Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008 Sep 17;34(4):964–971. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.141

Two-week treatment with the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram reduces contextual anxiety but not cued fear in healthy volunteers: a fear-potentiated startle study

Christian Grillon 1, Chanen Chavis 1, Matthew F Covington 1, Daniel S Pine 1
PMCID: PMC2639632  NIHMSID: NIHMS64552  PMID: 18800069

Abstract

Chronic treatment with serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) alleviates both anxiety symptoms and associated physiologic disturbances in anxious patients. However, limited research considers the degree to which chronic SSRI treatment influences anxiety in healthy individuals. This study examined the effect of 2-week citalopram treatment on two threat responses: short- and long-duration potentiated startle. Prior work suggests that these two responses provide neurally and functionally distinct models of fear and anxiety, respectively, in rodents. Healthy volunteers (n=53) received either placebo or citalopram (20 mg/day) for two weeks under double-blind conditions. They were each tested twice, before and after treatment. Participants were exposed to three conditions including one in which predictable aversive shocks were signaled by a cue, a second in which unpredictable shocks were anticipated, and a third in which no shocks were administered. Aversive states were indexed by acoustic startle. Phasic fear-potentiated startle to the threat cue as well as sustained startle potentiation to the experimental context in the predictable and unpredictable conditions were investigated. Citalopram affected neither baseline startle nor short-duration fear-potentiated startle to discrete threat cues. However, citalopram reduced long-duration startle potentiation in the predictable conditions. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that short- and long-duration aversive states are mediated by distinct neural systems. They suggest that citalopram alleviates symptoms of anticipatory anxiety, not fear, by acting on mechanisms underlying long-duration aversive states.

Keywords: Fear, anxiety, SSRI, citalopram, startle reflex, predictability

Introduction

Growing evidence suggest that short- and long duration aversive states are functionally distinct (reviewed in (Grillon 2008; Walker and Davis in press). Studies based on the startle reflex suggest that the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) and the bed nucleus of the stria terminali (BNST) are involved, respectively, in phasic fear responses (CeA) to short-duration discrete cues and sustained anticipatory anxiety states to long-duration diffuse cues (BNST) (Walker and Davis in press). More specifically, phasic fear-potentiated startle to a discrete cue that signals a shock is mediated by the medial division of the CeA (CeAM), whereas sustained potentiation of startle to more diffuse threat is mediated by projections from the basolateral amygdala (BLA) and lateral CeA to the BNST (Walker and Davis in press). The hypothesis of a role for the BNST in sustained anxiety is consistent with results showing a role of the BNST in light-enhanced startle (Walker and Davis 1997), context conditioning (Sullivan et al. 2004), Pavlovian fear conditioning with long-duration discrete cues (Waddell et al. 2006), and in the behavioral consequences of uncontrollable stress (Hammack et al. 2004).

If phasic and sustained threat responses reflect activity in dissociable neural systems, a pharmacological dissociation of these responses should be possible. Further, if sustained responses model anticipatory anxiety, one would expect these responses to be sensitive to medications that alleviate anticipatory anxiety in patients. We provided initial evidence for a pharmacological dissociation between phasic fear and sustained anxiety (Grillon et al. 2006). We showed that the benzodiazepine alprazolam did not affect fear-potentiated startle to a threat cue signaling a shock, but reduced the sustained potentiation of startle in contexts (i.e., contextual anxiety) in which shocks were administered (Grillon et al. 2006). These result, together with the recent finding that contextual anxiety but not cued fear is increased in panic disorder (Grillon et al. 2008), suggest that contextual anxiety models a clinically-relevant phenomenon.

In the present study, we investigated the effects of the serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRIs) citalopram on cued fear and contextual anxiety. SSRIs are the medication of choice for anxiety, alleviating, among others, symptoms of anticipatory anxiety. A better understanding of the nature of aversive responses affected by SSRIs may have clinical implications and may help identify neural mechanisms of action of these drugs.

