 99.1 (14.6)
    Yes 640 (16.4) 102.5 (15.3)* 321 (7.7) 103.8 (14.5)**
Eye-opener
    No 3548 (91.1) 100.7 (15.2) 4074 (98.2) 99.5 (14.6)
    Yes 348 (8.9) 102.2 (15.4) 75 (1.8) 100.4 (15.8)
Ever had alcohol problem (CAGE score ≥ 2)
    No 3100 (79.6) 100.5 (15.1) 3814 (91.9) 99.1 (14.6)
    Yes 795 (20.4) 102.2 (15.4)* 334 (8.1) 103.6 (14.3)**
Frequency of drinking
    Never had alcohol 52 (1.3) 94.3 (15.9) 73 (1.7) 90.7 (15.0)
    Never nowadays 81 (2.1) 98.1 (13.4) 232 (5.5) 98.6 (14.5)
    Less often 324 (8.2) 94.7 (14.7) 785 (18.6) 94.7 (14.9)
    2 or 3 times per mo 442 (11.2) 98.5 (14.9) 678 (16.1) 97.9 (13.7)
    Once a wk 788 (20.0) 98.2 (14.6) 986 (23.3) 98.3 (14.1)
    2 or 3 days per wk 1570 (39.8) 102.8 (15.0) 1110 (26.3) 103.0 (14.0)
    On most days 691 (17.5) 104.2 (15.3)** 358 (8.5) 105.7 (14.7)**
Alcohol units per wk, men/women
    None 262 (7.5) 96.7 (14.7) 437 (14.0) 98.3 (13.9)
    1–10/1–7 795 (22.8) 100.8 (15.0) 1258 (40.2) 99.8 (14.3)
    11–21/8–14 774 (22.2) 101.7 (15.6) 749 (23.9) 102.4 (14.1)
    22–35/15–21 677 (19.4) 102.9 (15.0) 325 (10.4) 102.9 (14.6)
    36–50/22–35 402 (11.5) 101.2 (14.6) 239 (7.6) 102.7 (12.7)
    > 50/> 35 575 (16.5) 103.0 (15.1)* 122 (3.9) 99.9 (12.7)**

Note. CAGE = cut down, annoying, guilty, eye-opener. See the “Methods” section for details.

*P < .01; **P < .001.

In our analyses, 57.3% of men and 34.8% of women reported that they drank alcohol on at least 2 to 3 days per week. Information on weekly intake of alcohol was available for 3485 men (91% of those questioned about intake) and 3130 women (80% of those questioned). Of these, 47.4% of men and 21.9% of women were drinking in excess of recommended limits (21 units/wk for men, 14 units/wk for women). Among both men and women, frequency of drinking alcohol tended to increase with childhood mental ability scores. Men and women who said they drank on most days had the highest mean childhood mental ability score, whereas those who reported that they never had alcohol had the lowest mean mental ability score. We saw a similar pattern with alcohol intake, although, among women, IQ scores peaked in those reporting drinking 15 to 21 units in the preceding week, with a drop in mean IQ for those consuming more than 35 units. The relation of mental ability with frequency of drinking and alcohol units consumed per week was not monotonic for men and women.

Table 3 shows history of ever having alcohol problems (as indicated by CAGE score), frequency of drinking, and alcohol units consumed per week, according to current social class. Among women (P = .005) but not men (P = .44), we found a statistically significant association between current social class and alcohol problems. The proportion of women with a history of alcohol problems was highest among women from professional and managerial classes. Women and men from professional and managerial classes were more likely than were those from other social groups to report that they drank alcohol on most days. There were also statistically significant associations among both genders between current social class and the number of alcohol units consumed per week; among both men and women, the proportion of those who reported drinking no alcohol the previous week was lowest in the professional and managerial classes. The intake of more than 50 units of alcohol in the past week was somewhat more prevalent among men in unskilled or skilled manual occupations in comparison with those in professional or managerial employment. Similar patterns were evident among women. The relations between parental social class, academic and vocational qualifications, and annual earnings and all 3 alcohol outcomes were very similar, among both men and women, to those described for current social class (data not shown).

TABLE 3.

