Abstract
Bridges that span structural holes are often explained in terms of the entrepreneurial personalities or rational motivations of brokers, or structural processes that lead to the intersection of social foci. I argue that the existence and use of bridges in interpersonal networks also depends on individuals’ health. Poor health may make it more difficult to withstand the pressures and to execute some of the common tasks associated with bridging (e.g., brokerage). I examine this possibility using egocentric network data on over 2,500 older adults drawn from the recent National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (NSHAP). Multivariate regression analyses show that both cognitive and functional health are significantly positively associated with bridging, net of sociodemographic and life-course controls. The relationship between functional (kinesthetic) health and bridging appears to be partially mediated by network composition, as older adults who have poorer functional health also tend to have networks that are richer in strong ties. Several potential mediation mechanisms are discussed. Cognitive function remains significantly associated with bridging net of network composition, suggesting that the inherent challenges of maintaining bridging positions may be more difficult to cope with for those who have cognitive impairments than for those who have functional impairments such as limited mobility. An alternative explanation is that cognitively impaired individuals have more difficulty recognizing (and thus strategically using) bridges in their networks. Theoretical implications and possibilities for future research are discussed.
INTRODUCTION
Social network bridging is central to macrosocial processes, including the diffusion of information and innovations (Burt 2004; Granovetter 1973), the epidemic spread of diseases like HIV (Aral 2000; Gorbach et al. 2000; Morris et al. 1996), community cohesion (Beyerlein and Hipp 2005; Putnam 2000), and the small world phenomenon in general (Watts 1999). It is also a key mechanism through which social network structure affects individuals. Bridging can be contrasted with triadic closure and all of the social capital benefits (Coleman 1988) that are associated with it (Burt 2005). But bridging has its own advantages. Simmel (1950b) was the first to discuss the implications of serving as an intermediary between two parties, including the ability to mediate conflicts and to play two parties against each another for personal gain (i.e., tertius gaudens). Furthermore, bridging yields brokerage potential, as those who occupy bridging positions can transfer resources between parties and extract profits from such transfers (Burt 1976, 1992; Gould 1989; Gould and Fernandez 1989; Marsden 1982). Finally, bridging yields access to distinct pools of information and other resources, which can facilitate social mobility (Granovetter 1973).
But what makes bridging possible? Two approaches to understanding the origins of bridges underlie social network research. First, we can work backwards from the fact that bridges cannot exist in the absence of clusters and the structural holes that exist between them. Therefore, perspectives which emphasize the predominance of social cohesion in networks and the self-organizing effects of activity occurring within foci and homogenous social circles (Blau 1977; Feld 1981; Friedkin 1984; Lazarsfeld and Merton 1954; Laumann 1966; McPherson, Smith-Lovin, and Cook 2001) focus on the origins of the structural conditions in which bridging occurs. This work notes that clusters, cliques, and other cohesive social groupings emerge out of actors’ exposure to and familiarity with each other, as well as their adherence to group norms and pressures toward conformity (e.g., see Friedkin 1984). Transitivity theory, for instance, is based on the idea that when A nominates B and B nominates C, A also tends to nominate C (Holland and Leinhardt 1970). There are some social-psychological mechanisms behind the principle of transitivity, but it is usually treated as a structural force (Feld 1981; Wellman 1983).
This work undergirds Granovetter’s (1973) forbidden triad argument, which views weak social ties as a basis of bridging. Infrequent face-to-face contact, restricted access, and emotional distance between actors reduce cohesion and conformity, making it more possible for those who are dissimilar or who move in otherwise separate social circles to form a (weak) relationship. This argument is consistent with Feld’s (1981) focus theory, which holds that local bridges exist where pressure toward transitivity is low and where there are less constrictive common “foci” linking two actors. Bridging thus results from individuals’ involvement in social contexts that are in some way incompatible or otherwise do not overlap. These observations also can be used to explain the fact that bridges tend to be more unstable and decay more rapidly than embedded (“Simmelian”) ties that contribute to triadic closure (Burt 2002). One reason why Simmelian ties are more “sticky” (last longer) than bridges is that they are more conducive to the development of agreement among actors (Krackhardt 1998), making them more congenial ties than bridges and less prone to structural imbalance.
These structural approaches to bridging can be contrasted with agency-based perspectives which hold that the roots of many relational phenomena, including bridging behavior, are found in individuals’ goals, intentions, or personalities (Emirbayer and Mische 1998). Although not always writing explicitly in terms of individual agency or rational choice, some scholars emphasize aspects of bridging that serve the interests of self-serving individuals. Simmel (1950b) discussed at length some of the individual-level benefits that derive from the opportunity to act as an intermediary between two parties (e.g., tertius gaudens) – an opportunity that is most available when one serves as an intermediary between actors who do not interact with each other. Some researchers have used Simmel’s observations to highlight the implications of network structure for brokerage potential in networks (Burt 1976; Gould 1989; Marsden 1982). Gould and Fernandez (1989) detail five forms of brokerage, including gatekeeping and coordinating. Similarly, Burt (1992) discusses the individual-level benefits associated with occupying the space that exists between two otherwise disconnected clusters (structural holes), including control the flow of resources between the two groups.
Other work points to psychological predispositions and personalities (e.g., the “entrepreneurial personality”) in an effort to explain individuals’ different positions within social networks (see Hallinan and Kubitschek 1988; Klein et al. 2004; Stokes 1985). Burt, Jannotta, and Mahoney (1998) present evidence that people whose networks contain structural holes tend to have the personality of “entrepreneurial outsiders,” as opposed to the personality of “conforming or obedient insiders.” Similarly, Kalish and Robins (2006) argue that individualists and people who have a high sense of control are the most likely to maintain ties with unconnected network members. Non-individualistic, extroverted people who have tendencies toward group identification are the most likely to have closed triads. Kadushin (2002) also explores the roots of individual motivations to participate in certain kinds of relationships, noting that participation in structures that are rich with structural holes is often motivated by psychological drives toward self-efficacy and mastery.
The point of this paper is to provide an alternative framework for understanding individuals’ occupancy of bridging positions. In addition to individual rational interests, personalities, and the structural forces that act upon them, their physical and cognitive capacities may affect, directly and indirectly, their opportunities and/or abilities to occupy bridging positions. A vast body of research examines the argument that social network structure affects individuals’ physical and mental health (e.g., see Berkman and Kawachi 2000; House, Umberson, and Landis 1988), but few scholars have explored the reverse possibility. In the next section, I develop two broad approaches to exploring the role health plays in shaping individuals’ opportunities to form and sustain bridges in social networks.
POSSIBLE IMPLICATIONS OF HEALTH FOR BRIDGING
Health may affect individuals’ bridging opportunities and behavior directly or indirectly. First, it is important to note that different kinds of positions within social networks make different demands on their occupants. Bridges can be especially demanding with respect to occupants’ physical and cognitive capacities, therefore posing a particular challenge to those with physical or cognitive impairments. Second, health declines may indirectly deprive individuals of weak ties, which could restrict bridging. This section fleshes out these perspectives in greater detail regarding several specific mechanisms.
Health Problems and the Challenges of Bridging
One basis for the claim that health might affect one’s bridging opportunities is that physical and cognitive impairments make it more difficult to perform some of the tasks associated with bridging. The basic notion that severe impairments can affect individuals’ abilities to engage in social action is not new. Research has shown that health problems reduce involvement in voluntary associations, for example (Li and Ferraro 2006; Thoits and Hewitt 2001). Similarly, some medical sociology and disability research treats health as a set of enabling (or disabling) physical and cognitive conditions that directly facilitate (or hamper) social action (see Frank 1991; Lester and Tritter 2005; Robillard 1999). Thus, health can be viewed as a resource, and is sometimes discussed explicitly as a form of human capital (Becker 2007).
Bridging positions may be more difficult to maintain for people who have certain health problems. One reason is that bridging positions are especially taxing, high-pressure positions. Scholars have noted that more effort is required to fulfill the social responsibilities associated with bridging positions than other types of positions (e.g., Burt 2002; Feld 1981). Because bridges span the structural holes that exist between unconnected social clusters, occupying a bridging position sometimes involves transmitting high volumes of information and other resources. Some aspects of bridging also suggest cognitive challenges. For example, if the clusters being linked are very different, especially if they are governed by different social norms, interacting concurrently with both requires the ability to “switch” back and forth between different cognitive frameworks (Mische and White 1998; White 2008).