Little is currently known about the effect of SSRIs on experimental anxiety in humans. Most studies have examined the effect of a single dose of SSRIs (Harmer et al. 2003a; Kemp et al. 2004), but acute treatment may not inform on the anxiolytic effects of SSRI, which emerge only after chronic administration. In fact, SSRIs can be anxiogenic initially in patients (Gorman et al. 1987) and healthy volunteers (Grillon et al. 2007). Pharmacologically dissociating acute, anxiogenic from chronic anxiolytic effects in experimentally models would provide clinically-relevant experimental data. However, experimental studies using chronic treatments are rare, due to the ethical and scientific hurdles confronting such research. Importantly, in one of the few available studies in this area, it was found that 7 days of citalopram (20 mg/day) impairs the recognition of facial expression of fear and anger (Harmer et al. 2004), reduces the modulation of startle by affective pictures (Harmer et al. 2004), and decreases amygdala responses to masked fearful facial expression (Harmer et al. 2006). However, the extent to which SSRIs affect emotional responses to more evocative threat cues remains unclear. Moreover, the degree to which these changes manifest with longer exposures more typical of clinical use also has been minimally explored.

Using the startle reflex, the present study examined the effect of two weeks of treatment with cilatopram on two types of responses to the threat of shock: fear associated with a discrete threat cue signaling a shock (fear-potentiated startle) and anticipatory anxiety experienced in the contexts (context-potentiated startle) in which the shocks are administered (Grillon et al. 2006; Grillon et al. 2004). Two such contexts were implemented, one associated with predictable (i.e., signaled by a cue) shocks and the other with unpredictable (i.e., not signaled) shocks. Note that context, as defined here, refers to the background features presented during a specific experimental condition (Otto and Poon 2006). It was expected that citalopram would reduce contextual anxiety. This hypothesis was based on the fact that SSRIs alleviate symptoms of anticipatory anxiety in anxious patients (Romano et al. 2004) and reduce risk assessment, a model of anticipatory anxiety, in mice (Blanchard et al. 2003). Existing data on cued fear generate competing hypotheses. Animal studies suggest that chronic citalopram can reduce cued fear conditioning (Burghardt et al. 2004), suggesting chronic citalopram may also reduce phasic fear in humans. On the other hand, data on experimental anxiety in humans, as noted above, suggest that benzodiazepines target long-duration as opposed to short-duration aversive responses. Similarly, prior clinical data on SSRI treatment suggest that clinically-relevant treatments do not target fear-related symptoms (i.e., simple phobia) (Baldwin et al. 2005). However, no prior data examines experimentally-induced cued fear in humans.

Methods

Participants

Participants were paid healthy volunteers who gave written informed consent approved by the NIMH Human Investigation Review Board. Inclusion criteria included 1) no past or current psychiatric disorders as per Structured Clinical Interview for DSMIV (SCID: (First et al. 1995)), 2) no history of mania in any first degree relatives; 3) no medical condition that interfered with the objectives of the study as established by a physician, and 4) no use of elicit drugs or psychoactive medications as per urine screen. All potential participants met with a psychiatrist prior to providing consent. The full details of potential risks, including the risk for suicidal ideation, were discussed with each subject. They were randomized to the placebo or citalopram group. Twenty eight subjects (19 males) with a mean age of 28.8 years (SD = 6.9 years) were included in the placebo group. Twenty eight subjects also were included in the citalopram group. However, three subjects did not take the medication (see below). Their results were not included in the analysis. The final citalopram group consisted 25 subjects (15 males) with a mean age of 26.8 years (SD = 5.4 years).

Drugs

A double-blind between-group pre-/post treatment design was implemented in which one group received placebo and the other citalopram. Subjects were randomized to receive placebo for 14 days or citalopram at the dose of 10 mg/day for two days followed by 20 mg/day for 12 days. To ensure that subjects took the drugs blood levels of 5-hydroxytryptophan, the precursor of serotonin, were examined at the conclusion of the study. Following the study, subjects were tapered over four days to 10 mg/day. Study medication was then discontinued.