Current Social Class and Adult Alcohol Outcomes, by Gender: 1970 British Cohort Study, 1980–1981 and 1999–2000

Professional/Managerial, No. (%) Skilled Nonmanual, No. (%) Skilled Manual, No. (%) Semiskilled/Unskilled, No. (%) Unknown, No. (%)
Men
Ever had alcohol problem
    (CAGE score ≥ 2)
    No 1215 (80.5) 379 (79.5) 1005 (79.8) 437 (77.6) 64 (74.4)
    Yes 294 (19.5) 98 (20.6) 255 (20.2) 126 (22.4) 22 (25.6)
Frequency of drinking
    Never had alcohol 17 (1.1) 9 (1.9) 17 (1.3) 13 (2.3) 2 (2.3)
    Never nowadays 28 (1.8) 4 (0.8) 28 (2.2) 14 (2.4) 5 (5.7)
    Less often 86 (5.6) 37 (7.6) 125 (9.8) 63 (10.9) 13 (14.7)
    2–3 times per mo 119 (7.8) 64 (13.2) 169 (13.3) 82 (14.2) 8 (9.0)
    Once a week 270 (17.7) 104 (21.4) 278 (21.9) 118 (20.5) 18 (20.5)
    2–3 days per wk 678 (44.4) 203 (41.8) 458 (36.0) 204 (35.4) 27 (30.7)
    On most days 328 (21.5) 65 (13.4) 201 (15.8) 82 (14.2) 15 (17.0)**
Alcohol units per wk
    None 79 (5.7) 31 (7.1) 108 (9.8) 38 (7.8) 6 (8.8)
    1–10 322 (23.1) 97 (22.3) 235 (21.3) 124 (25.6) 17 (25.0)
    11–21 319 (22.9) 111 (25.5) 236 (21.4) 98 (20.2) 10 (14.7)
    22–35 289 (20.8) 97 (22.3) 198 (17.9) 81 (16.7) 12 (17.7)
    36–50 168 (12.1) 40 (9.2) 129 (11.7) 57 (11.8) 8 (11.8)
    >50 215 (15.5) 59 (13.6) 199 (18.0) 87 (17.9) 15 (22.1)*
Women
Ever had alcohol problem
    (CAGE score ≥ 2)
    No 1278 (89.8) 1480 (93.3) 347 (92.8) 631 (92.8) 78 (91.8)
    Yes 145 (10.2) 106 (6.7) 27 (7.2) 49 (7.2) 7 (8.2)*
Frequency of drinking
    Never had alcohol 19 (1.3) 29 (1.8) 3 (0.8) 18 (2.6) 4 (4.5)**
    Never nowadays 66 (4.6) 86 (5.3) 20 (5.3) 50 (7.2) 10 (11.2)
    Less often 169 (11.7) 294 (18.2) 88 (23.3) 206 (29.5) 28 (31.5)
    2 or 3 times per mo 191 (13.2) 271 (16.8) 76 (20.2) 131 (18.8) 9 (10.1)
    Once a week 296 (20.5) 435 (26.9) 84 (22.3) 156 (22.4) 15 (16.9)
    2 or 3 days per wk 493 (34.1) 401 (24.8) 78 (20.7) 116 (16.6) 22 (24.7)
    On most days 208 (14.4) 100 (6.2) 28 (7.4) 21 (3.0) 1 (1.1)
Alcohol units per wk
    None 137 (11.5) 180 (14.9) 36 (13.6) 76 (17.9) 8 (17.0)
    1–7 452 (38.0) 526 (43.6) 93 (35.2) 172 (40.6) 15 (31.9)
    8–14 314 (26.4) 282 (23.4) 58 (22.0) 85 (20.1) 10 (21.3)
    15–21 141 (11.9) 103 (8.5) 34 (12.9) 40 (9.4) 7 (14.9)
    22–35 103 (8.7) 73 (6.1) 25 (9.5) 34 (8.0) 4 (8.5)
    >35 41 (3.5) 43 (3.6) 18 (6.8) 17 (4.0) 3 (6.4)*

Note. CAGE is an acronym based on the 4 questions that comprise the inventory. See the “Methods” section for details.

*P < .01; **P < .001.