Furthermore, social friction is inherent in the architecture of structural holes. This was implied in research on structural balance and ranked clusterability (Davis and Leinhardt 1972), which argued that separate clusters tend to be connected by negatively valenced ties. Similarly, scholars concur that so-called “brokers” (a term that is sometimes used to refer to people who occupy bridging positions) often face conflicting demands which can germinate into role conflict (Aldrich and Herker 1977; Friedman and Podolny 1992; Podolny and Baron 1997). Coleman (1990) noted that “boundary spanners” in organizational contexts often experience role conflicts arising from the separate, sometimes inconsistent sets of expectations originating in separate actors (see also Kahn et al. 1964; Spekman 1979). To make matters worse, there are no third-party ties in common between those on either side of a bridge. No one else is invested in both parties simultaneously, so there is no one who can monitor or “look out for” the integrity of the bridge itself, apart from the bridging position occupant(s).
Embeddedness in a dense network, comprised mainly of closed triads, seems to require less effort to maintain. As Burt (1992) puts it: “Leisure and domestic clusters are a congenial environment of low-maintenance … contacts” (24). Of course, being embedded in a dense network is not always easy. Dense networks offer their own challenges, including increased monitoring, more stringent rules, and greater pressure toward conformity. Tightly-knit networks also tend to host more rigid, segregated role expectations (e.g., see Bott 1957; Hill 1988). But bridging positions require intact physical and cognitive capacities to an extent that other types of network positions do not, in part because these capacities are more central to the execution of bridging responsibilities. These observations combine to support a general assertion: Structural holes constitute perhaps the most inhospitable, challenging of social climates, and individuals who have better physical and cognitive health may be better able to weather such climates.
Lastly, poor health may intervene between individual agency (e.g., intention) and action. As noted above, some network scholars (especially Burt 1992) have argued that individuals occupy bridging positions because of the “strategic” advantage in accessing and controlling resources that comes with those positions. Yet, a long line of research in cognitive psychology suggests that before one can strategically navigate a social structure, one must first “learn” that structure (De Soto 1960). This work demonstrates that most people assume widespread symmetry, transitivity, and balance in social structures, as cognitive “schemas” based on these properties organize individuals’ perceptions and memories of social structure (De Soto and Kuethe 1959; Freeman, Freeman, and Michaelson 1988; Press, Crockett, and Rosenkrantz 1969; Zajonc and Burnstein 1965). These are tendencies which can impede perception, or recall, of structural holes. Thus, one’s perception and recollection of structural holes may depend on various factors, including the extent of one’s experience with such structures (Janicik and Larrick 2005), contact frequency (Freeman, Romney, and Freeman 1987), one’s position in the structure being assessed (Krackhardt 1987), and one’s degree of concern over one’s own social status (Flynn et al. 2006). It is also reasonable to suggest that because structural holes are more difficult to perceive and process cognitively than other kinds of structures (as people cannot rely on oversimplified cognitive schemas to identify them), intact cognitive function is also key to the detection, and thus indirectly to the strategic use, of bridging positions. Cognitively impaired individuals who have the same number of bridging opportunities as others may simply be less able to recognize these opportunities because this requires the ability to apply more complex cognitive schemas.
For all of these reasons, the general hypothesis that will be tested in this paper is: People who have poorer physical and/or cognitive health have fewer structural holes and fewer bridging opportunities in their personal networks. However, I should stress that it is beyond the scope of this paper to determine the exact mechanisms that account for this pattern. It may have something to do with the physical and/or cognitive challenges of bridging, or challenges associated with identifying bridging opportunities. And, as I discuss in the following section, there may be structural mechanisms operating within networks as well, which support a more indirect link between health and bridging.
Health Problems and Network Composition
There are several mechanisms by which health problems could lead to triadic closure and increased network density, and thereby reduce bridging opportunities. First, many scholars describe a surge of social support from network members that follows the onset of severe health problems. This idea underlies the social support convoy model and research on social support networks (see Berkman and Kawachi 2000; Kahn and Antonucci 1981; Thoits 1995; c.f., Lipton 1981). Network members who respond to health decline with offers of support often increase contact with each other as well. This facilitates network members’ mutual awareness of each other’s resources, availability of time, and coordination of caregiving duties (Beggs, Haines, and Hurlbert 1996; Kazak and Marvin 1984). To the extent that this activity joins together previously unconnected network members, the density of the support recipient’s network may increase, thus closing structural holes.
A somewhat more likely scenario is that health problems and disabilities eventually strain the types of social relationships that otherwise would give rise to bridging within one’s network. This process takes some explaining. With the exception of spousal ties, kin relations, and exceptionally close friendships, most social ties depend on the norm of direct reciprocity (Gouldner 1960). Unfortunately, people who suffer from severe health problems and disabilities may have more trouble reciprocating in basic social exchanges. This introduces an element of inequity which can jeopardize social relationships (Kuijer, Buunk, and Ybema 2001; Sprecher 2001; Thompson and Pitts 1992; Utne et al. 1984). The decay and dissolution of inequity-strained ties does necessarily reflect selfishness on the part of those who under-benefit in these relationships. Rather, over-benefitted parties are often unwilling to endure severely inequitable relationships as well, as the inability to reciprocate can lead to guilt, depression, self-consciousness, embarrassment, and decreased sense of control (Dowd 1975; Jang et al. 2002; Schieman and Turner 1998; Ville et al. 2001).
Care for severe impairments is often an uncomfortable experience for both the caregiver and the care recipient, especially when it involves uncomfortable physical contact (e.g., lifting the recipient from a wheelchair) or helping with unpleasant bodily functions or hygiene tasks. These experiences violate the normal boundaries of many social relationships, and even close friends and family members may reduce contact in these situations to avoid embarrassment. For these reasons, “[b]oth friends and peripheral kin may drop out of networks when the…[recipient] needs help with personal care or body contact tasks, which violate norms of privacy and are incongruent with the usual expectations of the relationship” (Stoller and Pugliesi 1991:182).
These strains usually pose less of a threat to spousal or close kin relations, in part because these types of ties are not usually predicated on norms of direct reciprocity. Kin ties in particular depend more on generalized forms of reciprocity which are indirect, which oscillate between “give” and “receive” less predictably, and/or which involve intergenerational exchanges which can flow in only one direction for years at a time (Ingersoll-Dayton and Antonucci 1988; Wentkowski 1981). For instance, parents invest many resources in their children with the expectation that the reward will come later in their lives (Antonucci and Akiyama 1987; Neufeld and Harrison 1995). Because of this, close kin relations are more likely to endure in the face of increased caregiver burden and the strains it puts on caregivers (Adams and Bleizsner 1995; see also Horan et al. 2006). Bound by normative obligations to provide care or by affection for the recipient, family members may be unable or reluctant to withdraw care or stop contact (Stoller and Pugliesi 1991).
One possible result of the social strains caused by some physical and cognitive health problems, then, is the disproportionate loss of the kinds of weak ties which give rise to bridging, combined with the strengthening of already-strong ties. Thus, the onset of a serious health problem or disability may lead to the paring down of one’s social network and greater interconnectedness among the alters within it. This, in turn, means fewer structural holes and less bridging. Therefore, the general hypothesis proposed above can be modified as follows to suggest an indirect association: People who have poorer health have fewer structural holes and fewer bridging opportunities in their personal networks, partly because their personal networks are more comprised of strong ties and close relations who are likely to know each other.
DATA AND ANALYSIS
I test these hypotheses using data from the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (NSHAP), a nationally representative study of 3,005 non-institutionalized older adults between 57–85 years of age. NSHAP is a population-based study conducted by the National Opinion Research Center (NORC) in 2005–2006. The sample for the study was selected from a multi-stage area probability design screened by the Institute for Social Research (ISR) for the Health and Retirement Study (HRS). The original HRS design oversampled by race/ethnicity. NSHAP retained this design and also oversampled by age and gender to produce approximately equal cell sizes by gender across three age categories. The final response rate is 75.5%.1
Why Older Adults?