Procedure

Subjects were tested twice, once before and once after treatment. Subjects were seen by a psychiatrist at both the start and the end of the study. Phone contact was also maintained between these visits as well as in the week following the tapering of study medication. The two testing sessions were identical, except that at the end of the first session, subjects were given the placebo or citalopram pills to take every morning for the following 14 days. The second testing session was scheduled on the day of the last pill.

Each session started with a block of nine startle stimuli delivered every 18−25 sec to reduce initial startle reactivity and to assess the effect of treatment on baseline startle. Afterwards the shock workup procedure was initiated, setting the shock intensity at a level that was highly annoying but not painful. All the subjects received three shocks during this period. Fifteen minutes later, participants were given precise instructions regarding the conditions under which they would and would not receive a shock. The experiment consisted of three different conditions (Fig. 1), a no shock condition (N), and two conditions during which shocks were administered either predictably (P), that is, only in the presence of a threat cue, or unpredictably (U). Each condition lasted approximately 150 sec. In each 150-sec condition, an 8-sec cue was presented four times. The cues consisted of different geometric colored shapes for the different conditions (e.g., blue square for N, red circle for P). The cues signaled the possibility of receiving a shock only in the P condition, but had no signal value in the N and U conditions. Participants were verbally instructed regarding the risk of shock in the different conditions and they were informed of the contingency or lack of contingency between shock and cues in the P and U conditions. In addition, instructions were also displayed on a computer monitor throughout the experiment displaying the following information: “no shock” (N), “shock only during shape” (P), or “shock at any time” (U). During each P and U condition, one shock was administered, during the cue in the P condition and in the absence of the cues in the U condition. In each N, P, and U condition, six acoustic startle stimuli were delivered, three during inter-trial intervals (ITI; i.e., between cues) and one during three of the four cues, 5−7 sec following cue onset. The threat experiment consisted of two recording blocks with a 5−10 min rest between blocks. Each block started with the delivery of six startle stimuli (pre-test startle) and consisted of three N, two P, and two U conditions in one of the following two orders: P N U N U N P or U N P N P N U. Each participant was presented both of the orders, with half the participants starting with the P condition. One shock was administered in each individual P and U condition for a total of four shocks in the four P conditions and four shocks in the four U conditions. The shock was delivered 7.5 sec following cue onset in the P condition. It was administered either 7 sec or 10 sec following cue offset in the unpredictable condition. No startle stimuli could follow a shock by less than 10 sec.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Schematic of the experiment. In one block, subjects were presented with three neutral (N), two predictable (P), and two unpredictable (U) contexts (order UNPNPNU as shown or PNUNUNP). Each subject was presented with two blocks, each associated with a different order. Each N, P, and U condition contained four 8-sec duration cues. In the P condition (as shown), the shocks was administered only in the presence of the cue. In the U condition, the shocks were administered randomly in the absence of the cue. In the N condition, no shock was administered. The ↑ indicate a startle stimulus. The ▲ indicates a shock.

During testing, the Spielberger's state portion of the state-trait anxiety inventory questionnaire (Spielberger 1983) was administered three times, just after subjects’ arrival, at the end of the first block, and just after the second block. In addition, after each recording block, subjects retrospectively rated their anxiety level in the presence and absence of the cue in each condition (N, P, U) on an analog scale ranging from 0 (not at all anxious) to 10 (extremely anxious).

Stimuli and Physiological Responses

Stimulation and recording were controlled by a commercial system (Contact Precision Instruments, London, England). The acoustic startle stimulus was a 40-ms duration, 103 dB (A) burst of white noise with a near instantaneous rise time presented binaurally through headphones. The eyeblink reflex was recorded with electrodes placed under the left eye. Amplifier bandwidth was set to 30−500 Hz. The electric shock was produced by a constant current stimulator and administered on the left wrist.