Table 4 shows the relations between childhood mental ability score and adult drinking patterns and risk of alcohol problems; results for adult drinking patterns are presented as cumulative odds ratios across the alcohol categories. The relations between childhood mental ability score and risk of ever having alcohol problems (P for interaction term = .004) or drinking alcohol more frequently (P for interaction term = .043) differed between the genders. In univariate analyses, among both men and women, higher childhood mental ability score was associated with significantly increased risks of ever having alcohol problems (as indicated by a CAGE score of ≥ 2), drinking alcohol more frequently, and having a higher weekly alcohol intake. In general, individual control for markers of socioeconomic circumstances had little effect on the relation between childhood ability score and the 3 alcohol outcomes. Similarly, multiple controls for markers of socioeconomic position had little influence on the IQ-alcohol endpoint, with the exception of drinking alcohol frequently, where some attenuation was seen. We examined whether further adjustment for parental education might influence the risk estimates, but it also had little effect (data not shown). We also examined whether the relation between childhood mental ability and alcohol problems varied according to strata of social class (childhood or current), educational qualifications, or earnings, but we found no statistically significant interactions. Restricting the analysis of alcohol problems and frequency of drinking to current drinkers had little effect on the multivariate-adjusted estimates of risk.

TABLE 4.

Odds Ratios for the Relation of 15-Point (1 SD) Increase in Childhood Mental Ability Score With Alcohol Outcomes, by Gender: 1970 British Cohort Study, 1980–1981 and 1999–2000

Ever Had Alcohol Problems, OR (95% CI) Drinks Alcohol More Frequently, OR (95% CI) Higher Weekly Alcohol Intake, OR (95% CI)
Men
Total, No. 3070 3104 2807
Adjustments
    Unadjusted 1.13 (1.04, 1.23) 1.36 (1.29, 1.45) 1.11 (1.04, 1.19)
    Parental social class 1.13 (1.03, 1.24) 1.34 (1.22, 1.40) 1.11 (1.04, 1.19)
    Current social class 1.18 (1.07, 1.30) 1.27 (1.18, 1.36) 1.12 (1.05, 1.20)
    Qualifications 1.13 (1.04, 1.24) 1.25 (1.16, 1.34) 1.12 (1.04, 1.20)
    Has children 1.11 (1.02, 1.22) 1.33 (1.25, 1.42) 1.09 (1.02, 1.17)
    Annual gross earnings 1.15 (1.03, 1.26) 1.35 (1.27, 1.44) 1.11 (1.05, 1.18)
    All characteristics 1.16 (1.04, 1.28) 1.18 (1.10, 1.27) 1.12 (1.04, 1.20)
    All, current drinkers only 1.16 (1.05, 1.29) 1.17 (1.08, 1.26)
Women
Total, No. 2859 2896 2286
Adjustments
    Unadjusted 1.44 (1.24, 1.67) 1.54 (1.43, 1.66) 1.26 (1.16, 1.36)
    Parental social class 1.42 (1.22, 1.65) 1.43 (1.32, 1.54) 1.24 (1.14, 1.35)
    Current social class 1.35 (1.16, 1.59) 1.37 (1.27, 1.48) 1.23 (1.13, 1.34)
    Qualifications 1.46 (1.24, 1.71) 1.38 (1.27, 1.49) 1.24 (1.14, 1.36)
    Has children 1.36 (1.18, 1.58) 1.45 (1.35, 1.56) 1.21 (1.11, 1.31)
    Annual net earnings 1.44 (1.24, 1.67) 1.48 (1.38, 1.58) 1.25 (1.16, 1.36)
    All characteristics 1.38 (1.17, 1.64) 1.26 (1.16, 1.36) 1.21 (1.11, 1.33)
    All, current drinkers only 1.38 (1.16, 1.64) 1.25 (1.15, 1.35)

Note. OR = odds ratio; CI = confidence interval.

DISCUSSION

On the basis of a previous study from a different British cohort showing an apparent protective effect of high childhood mental ability scores against alcohol-induced hangovers in middle age,26 we hypothesized a relation between high ability-test score in childhood and a reduced risk of alcohol drinking problems in adulthood. The opposite relation was found: mental ability was positively associated with a history of alcohol problems, although this relation was nonlinear. In these analyses and those featuring alcohol intake as the outcomes of interest, associations with IQ were markedly stronger among women than among men.