This NSHAP sample is extraordinarily useful from a social gerontological standpoint. For one, health problems such as cognitive impairment and limited mobility are more common among older adults. If health is associated with bridging, older adults’ independence, brokerage potential, and access to unique resources may be threatened more than younger adults’. Second, older adults value their independence and control over personal affairs, especially in the face of modernization processes, which some social gerontologists claim disempower older adults and increase their dependency on younger generations (Burgess, 1960; Townsend, 1981; Turner, 1989). Social network researchers have shown that being embedded in relationships with people who are strongly connected to each other (i.e., “Simmelian ties”) reduces not only one’s bargaining power and access to unique pools of resources (Burt 1992; Granoveter 1973), as well as individuality and independence (Krackhardt 1999; Simmel 1950a,b). For older adults, having overbearing support givers can engender emotional distress, feelings of vulnerability, low self-esteem, and a sense of being coerced (Cohler, 1983; Coyne, Wortman, and Lehman, 1988; Krause 1987; Lee, Netzer, & Coward, 1995; Martire et al. 2002; Silverstein, Chen, and Heller 1996; Vinokur & Vinokur, 1990). In fact, dependency on others, coupled with isolation from a broader network, is one of the key risk factors of elder mistreatment (Harris 1990; Lachs et al. 1994; Quinn and Tomita 1986). Maintaining bridging positions may therefore be especially critical to older adults’ abilities to avoid some of these common experiences in later life.
Social Network Data
NSHAP collected extensive information about older adults’ social connectedness (see Cornwell, Laumann, and Schumm 2008). I will focus on measures drawn from data concerning older adults’ egocentric networks, collected during the NSHAP in-person interviews. An egocentric network consists of N actors, including ego (pi) and a set of alters (pk), where k is network size. It can be delineated in a square matrix, A, containing N × N elements. Elements in A that represent relationships between ego and alters are denoted by a(pi, pk). To get this information, interviewers asked respondents the following:
From time to time, most people discuss things that are important to them with others. For example, these may include good or bad things that happen to you, problems you are having, or important concerns you may have. Looking back over the last 12 months, who are the people with whom you most often discussed things that were important to you?
Respondents could name up to five people (so, k ≤5), but they also indicated if they had more than five discussion partners. This name generator elicits names of strong, frequently accessed, long-term contacts (Marin 2004; Ruan 1998) – ties through which normative pressures and social influence are likely to operate (Burt 1984).2 Therefore, respondents’ egocentric networks are not likely to contain many weak ties, which will reduce the prevalence of structural holes and bridging.
Elements in A can be valued, since relationships in the egocentric network are reported in terms of the number of days of contact per year (0 to 365) between network members.3 Because ego is connected to all k alters, a(pi, pk) > 0. Elements in A that capture relationships between pairs of alters are represented by a(pj, pk), where 0 ≤ a(pj, pk) ≤ 365. This information is collected by asking respondents how often each of their network members contact each of their other network members. A is a symmetric network, so that a(pi, pk) = a(pk, pi) and a(pj, pk) = a(pk, pj).
It is important to note a potential complication with using egocentric network data in a study involving cognitive function. Part of my theory is that people who suffer from cognitive impairments are less able to maintain structural holes and to execute bridging responsibilities. But cognitive impairments may also impede people’s abilities to recognize structural holes – that is, if their ability to employ complex cognitive schemas is compromised. Therefore, we must keep in mind that egocentric network data which involve perceptions of alter-alter relationships, like NSHAP’s, could be affected by cognitively impaired respondents’ simplified cognitive schemas.
Bridging Measures
I am interested in how physical and cognitive health relate to individuals’ bridging within their core networks. There are several ways to operationalize bridging, depending on how it is conceptualized. My strategy is to examine two measures of (perceived) bridging and analyze how health is related to each of them as a check on sensitivity.
A person bridges what might be a structural hole whenever a pair of their network alters are not directly connected to each other – that is, a(pj, pk) < 1. In a symmetric egocentric network like those measured by NSHAP, there are possible pairs of alters. A basic measure of perceived bridging is simply the number of pairs of alters in the network that are seen as not being directly connected to each other (referred to here as “perceived brokerage potential”).4 (See Kalish and Robins (2006) for a discussion of the advantages of evaluating structural holes using triad-level measures instead of summary measures.)
One problem with this brokerage potential measure is that it does not rule out the possibility that other intermediaries aside from ego exist within the network. A lack of a tie between two alters does not necessarily mean that a structural hole exists there. Technically, ego (pi) only bridges a structural hole between pj and pk when ego is the only intermediary between them. Superior measures of bridging, then, would eliminate the possibility that some fourth party serves as an alternative intermediary between pj and pk, which occurs if pj and pk are both tied to a common third alter, thus creating a four-cycle (see Kalish and Robins 2006; Lazega and Pattison 1999; Pattison and Robins 2002). One useful measure, then, is a dichotomous indicator of whether the respondent reports being the sole intermediary between any of their network members (“sole bridge”) within the perceived network. Of course, we can not completely rule out the possibility that there is some fourth person out there who is also linked to pj and pk, but we at least know that there is no such person (according to the respondent) within the egocentric network.5
Independent Variables
The key variables used in the analyses are summarized in Table 1. According to the research discussed earlier, at least two aspects of health may influence (perceived) bridging capacity. First, functional health is measured using an index of 9 questions about respondents’ difficulty with activities of daily living, such as: “How much difficulty do you have bathing or showering?” I reverse-code and standardize these responses, then average them together to create a functional health index (α = .86), where positive values indicate better function. Cognitive function is measured using the Short Portable Mental Status Questionnaire (SPMSQ), which includes questions like “What day of the week is it?” It is scored as a count of the number of such items the respondent gets incorrect, with a maximum of 10 (Pfeiffer 1975).6
Table 1.
Variable | Weighted Mean | Standard Deviation | |
---|---|---|---|
Bridging measures | |||
Brokerage potential | Number of pairs of alters in R’s network that are not directly connected to each other. Range: 0 to 10. | 1.587 | 2.107 |
Sole bridge status | Whether R serves as the sole intermediary between alters who otherwise would not be connected to each other. {Yes =1, no =0} | .196 | .399 |
Network structural controls | |||
Kin composition | Proportion of alters who are kin by blood or marriage is calculated, then divided into tertiles to reduce skewness. Lowest tertile = 0, highest = 2. | 1.068 | .776 |
Closeness to alters | Average response to: “How close do you feel is your relationship with [name]?” Responses range from “not very close” (=1) to “extremely close” (=4). | 3.115 | .508 |
Frequency of contact with alters | Rs were asked how often they contact each alter. Eight possible responses range from “less than once a year” to “every day.” We transform responses to estimates of number of days of contact per year w/each alter (e.g., “every day” =365). We then take the average across all alters. Observed range: 2 to 365. | 198.256 | 85.448 |
Number of alter pairs | Number of alter-alter pairs in R’s network. Range: 0 to 10. | 6.123 | 3.597 |
Sociodemographic characteristics | |||
Age | R’s age in years, divided by 10. Range: 5.7 to 8.5. | 6.785 | .782 |
Female | Whether R is female {Yes =1, no =0} | .541 | .498 |
African-American | Whether R is African-American {Yes =1, no =0} | .093 | .366 |
Latino | Whether R is Latino. {Yes =1, no =0} | .063 | .295 |
Education | (Ref.) R had no high school education {1 =Yes, 0 =No} | ||
R graduated high school {1 =Yes, 0 =No} | .278 | .445 | |
R had some college {1 =Yes, 0 =No} | .292 | .448 | |
R graduated college {1 =Yes, 0 =No} | .258 | .423 | |
Retired | Whether R is retired {Yes =1, no =0} | .588 | .484 |
Marital status | (Ref.) R is married/lives with partner {1 =Yes, 0 =No} | ||
R is separated/divorced {1 =Yes, 0 =No} | .112 | .329 | |
R is widowed {1 =Yes, 0 =No} | .170 | .416 | |
R was never married {1 =Yes, 0 =No} | .029 | .178 | |
Depression | Average of 11 standardized ordinal items from a modified CES-D (α = .80). Range: −.603 to 2.727. | −.033 | .569 |
Cognitive function | Number of items from the short portable mental status questionnaire (SPMSQ), such as “What day of the week is it?”, that R answered correctly. Range: 0 to 10. | 9.489 | .996 |
Functional health | Average of 9 ordinal items (reverse-coded) assessing R’s difficulty with ADLS (α = .78). Range: −3 to 0. | −.259 | .485 |
Means incorporate person-level weights, with post-stratification adjustments for non-response. Estimates pertain to all respondents who have at least two network members and information on all of the variables used in the regression analysis.