Data Analysis

Peak amplitude of the startle/blink reflex was determined in the 20−100-ms time frame following stimulus onset relative to baseline (average baseline EMG level for the 50 ms immediately preceding stimulus onset) and averaged within each condition, after which they were standardized into T scores. Fear was operationally defined as fear-potentiated startle during the discrete cues. Fear-potentiated startle scores were calculated as the difference between startle magnitudes during the cues minus startle magnitudes during ITI. Anxiety was operationally defined as context-potentiated startle. Context-potentiated startle scores in the P and U conditions were calculated as the increase in ITI startle from the N to the P condition, and from the N to the U condition, respectively. The startle data and retrospective measures of subjective anxiety were analyzed with analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures. Alpha was set at .05 for all statistical tests. Greenhouse-Geisser corrections (GG-ε) were used for main effects and interactions involving factors with more than two levels.

Results

Blood levels of 5-hydroxytryptophan

The mean blood level of 5-hydroxytryptophan in the placebo group was 221 ng/ml (SD = 13 ng/ml, range = 125−379 ng/ml). Blood levels less than 50 ng/ml could not be detected. All subjects in the citalopram group had levels of 50 ng/ml or less, except the three excluded subjects who had levels in the range of the placebo group.

Startle magnitude

Startle magnitudes during baseline and during the cues and ITI in the N, P, and U conditions pre- and post-treatments are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Mean (SEM) startle magnitude (μV) before and after treatment at baseline, and during the cue and ITI

Baseline Neutral Predictable Unpredictable
Cue ITI Cue ITI Cue ITI
Placebo Before treatment 55.5 (2.0) 45.7 (.9) 46.3 (.7) 52.6 (1.0) 49.7 (.9) 52.6 (1.0) 52.0 (1.0)
Placebo After treatment 52.7 (2.0) 44.3 (.8) 44.2 (.9) 52.2 (1.0) 49.3 (.9) 52.6 (1.2) 51.8 (1.2)
Citalopram Before treatment 53.7 (2.2) 44.4 (.9) 43.8 (.7) 53.7 (1.0) 50.8 (1.0) 53.7 (1.0) 53.1 (1.0)
Citalopram After treatment 51.6 (2.0) 44.3 (.7) 44.2 (.8) 51.4 (1.2) 48.1 (1.0) 53.5 (1.2) 52.5 (1.2)

Baseline startle

Baseline startle (Table 1) was not affected by citalopram. A Treatment Group (placebo, citalopram) x Session (pre-treatment, post-treatment) x Sex (males, females) ANOVA conducted on the baseline startle magnitude scores revealed no significant main effect or interaction effect (all p>.1).

Fear-potentiated startle to the threat cue

The fear-potentiated startle scores were analyzed with a Treatment Group (placebo, citalopram) x Sex (males, Females) x Session (pre-treatment, post-treatment) x Condition (N, P, U) ANOVA. As expected, the magnitude of fear-potentiated startle was greater in the P condition compared to the N and U condition (Fig. 2). This was confirmed by a Condition main effect (F(2,98)=17.9, p<.00001, GG-ε=.92) and a Condition quadratic trend (F(1,49)=29.9, p<.00009). This effect was not affected by citalopram as reflected by a non significant Treatment Group x Session x Condition main effect or quadratic trend (F(2,98)-.7, ns and F(1,49)=.1, ns, respectively). An analysis restricted to the predictable condition confirmed the lack of effect of citalopram on cued fear-potentiated startle (Treatment Group x Session: F(1,49)=.8, ns).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Fear-potentiated startle response to the cue in each treatment group before and after treatment: Difference scores reflecting cue minus ITI startle magnitudes in the N, P, and U conditions.