A post hoc explanation for this effect could be that persons with greater cognitive abilities are more concerned with, and reflect more deeply upon, the potential health effect of alcohol drinking and so are more likely to respond positively to the CAGE items. The present data gave us an opportunity to investigate this. Two of the CAGE items could be regarded as capturing some aspects of self-monitoring of one's behaviors (cut down and guilty). Among women, the fully adjusted odds ratios (95% confidence intervals [CIs]) for responding positively to cut down, guilty, annoyed, and eye-opener were 1.31 (95% CI = 1.16, 1.47), 1.37 (95% CI = 1.16, 1.63), 1.09 (95% CI = 0.90, 1.31), and 1.27 (95% CI = 0.87, 1.87), respectively. Among men, the corresponding odds ratios were 1.29 (95% CI = 1.18, 1.41), 1.10 (95% CI = 0.99, 1.24), 1.02 (95% CI = 0.91, 1.15), and 1.20 (95% CI = 1.03, 1.39), respectively. There was therefore some mixed support for this suggestion of self-monitoring. Another explanation might be that success in the workplace requires, in some circumstances, a willingness to drink frequently and to excess in social situations. This obligation may particularly affect high-IQ scoring women attempting to succeed in traditionally male-dominated professions.

Previous Studies

A small number of studies have examined the relation between childhood mental ability and heavy intake of alcohol in adult life, revealing disparate findings, with inverse,8 U-shaped,9 and null10,11 associations reported. During revision of this article, a report appeared using data from another British cohort study that also examined the association between childhood IQ and problem drinking at age 53 years.27 In this study, IQ was also positively associated with alcohol problems, although, unlike our findings based on a larger sample, this effect did not appear to be differential according to gender. In the absence of any further studies, and mindful of the moderate-to-high correlation between mental ability and education28—and indeed other markers of socioeconomic circumstances—we therefore draw on the rather modest literature linking socioeconomic position with problem drinking. When a history of alcohol problems (as indexed by CAGE) was the outcome of interest, the association with educational attainment was inconsistent: persons with more basic education have been found to be both more2931 and less likely32 to report such problems.

Although we found a positive relation between mental ability and drinking problems among both genders that was stronger in women than in men, in a cohort of employed Finnish individuals, those women with higher educational attainment were more likely to report alcohol drinking problems, whereas, among men, the reverse was true.33 It is plausible that the relation between education (or IQ) and alcohol drinking problems varies because of methodological differences across studies (in particular, measurement of IQ and alcohol intake) but also culture and epoch. In these studies, the CAGE questionnaire was typically administered to survey populations that covered a wide age range and therefore varying contextual experiences, and the question of whether the effect of education on the prevalence of problem drinking differed according to age at measurement was not examined.29,34 In one of the few studies to closely resemble our own, Pulkkinen and Pitkanen30 found opposing results in a small-scale study (N = 369) of Finnish children followed into adult life, in which poor “educational success” at age 8 years was predictive of problem drinking at age 26 years among both men and women.

By contrast, studies that utilized alcohol problems as the endpoint of interest, relations between socioeconomic position and total alcohol intake, although varying by gender, offer more-concordant findings. Among employed persons, there is a stronger positive relation between grade of employment and drinking among women than among men3537; intake was higher among the professional groups. Similarly, in the Scottish Health Survey, investigators found that the relation between socioeconomic position and the same recommended drinking levels as used in the present analyses (21 units/wk for men, 14 units/wk for women) were again more-strongly positive among women than among men.26 These results follow our own when ability, as opposed to socioeconomic position, was the “exposure” of interest.

Strengths and Limitations

Our study has some advantages over previously published work. First, the cohort was well characterized, particularly for life-course socioeconomic position, which allowed us to examine independent effects of ability on alcohol intake and drinking problems. Second, the fact that the data were drawn from participants who reside throughout Great Britain gives our findings a high degree of generalizability.