I control for several sociodemographic characteristics, including age, gender, race/ethnicity, education, employment status, and marital status. I also control for depression, since not doing so may make it difficult to tell to what extent any associations between SPMSQ score and bridging are due to cognitive impairment or psychological distress, which covary. Following my arguments concerning the potential for indirect association between health and bridging, I also consider several structural characteristics of respondents’ networks. Factors that may reduce bridging potential include: 1) high frequency of contact with network members, since frequent interaction increases the likelihood that alters will know each other; 2) closeness to network members, for similar reasons; and 3) having a large contingent of kin in the network. (Note that the frequency of interaction measure is not included in the analysis of the frequency-based measure of bridging, however.) Network size is taken into account in some form in all of these analyses, as described below. Correlations among the measures of respondents’ bridging and other network characteristics are presented in Table 2.
Table 2.
Variable | (1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(1) Brokerage potential | -- | ||||
(2) Sole bridge status | .621*** | -- | |||
(3) Closeness to alters | −.245*** | −.214*** | -- | ||
(4) Frequency of contact | −.375*** | −.259*** | .309*** | -- | |
(5) Kin composition | −.547*** | −.382*** | .306*** | .250*** | -- |
(6) Number of alter pairs | .501*** | .114*** | −.093*** | −.269*** | −.242*** |
p < .001 (two-tailed tests)
Estimates pertain to all respondents who have at least two network members and information on all of the variables used in the regression analysis, including the sociodemographic and health measures.
Analysis
My argument is that health problems can limit individuals’ bridging in a couple of ways: 1) by making it more difficult to meet the physical and/or cognitive demands involved in attaining and maintaining bridging positions, or to perceive structural holes; and 2) by altering the composition of individuals’ social networks in such a way that there are fewer weak ties, which, in turn, is likely to reduce bridging. The latter is an indirect pathway, whereby network composition mediates the relationship between health and perceived bridging. The NSHAP data are cross-sectional and there are no viable instruments, so I cannot definitively demonstrate causality. But I can at least perform a mediation model (Baron and Kenny 1986; Judd and Kenny 1981) to determine which pathway is more plausible based on the available data.
The first step in the mediation model is to demonstrate that there is a relationship between health and perceived bridging. This is accomplished via regression analyses predicting both of the bridging measures using measures of network composition (controlling for sociodemographic measures). Perceived brokerage potential, which is measured as the number of pairs of alters in an individual’s network that are seen as not being directly connected to each other, is predicted using negative binomial regression. The count of the total number of pairs of alters in the network (connected or not) is used as the exposure variable. This model essentially predicts the proportion of pairs of alters that are not seen as being directly connected to each other (i.e., the inverse of network density). Negative binomial models are like Poisson models in that they predict count outcomes, but negative binomial models also include a parameter, σ2, that accounts for overdispersion (McCullagh and Nelder 1989). Sole bridging status is predicted using logistic regression. (In this model, a control is added for the number of alter-pairs in the respondent’s network.)
Second and third steps are required to determine if network composition is a potential mediator in any relationship between health and perceived bridging. In the second step, three measures of network composition (kin composition, closeness to network members, and frequency of interaction with network members) are predicted using the three health measures (depression, cognitive function, and functional health), controlling for sociodemographic characteristics. Respondents were asked to rate how close they are to each of their network members on an ordinal scale (1 = “not very close,” 4 = “extremely close”), and these estimates are averaged together. This variable is predicted using survey-adjusted linear regression. Respondents also reported how frequently they interact with each network member on an ordinal scale (1 = “less than once a year,” 8 = “every day”). These reports were used to estimate the number of days per year respondents interact with their network members, and the estimates were then averaged together. This version of the variable works well as a predictor, but it is too skewed for use as a dependent variable in a linear model. Therefore, I sum the estimated number of days per year respondents interact with all of their network members and predict this total using negative binomial regression, and use the number of network members as the exposure variable. Finally, kin composition can be quantified using the proportion of discussion partners who are kin. However, ceiling effects (caused by the cap placed on the number of network members allowed) contributed to skewness which appeared to impair the performance of this variable as a predictor, so it was divided into tertiles to create an ordinal measure. The lowest tertile contains those with less than half kin, and the top tertile contains those with only kin confidantes. This variable is modeled using a survey-adjusted ordered logistic regression model (McCullagh 1980).
In the third step, the bridging measures are predicted using the three measures of network composition. In these models, the network composition measures are employed as predictors, but the health measures are not. If significant relationships are observed in both the second and third steps of this sequence (net of sociodemographic controls), the results are consistent with the idea that network composition may mediate between health and perceived bridging.
Finally, a fourth set of models examines whether health remains a significant predictor of perceived bridging even after network composition measures are included. If not, this can be taken as evidence that network composition fully mediates the relationship between health and perceived bridging. If health remains significant in these final models, network structure is likely only a partial mediator (Kenny, Kashy, and Bolder 1998).
All of these analyses pertain to respondents who have at least two network members. All models include person-level weights to account for differential probabilities of selection (with post-stratification adjustments for non-response by age and urbanicity). Analyses are conducted using Stata’s survey (“svy”) commands which, in addition to weighting, incorporate the clusters and strata of NSHAP’s sample design, and employ Taylor linearized variance estimation.
RESULTS
This analysis pertains to the 2,570 respondents in the NSHAP sample who have at least two network members and who provided data on both measures of bridging. For the most part, perceived bridging opportunities are relatively scarce in these networks. These respondents’ networks contain 9,784 alters in networks of varying size, and 15,201 potential pairs of alters. Overall, 11,337 (about 75 percent) of these alter-pairs are reportedly directly connected to each other. About half of the respondents report at least one disconnected pair in their network, and about 35 percent report more than one. The average number of disconnected pairs per network (that is, the number of alter-pairs without an apparent direct connection to each other) is 1.6. None of this is surprising, as core discussion networks tend to be quite dense, both among older adults and in the general population (Cornwell et al. 2008; Marsden 1987).
Table 3 presents results from the regression analyses predicting the number of pairs of alters in older adults’ core networks that are reportedly not directly connected to each other (i.e., “perceived brokerage potential”). I will not spend much time discussing the relationship between sociodemographic controls and perceived bridging. There are some general trends, however, that are worth pointing out. The oldest adults, women, and unmarried respondents all tend to report fewer unconnected alter-pairs in their networks and are less likely to report being the only bridge between unconnected network members. Latinos and those with less formal education tend to have less perceived brokerage potential in their networks, and retirees are less likely to report having sole bridging status. Finally, depressive symptomology is positively associated with bridging. (Although this is not a focus of my analysis, it would be interesting to explore to what extent this reflects role conflict or stress associated with maintaining ties to disconnected network members, or, alternatively, the negative effects of not being embedded in a tight-knit, dense network. Depression is mainly included here because it is a consistent covariate of the SPMSQ measure.)
Table 3.
Predictors | Brokerage potential (Negative binomial)a | Sole bridge status (Logistic)b | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Age (divided by 10) | 10.293* (.038) | 4.591 (.027) | 1.218* (.119) | 1.131 (.120) |
Female | 27.437*** (.041) | 28.235*** (.040) | 1.449** (.188) | 1.691** (.273) |
African-American | −9.691 (.066) | 1.863 (.055) | .929 (.151) | 1.227 (.207) |
Latino | −32.354** (.123) | −9.698 (.113) | .679 (.137) | .918 (.230) |
Education (ref = less than high school) | ||||
Graduated high school | −2.432 (.084) | −3.777 (.084) | .974 (.160) | .882 (.177) |
Some college | 31.254** (.083) | 13.958 (.081) | 1.374 (.263) | 1.054 (.214) |
College degree | 42.377*** (.080) | 15.756* (.072) | 1.191 (.238) | .771 (.159) |
Retired | −7.144 (.065) | −13.355*** (.046) | .725* (.096) | .621** (.091) |
Marital status (ref = married/partnered) | ||||
Separated/divorced | 72.012*** (.061) | 25.626*** (.058) | 4.722*** (.821) | 3.199*** (.589) |
Widowed | 11.342 (.071) | 11.272 (.074) | 2.227*** (.354) | 2.343*** (.430) |
Never married | 75.802*** (.124) | 15.597 (.107) | 5.321*** (1.637) | 2.832** (1.026) |
Depression | 16.291** (.043) | 9.054* (.036) | 1.352** (.141) | 1.282* (.142) |
Cognitive function | 7.907* (.034) | 5.690* (.024) | 1.185* (.079) | 1.213** (.083) |
Functional health | 20.380* (.088) | 4.095 (.078) | 1.551** (.204) | 1.368* (.194) |
Closeness to network members | −12.212* (.056) | .616** (.096) | ||
Frequency of interaction w/network members | −.379*** (.000) | .993*** (.001) | ||
Kin composition | −50.154*** (.039) | .326*** (.030) | ||
F (d.f.) | 18.57*** (14, 36) | 87.03*** (17, 33) | 13.22*** (15, 35) | 23.47*** (18, 32) |
p < .05,
p < .01,
p < .001 (two-tailed tests)
The total number of pairs of alters in the network is included as the exposure variable. Negative binomial coefficients are transformed to reflect percent change ([(exp b) − 1]100). Standard errors for the original coefficients are in parentheses.