Contextual anxiety

An initial analysis of the ITI startle data (Table I) confirmed our previous results (Grillon et al. 2006) that startle magnitude increased progressively from the N to the P to the U condition. A Treatment Group (placebo, citalopram) x Sex (males, females) x Session (pre-treatment, post-treatment) x Condition (N, P, U) ANOVA confirmed this effect (Condition linear trend: (F(1,49)=160.2, p<.0001). Startle during ITI was differentially affected by citalopram and placebo as reflected by a significant Treatment Group x Session x Condition (F(2,98)=3.3, p<.04, GG-ε=.96) and Treatment Group x Session x Condition quadratic trend (F(1,49)=6.2, p<.02). Fig. 3, which shows the context-potentiated startle data (difference scores for ITI startle between P and N, and U and N), indicates that context-potentiated startle was slightly increased after treatment in the placebo group, but it was reduced in the citalopram group, suggesting an anxiolytic effect of citalopram, especially in the P condition. These observations were confirmed statistically. Treatment Group x Session ANOVAs on the context-potentiated startle scores showed reduced contextual potentiation of startle in the citalopram group in the P condition (Treatment Group x Session interaction: F(1,51)=8.3, p<.006), while the reduction in the U condition failed to reach significance (F(1,51)=2.4, p=.12). Further, the reduction of startle potentiation in the P condition by citalopram was confirmed by within-group analyses that showed a significant Session x Condition interaction for the N vs. P condition comparison in the citalogram group (F(1,24)=6.0, p<.02), but not the placebo group (F(1,24)=2.2, ns).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Context-potentiated startle in each treatment group before and after treatment: Difference scores between ITI startle magnitudes in the threat conditions (P and U) and ITI startle magnitude in the N condition. * indicates a significant (p<.05) reduction after treatment.

Females showed greater contextual anxiety compared to males (F(2,98)=4.0, p<.04, GG-ε=.87), but this association was not moderated by citalopram (F(2,98)=.8, ns), GG-ε=.87). The mean startle potentiation t-score from the no shock to the P and U conditions (averaged together) was 6.5 (sem=.9) and 5.6 (sem=1.1) in females and males, respectively.

Retrospective ratings of anxiety

The subjective anxiety ratings were analyzed in analogous manner as the startle data.

Cued fear

The retrospective anxiety rating scores (Table 3) were analyzed in the same way as the fear-potentiated startle data, i.e., using difference scores. Subjective anxiety to the cues (relative to ITI) was greater in the P condition compared to the N and U condition (Condition main effect: (F(2,98)=85.3, p<.0001, GG-ε=.71). This effect was not affected by citalopram as reflected by a non significant Treatment Group x Session x Condition (F(2,98)=1.5, ns). An analysis restricted to the P condition also did not reveal any effect of citalopram on subjective anxiety during the cue (Treatment Group x Session, F(1,49)=1.9, ns).

Table 3.

Mean (SEM) state anxiety at three time points during testing


Arrival
After 1st block
After 2nd block
Placebo pre-treatment 26.6 (1.3) 30.9 (1.7) 28.6 (1.6)
    post-treatment
28.3 (1.4)
30.3 (1.8)
28.2 (1.4)
Citalopram pre-treatment 27.3 (1.3) 31.3 (1.7) 29.1 (1.6)
    post-treatment 27.4 (1.8) 30.0 (1.7) 28.2 (1.4)

Contextual anxiety

Like ITI startle amplitude, subjective anxiety (Table 3) increased linearly from the N to the P to the U condition (Condition linear trend: F(1,49)=281.3, p<.0001). The linear effect was less steep in the citalopram compared to the placebo group (Treatment Group x Session x Condition x Treatment interaction linear trend: (F(1,49)=4.1, p<.05), reflecting a relative decrease in subjective anxiety in the citalopram group from the N to the P to the U condition. Subsequent analyses showed a trend for less anxiety in the citalopram group compared to placebo in the unpredictable (F(1,49)=3.8, p<.06) but not the predictable : (F(1,49)=.1, ns) condition.

State anxiety

The state anxiety scores (Table 3) were analyzed with a Treatment Group (placebo, citalopram) x Sex (males, females) x Session (pre-treatment, post-treatment) x Time (baseline, after 1st block, after 2nd block) ANOVA. There was only a significant main effect of time (F(2,98)=8.0, p<.00001) and a Time quadratic trend (F(1,49)=15.8, p<.00001), reflecting a slight increase in anxiety after the 1st block. State anxiety after the 1st block was larger than at baseline (F(1,52)=15.2, p<.00001) or after the 2nd block (F(1,52)=15.0, p<.00001).