There are of course some study limitations. First, the longitudinal nature of the study has inevitably led to some attrition, raising concerns about selection bias. Thus, only 46% of the participants at the 30-year follow-up had taken part in all earlier surveys of the cohort, although 80% of the participants in the 30-year follow-up had missed none or only 1 of these earlier sweeps, and 74% of them had taken the British Ability Scales test as part of the 10-year follow-up.38 The participants at the 30-year follow-up did gain significantly higher scores at age 10 for both verbal and nonverbal ability than those who did not take part, but the size of the differences was modest (0.3 of a standard deviation). Expressed differently, 27% of cohort members who were excluded from the analytic sample were in the bottom fifth of the IQ distribution at age 10, versus 17.3% who were included. Comparable figures for parents from semi- or unskilled occupational social class were 17.9% and 14.8%, respectively. It is not possible to assess if the follow-up at age 30 years disproportionately omitted individuals with the most-severe alcohol abuse. However, unless the relations of childhood mental ability with alcohol problems and alcohol intake were in the opposite direction in nonresponders to that found in our analyses—a highly unlikely scenario—little bias will have been introduced. Second, the questions pertaining to alcohol intake concerned only the preceding 7-day period, which may not have necessarily provided an indication of typical intake in all cases.

In our large-scale cohort study, mental ability at age 10 years was positively related to alcohol problems and alcohol intake 20 years later. This association essentially held after we controlled for markers of life-course socioeconomic position. Given that these findings ran counter to our expectations, and given the paucity of studies in this area, further examination of the relation between childhood IQ and adult drinking patterns, and whether these differ between men and women in different cultural and historical contexts, is needed.

Human Participant Protection

No protocol approval was required.

Acknowledgments

C. Emslie and K. Hunt are funded by the UK Medical Research Council. G. D. Batty is a Wellcome Trust Fellow. I. Deary is the recipient of a Royal Society–Wolfson Research Merit Award. The 10-year follow-up was carried out by the Department of Child Health, Bristol University. The 30-year follow-up was carried out under the auspices of the Joint Centre for Longitudinal Research (comprising the Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Institute of Education, University of London, the International Centre for Health and Society, University College Medical School, London, and the National Centre for Social Research). We thank the UK Data Archive, University of Essex, for providing the data.

Note. The original data creators, depositors or copyright holders, the funding agencies, and the UK Data Archive bear no responsibility for the analyses and interpretation presented here.