Includes control for the number of alter-pairs in the network.
I will spend most of the results section examining associations between the two health measures and perceived bridging. The first step is to establish that there is an association between these sets of variables. The two initial models in Table 3 (columns 1 and 3) show that there are significant positive relationships between respondents’ cognitive and functional health, on the one hand, and perceived bridging, net of sociodemographic measures. A one-unit increment in cognitive function (equivalent to answering one additional question correctly on the SPMSQ) is associated with reporting about 8 percent more unconnected alter-pairs in one’s networks. It is also associated with 19 percent greater odds of reporting being the sole bridge between any two pairs of network members. Better functional health is also associated with more bridging. A one-unit increment in functional health (e.g., reporting “no difficulty” with all of the ADLs as opposed to reporting “some difficulty” with them) is associated with a 20 percent increment in brokerage potential as well as 55 percent greater odds of reporting being the sole link between two unconnected network members.
One of my hypotheses is that network structure partially mediates the relationship between health and bridging. The second step in exploring mediation is to determine if there is a link between health and network composition. Results of this part of the analysis (presented in Table 4) suggest that older adults who have slightly better cognitive function actually have closer contacts in their networks. Yet, at the same time, cognitive function is not significantly associated with either frequency of contact with network members or kin composition. Functional health, on the other hand, is consistently negatively associated with all three measures of network tie strength. On average, older adults who are better able to complete everyday activities are less close to their network members, on average, have less contact with them, and have fewer kin ties in their networks. The implications of these results for the mediation hypothesis will be addressed momentarily.
Table 4.
Predictors | Closeness to Network Members (Linear regression) | Frequency of contact w/network members (Negative binomial)a | Kin composition (Ordered logistic)b |
---|---|---|---|
Age (divided by 10) | −.079*** (.016) | −4.508* (.018) | −.039 (.070) |
Female | .115*** (.024) | 2.449 (.019) | −.158 (.124) |
African-American | .107** (.029) | 14.823** (.038) | .173 (.133) |
Latino | −.034 (.034) | 15.013*** (.030) | .776*** (.158) |
Education (ref = less than high school) | |||
Graduated high school | −.044 (.033) | −2.666 (.027) | .031 (.150) |
Some college | −.062 (.032) | −7.796* (.031) | −.383** (.124) |
College degree | −.093** (.034) | −15.198*** (.033) | −.482** (.152) |
Retired | .024 (.025) | −8.793*** (.024) | .057 (.089) |
Marital status (ref = married/partnered) | |||
Separated/divorced | −.113** (.034) | −13.412*** (.031) | −.943*** (.107) |
Widowed | −.011 (.027) | −4.324 (.027) | −.097 (.095) |
Never married | −.267** (.073) | −16.437* (.073) | −1.046*** (.280) |
Depression | −.111*** (.026) | −3.982* (.019) | −.081 (.084) |
Cognitive function | .030* (.012) | −1.300 (.007) | −.096 (.060) |
Functional health | −.056* (.026) | −7.655** (.026) | −.271* (.110) |
F (d.f.) | 9.68*** (14, 36) | 10.01*** (14, 36) | 19.17*** (15, 35) |
p < .05,
p < .01,
p < .001 (two-tailed tests)
The total number of pairs of alters in the network is included as the exposure variable. Negative binomial coefficients are transformed to reflect percent change ([(exp b) − 1]100). Standard errors for the original coefficients are in parentheses.
Coefficients are odds ratios representing the relative likelihood of being in a higher category of kin composition per one-unit increment in the corresponding predictor. Number of children is included as a predictor.
The third step is to determine if there is a link between the potential mediator (network composition) and the main outcome of interest (bridging). Table 5 shows that network composition is indeed strongly and consistently associated with bridging. Older adults who are closer to their network members report having fewer unconnected network members and are less likely to serve as the sole perceived bridge between any pair of alters within their networks. Similarly, those who have more frequent interaction with their network members and/or who have more kin-based networks also tend to report less brokerage potential and lower odds of reportedly being the sole bridge between unconnected network members. Therefore, as structural network research would suggest, individuals who are ensconced in strong-tie networks have fewer bridging opportunities.
Table 5.
Predictors | Brokerage potential (Negative binomial)a | Sole bridge status (Logistic)b |
---|---|---|
Age (divided by 10) | 3.175 (.027) | 1.045 (.107) |
Female | 28.606*** (.040) | 1.637** (.269) |
African-American | .984 (.055) | 1.176 (.203) |
Latino | −10.693 (.110) | .878 (.219) |
Education (ref = less than high school) | ||
Graduated high school | −2.597 (.085) | .997 (.197) |
Some college | 15.916 (.076) | 1.209 (.249) |
College degree | 16.868* (.066) | .874 (.168) |
Retired | −13.488** (.046) | .628** (.091) |
Marital status (ref = married/partnered) | ||
Separated/divorced | 26.239*** (.058) | 3.192*** (.579) |
Widowed | 12.545 (.072) | 2.390*** (.421) |
Never married | 14.546 (.105) | 2.616** (.897) |
Closeness to network members | −12.658* (.053) | .615** (.098) |
Frequency of interaction w/network members | −.384*** (.000) | .993*** (.001) |
Kin composition | −50.218*** (.041) | .327*** (.032) |
F (d.f.) | 98.03*** (14, 36) | 26.66*** (15, 35) |
p < .05,
p < .01,
p < .001 (two-tailed tests)
The total number of pairs of alters in the network is included as the exposure variable. Negative binomial coefficients are transformed to reflect percent change ([(exp b) − 1]100). Standard errors for the original coefficients are in parentheses.
Includes control for the number of alter-pairs in the network.
The key question now is to what extent network composition may mediate the relationship between health and bridging. We saw earlier that cognitive function is not significantly associated with two of the three measures of network structure. At the same time, cognitive function remains significantly associated with both measures of bridging, net of the three measures of network composition, as shown in Table 6. Taken together, these results suggest a non-mediated relationship between cognitive function and (perceived) bridging. It would appear, then, that the association between cognitive function and bridging does not have as much to do with cognitive decline leading to greater support by primary group members, or the loss of weak contacts who cannot cope with caregiving duties, as it does with the more direct difficulties cognitive impairments introduce into the task of executing bridging duties (e.g., being able to deal with the coordinating challenges of brokerage) or the task of perceiving bridging opportunities in one’s network, regardless of network composition.
However, mediation may play a role in the relationship between functional health and bridging. The final models in Table 3 (columns 2 and 4) show that this relationship is at least partially attenuated when network composition is controlled. The association between functional health and the reported number of unconnected alter-pairs in ego’s network is reduced to non-significance, and the relationship between functional health and sole bridge status is cut by one-third (but remains significant at p < .05). These results suggest that the positive relationship between functional health and perceived bridging is at least partially mediated by network composition, and may even be fully mediated in the case of brokerage potential. Older adults who suffer from functional health problems (e.g., limited mobility) have stronger, more frequently accessed, and more kin-based network ties, and this in turn impairs their perceived brokerage and bridging potential.
DISCUSSION
Social network scholars have mainly offered agency-based and structural explanations for the presence of bridges in social networks. I have argued that individuals’ physical and cognitive health may also play a role in bridging. Analyses of older adults’ egocentric social networks suggest that people who have poorer cognitive and functional health may be less likely to span structural holes. Only cognitive function has a consistent positive association with bridging net of other network characteristics like kin composition, however, suggesting several possible explanations.