Discussion

The present study used a psychopharmacological probe, citalopram, to establish a differentiation between short-duration and long-duration aversive states modeled by cued fear and contextual anxiety. The results show that a 2-week treatment with citalopram reduced contextual anxiety without affecting cued fear. The impetus for seeking a psychopharmacological differentiation between these two states comes from fear-potentiated startle studies in rodents (Walker and Davis in press) and from empirical works in our laboratory. We have previously reported, using the same experimental design implemented in the present study, that cued fear is not affected by the benzodiazepine alprazolam (Grillon et al. 2006) and does not differ between healthy subjects and patients with panic disorder (Grillon et al. 2008). On the other hand, contextual anxiety is reduced by alprazolam (Grillon et al. 2006) and is elevated in patients with panic disorder, relative to healthy subjects (Grillon et al. in press). These results are consistent with the hypothesis that long-duration aversive states model anticipatory anxiety in anxious patients.

There is evidence that SSRIs are efficacious in a number of anxiety disorders, but their mechanism of action is still largely unknown (Nutt et al. 1999). The present results suggest that citalopram does not affect CeA-mediated fear responses to a discrete threat cue. This is consistent with the clinical observation that fear-related symptoms such as specific phobias, respond less well to treatments such as citalopram than other pathological anxiety states, such as the anticipatory anxiety in panic disorder (Baldwin et al. 2005). This is also consistent with pre-clinical evidence in rodents showing that the serotoninergic system is not implicated in the expression of cued fear-potentiated startle (Davis et al. 1988; Kehne et al. 1988). The findings that citalopram reduced contextual anxiety is in line with the hypothesis that SSRIs have an ‘anti-worry’ effect in anxious patients (Andrews et al. 1998). SSRIs also affect sustained anxiety states in animal models. In rodents, exposure to a cat litter, a model of sustained anxiety (Blanchard et al. 2001), produces a long-lasting increase in baseline startle that is suppressed by chronic sertraline (Matar et al. 2006). It is too early to speculate on the mechanisms responsible for the reduction of context-potentiated startle by citalopram. However, given the role of the stress hormone corticotrophin releasing factor (CRH) in the BNST in the mediation of the long-duration potentiation of startle (Walker and Davis in press), it is possible that reduction in persistent anxious states is dependent on an interaction between serotonin and CRH. Indeed, there is evidence of interactions between CRF and SSRIs. Fluoxetine reduce CRF in depressed patients (De Bellis et al. 1993) and tryptophan depletion, which reduces brain serotonin, has the opposite effect (Tyrka et al. 2004).

While citalopram reduced context-potentiation of startle, this effect reached significance only in the predictable condition. One potential interpretation of these findings is that citalopram promotes the processing of or reliance on safety cues, thereby, reducing the overgeneralization of fear. Indeed, the nature of contextual anxiety in the predictable and unpredictable conditions substantially differ. In the predictable condition, context is measured during periods of clearly identifiable safety; this reflects the fact that shock could be administered only during the threat cues; their absence clearly signals periods of safety (Seligman and Binik 1977). Hence, contextual anxiety in the predictable condition reflects generalization of fear from the cue to the context. This in not the case, in contrast, for contextual anxiety in the unpredictable condition, where contexts involves the actual risk of shocks, which can occur at any time. Therefore, citalopram's greater efficacy on contextual anxiety in the predictable, as opposed to the unpredictable, condition may reflect a reduction in the generalization of fear. This hypothesis is consistent with results of a knock out mouse model of anxiety involving the serotonin system (Klemenhagen et al. 2006). Mice lacking the serotonin 1A receptor (5-HT1AR KO) exhibit increased anxiety-like behaviors (Parks et al. 1998). These mice show a strong tendency to overgeneralize fear, especially under conditions of competing safety and threat cues (Klemenhagen et al. 2006). One possibility is that SSRIs make safety cues more salient, reducing overgeneralization of fear. Of note, reminiscent of the present lack of effect of citalopram on cued fear, mice lacking the serotonin 1A receptor do not show abnormal responses to a discrete conditioned fear cue (Klemenhagen et al. 2006).