References

  • 1.Gottfredson LS. Mainstream science on intelligence: an editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography. Intelligence 1997;24:13–23 [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Taylor MD, Hart CL, Davey Smith G, et al. Childhood mental ability and smoking cessation in adulthood: prospective observational study linking the Scottish Mental Survey 1932 and the Midspan studies. J Epidemiol Community Health 2003;57:464–465 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Batty GD, Deary IJ, MacIntyre S. Childhood IQ and life course socioeconomic position in relation to alcohol-induced hangovers in adulthood: the Aberdeen Children of the 1950s Study. J Epidemiol Community Health 2006;60:872–874 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Batty GD, Deary IJ, MacIntyre S. Childhood IQ in relation to risk factors for premature mortality in middle-aged persons: the Aberdeen Children of the 1950s study. J Epidemiol Community Health 2007;61:241–247 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Batty GD, Deary IJ, Schoon I, Gale CR. Mental ability across childhood in relation to risk factors for premature mortality in adult life: the 1970 British Cohort Study. J Epidemiol Community Health 2007;61:997–1003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Batty GD, Deary IJ, Schoon I, Gale CR. Childhood mental ability in relation to food intake and physical activity in adulthood: the 1970 British Cohort Study. Pediatrics 2007;119:e38–e45 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Gale CR, Deary IJ, Schoon I, Batty GD. IQ in childhood and vegetarianism in adulthood: 1970 British Cohort Study. BMJ 2007;334:245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Fleming JP, Kellam SG, Brown CH. Early predictors of age at first use of alcohol, marijuana, and cigarettes. Drug Alcohol Depend 1982;9:285–303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Mortensen LH, Sorensen TI, Gronbaek M. Intelligence in relation to later beverage preference and alcohol intake. Addiction 2005;100:1445–1452 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wennberg P, Andersson T, Bohman M. Psychosocial characteristics at age 10; differentiating between adult alcohol use pathways. A prospective longitudinal study. Addict Behav 2002;27:115–130 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kubicka L, Matejcek Z, Dytrych Z, Roth Z. IQ and personality traits assessed in childhood as predictors of drinking and smoking behaviour in middle-aged adults: a 24-year follow-up study. Addiction 2001;96:1615–1628 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hemmingsson T, Melin B, Allebeck P, Lundberg I. The association between cognitive ability measured at ages 18–20 and mortality during 30 years of follow-up—a prospective observational study among Swedish males born 1949–51. Int J Epidemiol 2006;35:665–670 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Poulin C, Webster I, Single E. Alcohol disorders in Canada as indicated by the CAGE questionnaire. CMAJ 1997;157:1529–1535 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Butler N, Bonham D. Perinatal mortality. Edinburgh, Scotland: Livingstone; 1963 [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Butler N, Bynner J. 1970 British Cohort Study: Ten-Year Follow-Up. Colchester, England: UK Data Archive; 1997 [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Joint Centre for Longitudinal Research National Child Development Study and 1970 British Cohort Study (BSC70) Follow-Ups, 1999–2000. Colchester, Essex: UK Data Archive; 2003 [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Elliott C, Murray D, Pearson L. British Ability Scales. Windsor: National Foundation for Educational Research; 1978 [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Butler N, Haslum M, Barker W, Morris A. Child Health and Education Study. First Report to the Dept of Education and Science on the 10-Year Follow-Up. Bristol, England: University of Bristol; 1982 [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Carroll J. Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytical Studies. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2006 [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Office of Population, Censuses, and Surveys Classification of Occupations, 1980. London, England: Her Majesty's Stationery Office; 1980 [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ewing JA. Detecting alcoholism. The CAGE questionnaire. JAMA 1984;252:1905–1907 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.King M. At-risk drinking among general practice attenders: validation of the CAGE questionnaire. Psychol Med 1986;16:213–217 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Mayfield D, McLeod G, Hall P. The CAGE questionnaire: validation of a new alcoholism screening instrument. Am J Psychiatry 1974;131:1121–1123 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Department of Health The Health of the Nation. London, England: HMSO; 1992 [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Scott SC, Goldberg MS, Mayo NE. Statistical assessment of ordinal outcomes in comparative studies. J Clin Epidemiol 1997;50:45–55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Erens B, Moody A. Alcohol consumption. In Bromley C, Sproston K, Shelton N, The Scottish Health Survey. Volume 2: Adults. Edinburgh: The Scottish Executive; 2005 [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hatch SL, Jones PB, Kuh D, Hardy R, Wadsworth ME, Richards M. Childhood cognitive ability and adult mental health in the British 1946 birth cohort. Soc Sci Med 2007;64:2285–2296 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Deary IJ, Strand S, Smith P, Fernandes C. Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence2007;35:13–21 [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Parry CD, Pluddemann A, Steyn K, Bradshaw D, Norman R, Laubscher R. Alcohol use in South Africa: findings from the first Demographic and Health Survey (1998). J Stud Alcohol 2005;66:91–97 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Pulkkinen L, Pitkanen T. A prospective study of the precursors to problem drinking in young adulthood. J Stud Alcohol 1994;55:578–587 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Poulin C, Webster I, Single E. Alcohol disorders in Canada as indicated by the CAGE questionnaire. CMAJ 1997;157:1529–1535 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Schoon I, Parsons S. Life styles and health behaviour in three birth cohorts. In: Ferri E, Bynner J, Wadsworth M, eds. Changing Britain, Changing Lives. Three Generations at the Turn of the Century, London, England: Institute of Education Press; 2003 [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Roos E, Lahelma E, Rahkonen O. Work-family conflicts and drinking behaviours among employed women and men. Drug Alcohol Depend 2006;83:49–56 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Poulin C, Webster I, Single E. Alcohol disorders in Canada as indicated by the CAGE questionnaire. CMAJ 1997;157:1529–1535 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Tomiak M, Gentleman JF, Jette M. Health and gender differences between middle and senior managers in the Canadian Public Service. Soc Sci Med 1997;45:1589–1596 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Emslie C, Hunt K, MacIntyre S. How similar are the smoking and drinking habits of men and women in non-manual jobs? Eur J Public Health 2002;12:22–28 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Marmot MG, North F, Feeney A, Head J. Alcohol consumption and sickness absence: from the Whitehall II study. Addiction 1993;88:369–382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Plewis I, Calderwood L, Hawkes D, Nathan G. National Child Development Study and 1970 British Cohort Study Technical Report: Changes in the NCDS and BCS70 Populations and Samples Over Time. London, England: Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Institute of Education; 2004 [Google Scholar]

Articles from American Journal of Public Health are provided here courtesy of American Public Health Association

RESOURCES