It could be that the ability to execute the responsibilities and withstand the pressure associated with bridging positions is more mental than it is physical. In this regard, these results could help highlight the cognitive skills that are required to constantly transmit resources and “switch” between separate social domains (see Mische and White 1998; White 2008). To the extent that the personal challenges associated with occupying bridging positions contribute to their rapid decay rate (Burt 2002; Krackhardt 1998), the findings reported here suggest that cognitive function is an important resource for brokers.
At the same time these findings dovetail with work in cognitive psychology which points to the difficulty of identifying bridging opportunities in the first place (see Janicik and Larrick 2005). People who have impaired cognitive capacity may be less able to employ the more complex cognitive schemas that are required to identify bridging opportunities in the first place (see CITE). In this respect, these results could reflect cognitively impaired individuals’ reduced abilities to identify structural holes. This would have both empirical and methodological significance. On one hand, this still means that cognitively impaired people are less able to make strategic use of bridging opportunities, as they are less able to spot them in the first place. On the other hand, it suggests a potential limitation of egocentric network data in studying even mildly cognitively impaired individuals’ social networks. This should be of particular concern to social gerontologists studying older adults’ networks. More research will be needed to disentangle the effects of cognitive impairment on the ability to perceive structural holes and the ability to manage and execute actual bridging responsibilities.
Functional health may affect more than just one’s ability to occupy bridging positions. The link between functional health and bridging is partly due to the fact that those who have functional health problems like limited mobility have more strong tie-based networks. The way one’s network members react to one’s impairments may lead to a closing of structural holes. It may be that spouses and other close kin who are bound by strong social obligations respond with unconditional social support to help functionally impaired individuals perform everyday tasks, whereas weaker contacts drop out for one reason or another. Again, the exact mechanisms that give rise to this association need to be pinned down.
More research on the structural implications of health is badly needed. The cross-sectional nature of the NSHAP data leaves the issue of reverse-causation unaddressed here. An obvious counter-argument here is that people who do less bridging in their networks are more likely to develop health problems. While there is overwhelming evidence for the salubrious effects of social connectedness on overall self-rated health, mortality, and depression, there is much less research on how social network position affects health – not to mention specific aspects of health like kinesthetic and cognitive function. An intriguing possibility is that being around structural holes is good for one’s cognitive functioning. Some research in health studies suggests that involvement in engaging social environments is protective against cognitive decline (Bassuk, Glass, and Berkman 1999). With the need to coordinate contact and resource flow between unconnected parties, the process of repeated switching between them may be at the root of the link between bridging and cognitive health. Another argument, relating to Granovetter’s (1973) weak-tie theory, is that bridges provide better access to information about health, treatment, and prevention, which could help to at least slow the onset of health problems. Regardless of the mechanism, reverse-causation poses an interesting empirical possibility that will need to be examined in future work using longitudinal data.
Also, there is a legitimate concern here regarding the measurement of cognitive function. Severely cognitively impaired individuals are not included in the study, which leaves us with a fairly cognitively intact sample. This may render my analyses more conservative. A more serious concern relates to how people responded to the network name generator during the interviews. It is possible that those who have cognitive impairments were more likely to forget to list people who are more peripheral in their structure – for instance, people they speak to less often – thus artificially biasing their networks toward higher density levels. But if this is occurring, it is not just a matter of reporting bias. Rather, as implied above, it speaks directly to cognitively impaired respondents’ abilities to recall information about their closest network contacts. It betrays a limited capacity to remember key information about one’s network, such as who has which resources. This also would be detrimental to one’s ability to realize bridging and brokerage potential and to execute duties associated with it. So, while a reporting bias that is correlated with respondents’ SPMSQ scores may indicate a spurious link between cognitive function and network bridging positionality, it still calls our focus to the issue of cognitively impaired individuals’ abilities to realize bridging potential and to execute the actions associated with it.
Another issue that should be explored in future research is the possibility that the mechanisms that link individuals’ health to bridging may operate at the triad, as well as the individual, level. For one, it may oversimplify matters to assume that poor health affects all ego-alter-alter triads in the same way. It may be that some structural holes within ego’s network are more difficult to span than others, such as where ego acts as a broker of extremely high volumes of valuable information. In addition, my argument implies that alters’ health should matter as well. (Unfortunately, information about alters’ health is not currently available in NSHAP.) Both of these possibilities suggest triad-level mechanisms. As such, a multilevel model, in which ego-alter-alter triads are treated as nested components of individuals’ networks, may be more appropriate than the individual-level model used here. There are several potential modeling techniques that could be adapted for this situation (see, for example, Kalmijn and Vermunt 2007; van Duijn, van Busschbach, and Snijders 1999; Wellman and Frank 2001), although most multilevel models for egocentric networks consider ego-alter pairs, which means the models would need to be generalized to account for repeated observations of alters within alter-alter dyads as well.
Bridges play an important role in many important social processes. Bridges facilitate the flow and diffusion of information and other resources through social networks, tie disparate social groups together, and help to create small worlds. For individuals, bridges affect one’s ability to control the resources that flow through local social structure. From demographic and social-gerontological perspectives, the link between health and bridging may become an increasingly important issue over the next several decades. The population is aging rapidly, which may mean an increase in the overall prevalence of the types of cognitive and functional impairments that reduce individuals’ bridging capacities. The implications of this shift for both global social network connectivity and for the distribution of power and control over the flow of resources through social networks need to be explored.
Footnotes
Additional information about the NSHAP study can be found at: http://www.norc.org/NSHAP.
Respondents’ interpretations of what is “important” varies, as does the content of discussion with different alters (Bearman and Parigi 2004; Straits 2000). These variations do not substantially impact many of the characteristics of networks (Bailey and Marsden 1999). But to minimize potential impacts, NSHAP embedded leading examples in the name generator (see above). Also, the survey content which precedes the name generator may affect interpretations of the item and the number of alters named (McPherson, Smith-Lovin, and Brashears 2006; Sudman, Bradburn, and Schwarz 1996). NSHAP was designed with the social networks module appearing first, so interview order is not an issue here.
It is useful to experiment with alternative definitions of whether ties to and among alters in the network exist. In this paper, I define a network tie as present between two persons if contact occurs between them at least once a year. I conducted a supplementary analysis in which a tie is said to exist only when contact occurs at least once a month, and another using once a week as a cutoff. Using these alternative cutoffs does not affect the main findings, except that the positive association between functional health and bridging becomes increasingly significant as more stringent definitions of contact are used.
We could also assess the effective size of a person’s network, which estimates the number of non-redundant contacts (Burt 1992). However, because effective size is simply a scaled measure of density (Borgatti 1997), it is highly correlated with the brokerage potential measure (r =.96), which us easier to interpret.
Thus, the validity of this measure partly relies on the assumption that because both pj and pk are strongly linked to pi, any third alter who might link the two is likely to be strongly tied to pi as well (Granovetter 1973), and therefore would be present in the network if relevant as an alternative intermediary. If not, we would expect this third alter to be weakly tied to pj and pk .
Table 1 shows that there is little variation in the SPMSQ. Most people answered all 10 items correctly. About five percent of the sample answered three or more incorrectly, however, suggesting mild cognitive impairment according to Pfeiffer (1975). When using a dichotomous indicator of whether respondents scored above this cutoff, the SPMSQ is still significantly associated with bridging.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Adams RG, Bleiszner R. Aging well with friends and family. The American Behavioral Scientist. 1995;39:209–224. [Google Scholar]
- Aldrich H, Herker D. Boundary spanning roles and organization structure. The Academy of Management Review. 1977;2:217–230. [Google Scholar]
- Antonucci TC, Akiyama H. Social networks in adult life and a preliminary examination of the convoy model. Journal of Gerontology. 1987;42:519–527. doi: 10.1093/geronj/42.5.519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aral SO. Behavioral aspects of sexually transmitted diseases: Core groups and bridge populations. Sexually Transmitted Diseases. 2000;27:327–328. doi: 10.1097/00007435-200007000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey S, Marsden PV. Interpretation and interview context: Examining the General Social Survey name generator using cognitive methods. Social Networks. 1999;21:287–309. [Google Scholar]
- Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51:1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bassuk SS, Glass TA, Berkman LF. Social disengagement and incident cognitive decline in community-dwelling elderly persons. Annals of Internal Medicine. 1999;131:165–173. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-131-3-199908030-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bearman P, Parigi P. Cloning headless frogs and other important matters: Conversation topics and network structure. Social Forces. 2004;83:535–557. [Google Scholar]
- Becker GS. Health as human capital: Synthesis and extensions. Oxford Economic Papers. 2007;59:379–410. [Google Scholar]
- Beggs JJ, Haines VA, Hurlbert JS. Situational contingencies surrounding the receipt of informal support. Social Forces. 1996;75:201–222. [Google Scholar]
- Berkman LF, Kawachi I. Social Epidemiology. Oxford University Press; New York: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Beyerlein K, Hipp JR. Social capital, too much of a good thing? American religious traditions and community crime. Social Forces. 2005;84:995–1013. [Google Scholar]
- Blau PM. Heterogeneity and Inequality. Free Press; New York: 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Borgatti SP. Structural holes: Unpacking Burt’s redundancy measures. Connections. 1997;20:35–38. [Google Scholar]
- Bott E. Family and Social Network. Tavistock; London: 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Burgess EW, editor. Aging in Western Societies. University of Chicago Press; 1960. [Google Scholar]
- Burt RS. Positions in networks. Social Forces. 1976;55:93–122. [Google Scholar]
- Burt RS. Network items and the General Social Survey. Social Networks. 1984;6:293–339. [Google Scholar]
- Burt RS. Structural Holes: The Social Structure of Competition. Harvard University Press; Cambridge: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Burt RS. Bridge decay. Social Networks. 2002;24:333–364. [Google Scholar]
- Burt RS. Structural holes and good ideas. American Journal of Sociology. 2004;110:349–399. [Google Scholar]
- Burt RS. Brokerage and closure: An introduction to social capital. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Burt RS, Jannotta JE, Mahoney JT. Personality correlates of structural holes. Social Networks. 1998;20:63–87. [Google Scholar]
- Cohler B. Autonomy and interdependence in the family of adulthood: A psychological perspective. The Gerontologist. 1983;23:33–40. doi: 10.1093/geront/23.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coleman JS. Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal of Sociology. 1988;94:S95–S120. [Google Scholar]
- Coleman JS. Foundations of Social Theory. Harvard University Press; Cambridge: 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Cornwell B, Laumann EO, Schumm LP. The social connectedness of older adults: A national profile. American Sociological Review. 2008;73:185–203. doi: 10.1177/000312240807300201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coyne JC, Wortman CB, Lehman DR. The other side of support: emotional over-involvement and miscarried helping. In: Gottlieb BH, editor. Marshalling Social Support: Formats, Processes, and Effects. Sage; Beverly Hills, CA: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Davis JA, Leinhardt S. The structure of positive interpersonal relations in small groups. In: Berger J, editor. Sociological Theories in Progress. Houghton Mifflin; Boston: 1972. [Google Scholar]
- DeSoto CB. Learning a social structure. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1960;60:417–421. doi: 10.1037/h0047511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeSoto CB, Kuethe JL. Subjective probabilities of interpersonal relationships. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1959;59:290–294. doi: 10.1037/h0045430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dowd JJ. Aging as exchange: A preface to theory. Journal of Gerontology. 1973;30:584–594. doi: 10.1093/geronj/30.5.584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emirbayer M, Mische A. What is agency? American Journal of Sociology. 1998;103:962–1023. [Google Scholar]
- Feld SL. The focused organization of social ties. The American Journal of Sociology. 1981;86:1015–1035. [Google Scholar]
- Flynn FJ, Reagans RE, Amanatullah ET, Ames DR. Helping one’s way to the top: Self-monitors achieve status by helping others and knowing who helps whom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;91:1123–1137. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.6.1123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank A. At the Will of the Body: Reflections on Illness. Houghton Mifflin; Boston: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman LC, Freeman SC, Michaelson AG. On human social intelligence. Journal of Social and Biological Structures. 1988;11:415–425. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman LC, Romney AK, Freeman SC. Cognitive structure and informant accuracy. American Anthropologist. 1987;89:311–325. [Google Scholar]
- Friedkin NE. Structural cohesion and equivalence explanations of social homogeneity. Sociological Methods and Research. 1984;12:235–261. [Google Scholar]
- Friedman RA, Podolny J. Differentiation of boundary spanning roles: Labor negotiations and implications for role conflict. Administrative Science Quarterly. 1992;37:28–47. [Google Scholar]
- Gorbach PM, Sopheab H, Phalla T, Leng HB, Mills S, Bennett A, Holmes KK. Sexual bridging by Cambodian men: Potential importance for general population spread of STD and HIV epidemics. Sexually Transmitted Diseases. 2000;27:320–326. doi: 10.1097/00007435-200007000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gould RV. Power and social structure in community elites. Social Forces. 1989;68:531–552. [Google Scholar]
- Gould RV, Fernandez RM. Structures of mediation: A formal approach to brokerage in transaction networks. Sociological Methodology. 1989;19:89–126. [Google Scholar]
- Gouldner AW. The norm of reciprocity: A preliminary statement. American Sociological Review. 1960;25:161–178. [Google Scholar]
- Granovetter MS. The strength of weak ties. The American Journal of Sociology. 1973;78:1360–1380. [Google Scholar]
- Hallinan MT, Kubitschek WN. The effects of individual and structural characteristics on intransitivity in social networks. Social Psychology Quarterly. 1988;51:81–92. [Google Scholar]
- Harris DK. Sociology of aging. Harper & Row; New York: 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Hill MD. Class, kinship density, and conjugal role segregation. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1988;50:731–741. [Google Scholar]
- Holland PW, Leinhardt S. A method for detecting structure in sociometric data. American Journal of Sociology. 1970;70:492–513. [Google Scholar]
- Horan WP, Subotnik KL, Snyder KS, Neuchterlein KH. Do recent-onset schizophrenia patients experience a “social network crisis”? Psychiatry. 2006;69:115–129. doi: 10.1521/psyc.2006.69.2.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- House J, Umberson DJ, Landis K. Structures and processes of social support. Annual Review of Sociology. 1988;14:293–318. [Google Scholar]
- Ingersoll-Dayton B, Antonucci TC. Reciprocal and nonreciprocal social supports: Contrasting sides of intimate relationships. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences. 1988;43:S65–S73. doi: 10.1093/geronj/43.3.s65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jang Y, Haley WE, Small BJ, Mortimer JA. The role of mastery and social resources in the associations between disability and depression in later life. Gerontologist. 2002;42:807–813. doi: 10.1093/geront/42.6.807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janicik GA, Larrick RP. Social network schemas and the learning of incomplete networks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;88:348–364. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Judd CM, Kenny DA. Process analysis: Estimating mediation in treatment evaluations. Evaluation Review. 1981;5:602–619. [Google Scholar]
- Kadushin C. The motivational foundation of social networks. Social Networks. 2002;24:77–91. [Google Scholar]
- Kahn RL, Antonucci T. Convoys of social support: A life course approach. In: Kiesler SB, Morgan JN, Oppenheimer VK, editors. Aging: Social Change. Academic Press; New York: 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Kahn RL, Wolfe DM, Quinn R, Snoek JD. Organizational Stress: Studies in Role Conflict and Ambiguity. John Wiley; New York: 1964. [Google Scholar]
- Kalish Y, Robins G. Psychological predispositions and network structure: The relationships between individual predispositions, structural holes and network closure. Social Networks. 2006;28:56–84. [Google Scholar]
- Kalmijn M, Vermunt JK. Homogeneity of social networks by age and marital status: A multilevel analysis of ego-centered networks. Social Networks. 2007;29:25–43. [Google Scholar]
- Kazak AE, Marvin RS. Differences, difficulties and adaptation: Stress and social networks in families with a handicapped child. Family Relations. 1984;33:67–77. [Google Scholar]
- Kenny DA, Kashy DA, Bolger N. Data analysis in social psychology. In: Gilbert D, Fiske S, Lindzey G, editors. The Handbook of Social Psychology. 4. Vol. 1. McGraw-Hill; Boston: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Klein KJ, Lim BC, Saltz JL, Mayer DM. How do they get there? An examination of the antecedents of centrality in team networks. Academy of Management Journal. 2004;47:952–966. [Google Scholar]
- Krackhardt D. Cognitive social structures. Social Networks. 1987;9:109–134. [Google Scholar]
- Krackhardt D. Simmelian ties: Super, strong and sticky. In: Kramer RM, Neale M, editors. Power and Influence in Organizations. Sage; Thousand Oaks, CA: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Krackhardt D. The ties that torture: Simmelian tie analysis in organizations. In: Bacharach SB, Andrews SB, Knoke D, editors. Research in the Sociology of Organizations. Vol. 16. JAI; Stanford, CT: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Krause N. Understanding the stress process: Linking social support with locus of control beliefs. Journal of Gerontology. 1987;42:589–593. doi: 10.1093/geronj/42.6.589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuijer RG, Buunk BP, Ybema JF. Are equity concerns important in the intimate relationship when one partner of a couple has cancer? Social Psychology Quarterly. 2001;64:267–282. [Google Scholar]
- Lachs MS, Berkman L, Fulmer T, Horwitz RI. A prospective community-based pilot study of risk factors for the investigation of elder mistreatment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1994;42:169–173. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1994.tb04947.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laumann EO. Prestige and Association in an Urban Community. Bobbs-Merrill; New York: 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Lazarsfeld PF, Merton RK. Friendship as a social process: A substantive and methodological analysis. In: Berger M, Abel T, Page CH, editors. Freedom and Control in Modern Society. Van Nostrand; New York: 1954. [Google Scholar]
- Lazega E, Pattison PE. Multiplexity, generalized exchange and cooperation in organizations: A case study. Social Networks. 1999;21:67–90. [Google Scholar]
- Lee GR, Netzer JK, Coward RT. Depression among older parents-the role of intergenerational exchange. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1995;57:823–833. [Google Scholar]
- Lester H, Tritter JQ. Listen to my madness”: Understanding the experiences of people with serious mental illness. Sociology of Health and Illness. 2005;27:649–669. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9566.2005.00460.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Y, Ferraro KF. Volunteering in middle and later life: Is health a benefit, barrier or both? Social Forces. 2006;85:497–519. [Google Scholar]
- Marin A. Are respondents more likely to list alters with certain characteristics? Implications for name generator data. Social Networks. 2004;26:289–307. [Google Scholar]
- Marsden PV. Brokerage behavior in restricted exchange networks. In: Marsden PV, Lin N, editors. Social Structure and Network Analysis. Sage; Beverly Hills: 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Marsden PV. Core discussion networks of Americans. American Sociological Review. 1987;52:122–131. [Google Scholar]
- Martire LM, Stephens MAP, Druley JA, Wojno WC. Negative reactions to received spousal care: Predictors and consequences of miscarried support. Health Psychology. 2002;21:167–176. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCullagh P. Regression models for ordinal data (with discussion) Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B. 1980;42:109–142. [Google Scholar]
- McCullagh P, Nelder JA. Generalized Linear Models. 2. Chapman and Hall; New York: 1989. [Google Scholar]
- McPherson M, Smith-Lovin L, Cook JM. Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology. 2001;27:415–444. [Google Scholar]
- McPherson M, Smith-Lovin L, Brashears ME. Social isolation in America: Changes in core discussion networks over two decades. American Sociological Review. 2006;71:353–375. [Google Scholar]
- Mische A, White H. Between conversation and situation: Public switching dynamics across network domains. Social Research. 1998;65:695–724. [Google Scholar]
- Morris M, Podhisita C, Wawer MJ, Handcock M. Bridge populations in the spread of HIV/AIDS in Thailand. AIDS. 1996;10:1265–1271. doi: 10.1097/00002030-199609000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neufeld A, Harrison MJ. Reciprocity and social support in caregivers’ relationships: Variations and consequences. Qualitative Health Research. 1995;5:348–365. [Google Scholar]
- Pattison PE, Robins GL. Neighbourhood based models for social networks. In: Stolzenberg RM, editor. Sociological Methodology. Vol. 22. Blackwell Publishing; Boston: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Pfeiffer E. A short portable mental status questionnaire for the assessment of organic brain deficit in elderly patients. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1975;23:433–441. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1975.tb00927.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Podolny JM, Baron JN. Resources and relationships: Social networks and mobility in the workplace. American Sociological Review. 1997;62:673–693. [Google Scholar]
- Press AN, Crockett WH, Rosenkrantz PS. Cognitive complexity and the learning of balanced and unbalanced social structures. Journal of Personality. 1969;37:541–553. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1969.tb01763.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Putnam RD. Bowling Alone: The Collapse and Revival of American Community. Simon & Schuster; New York: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Quinn MJ, Tomita SK. Elder abuse and neglect: Causes, diagnosis and intervention strategies. Springer; New York: 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Robillard AB. Meaning of a Disability: The Lived Experience of Paralysis. Temple University Press; Philadelphia: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Ruan D. The content of the General Social Survey discussion networks: An exploration of the General Social Survey discussion name generator in a Chinese context. Social Networks. 1998;20:247–264. [Google Scholar]
- Schieman S, Turner HA. Age, disability, and the sense of mastery. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1998;39:169–186. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silverstein M, Chen X, Heller K. Too much of a good thing? Intergenerational social support and the psychological well-being of older parents. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1996;58:970–982. [Google Scholar]
- Simmel G. The isolated individual and the dyad. In: Wolff KH, editor. The Sociology of Georg Simmel. Free Press; New York: 1950a. [Google Scholar]
- Simmel G. The triad. In: Wolff KH, editor. The Sociology of Georg Simmel. Free Press; New York: 1950b. [Google Scholar]
- Spekman RE. Influence and information: An exploratory investigation of the boundary role person’s basis of power. Academy of Management Journal. 1979;22:104–117. [Google Scholar]
- Sprecher S. Equity and social exchange in dating couples: Associations with satisfaction, commitment, and stability. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 2001;63:599–613. [Google Scholar]
- Stokes JP. The relation of social networks and individual difference variables to loneliness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1985;48:981–990. [Google Scholar]
- Stoller EP, Pugliesi KL. Size and effectiveness of informal helping networks: A panel study of older people in the community. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1991;32:180–191. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Straits BC. Ego’s important discussants or significant people: An experiment in varying the wording of personal network name generators. Social Networks. 2000;22:123–140. [Google Scholar]
- Sudman S, Bradburn NM, Schwarz N. Thinking About Answers: The Application of Cognitive Processes to Survey Methodology. Jossey-Bass; San Francisco, CA: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Thoits PA. Stress, coping, and social support processes: Where are we? What next? Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1995;35:53–79. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thoits PA, Hewitt LN. Volunteer work and well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2001;42:115–131. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson SC, Pitts JS. In sickness and in health: Chronic illness, marriage, and spousal caregiving. In: Spacapan S, Oskapm S, editors. Helping and Being Helped. Sage; Newbury Park, CA: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Townsend P. The structured dependency of the elderly: A creation of social policy in the twentieth century. Ageing & Society. 1981;1:5–28. [Google Scholar]
- Turner BS. Ageing, politics and sociological theory. The British Journal of Sociology. 1989;40:588–606. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Utne MK, Hatfield E, Traupmann J, Greenberger D. Equity, marital satisfaction, and stability. Journal of social and Personal Relationships. 1984;1:323–332. [Google Scholar]
- van Duijn M, van Busschbach J, Snijders T. Multilevel analysis of personal networks as dependent variables. Social Networks. 1999;21:187–209. [Google Scholar]
- Ville I, Ravaud JF, Tetrafigap Group. Subjective well-being and severe motor impairments: The Tetrafigap Survey on the long-term outcome of tetraplegic spinal cord injured persons. Social Science and Medicine. 2001;52:369–384. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00140-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vinokur AD, Vinokur KD. In sickness and in health”: Patterns of social support and undermining in older married couples. Journal of Aging and Health. 1990;2:215–241. [Google Scholar]
- Watts DJ. Networks, dynamics, and the small-world phenomenon. The American Journal of Sociology. 1999;105:493–527. [Google Scholar]
- Wellman B. Network analysis: Some basic principles. Sociological Theory. 1983;1:155–200. [Google Scholar]
- Wellman B, Frank KA. Network capital in a multi-level world: Getting support from personal communities. In: Lin N, Cook K, Burt R, editors. Social Capital: Theory and Research. Aldine DeGruyter; Chicago: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Wentkowski GJ. Reciprocity and the coping strategies of older people: Cultural dimensions of network building. The Gerontologist. 1981;21:600–609. doi: 10.1093/geront/21.6.600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White HC. Identity & Control: How Social Formations Emerge. 2. Princeton University Press; Princeton: 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Zajonc RB, Bumstem E. The learning of balanced and unbalanced social structures. Journal of Personality. 1965;33:153–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1965.tb01378.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]