SSRIs are used to treat a wide range of mood and anxiety disorders, including panic disorder. While the present study showed an effect of citalopram on contextual anxiety in the predictable condition but not in the unpredictable condition, we recently showed that panic disorder patients have exaggerated contextual anxiety in the unpredictable condition, but not in the predictable condition (Grillon et al 2008). It is premature to speculate on the implication of these apparent inconsistencies. We used milder aversive stimuli in our panic disorder study (e.g., loud sounds, screams) compared to the present study, which used shocks. Because different types of aversive stimuli generate different levels of contextual anxiety (Grillon et al 2004), the two studies may not be directly comparable. In addition, the effect of SSRIs may be quite different in anxious patients compared to healthy individuals as suggested by studies with 5-HT1AR KO mice. These studies demonstrated different behavioral and anxiety responses to SSRIs in 5-HT1AR KO and control mice (Malagie et al 2002; Klemenhagen et al. 2006). Future studies should investigate whether SSRIs affect contextual anxiety in anxious patients (e.g., panic disorder), and whether this effect, if any, is associated with improvement in anticipatory anxiety symptoms.

The present results need to be interpreted in the context of its limitations. One limitation was that only a two-week treatment was implemented. Although this is longer exposure than in prior studies among healthy volunteers (Harmer et al. 2006; Harmer et al. 2004), data in patients suggest that clinical benefits can continue to accrue over many weeks. Hence, longer exposures would be advantageous, though ethical questions could be raised on the appropriateness of exposing healthy individuals to SSRIs for longer than two weeks. Regardless, it should be noted that, consistent with the clinical observation that SSRIs can be anxiogenic in patients following initial treatment, we reported an increase in cued fear and contextual anxiety in a similar study using a single 20-mg dose of citalopram (Grillon et al. 2007). Taken together, the current and prior results demonstrate a dissociation between the acute and chronic effects of SSRIs on two types of experimentally induced anxiety. As such, these data demonstrate that our model is sensitive to the anxiogenic and anxiolytic effects of citalopram. Given the parallels between these findings and effects in clinical anxiety, one might suspect longer exposures in healthy subjects, as in patients, would produce steadily increasing anxiolytic effects on contextual anxiety; such sustained exposure also might potentially produce an effect on cued fear. Regardless, one main conclusion emerging from the current work is that a dissociable effect of chronic citalopram manifests on cued fear and contextual anxiety.

A second limitation was that the citalopram group had larger contextual anxiety compared to the placebo group (predictable and unpredictable context; F(1,51)=12.09, p<.001 and F(1,51)=6.09, p<.01). The impact of these differences on the results is unclear. Greater pre-treatment contextual anxiety (i.e., greater ITI startle) in the predictable context could potentially affect the magnitude of fear-potentiated startle to the threat cue because of a ceiling effect. However, this is unlikely as startle magnitude of much larger magnitude compared to fear-potentiated startle were obtained during the initial habituation trials (data not show). The group difference in contextual anxiety also raises the question as to whether the decline in contextual anxiety in the predictable condition from pre-treatment to post-treatment in the citalopram group is due to regression to the mean rather than to a genuine reduction in anxiety. Although this is a possibility to consider, it is unclear why the citalopram group also did not experience a similar regression to the mean in the unpredictable context.

A final limitation was that subjective anxiety data did not fully match the startle findings. The subjective anxiety suggested that the anxiolytic effect of citalopram on contextual anxiety was stronger in the unpredictable condition, while the startle data suggest a stronger effect in the predictable condition. However, it is important to note that the startle data and the subjective reports converged in indicating an effect of citalopram on contextual anxiety but not on cued fear. In addition, reports of dissociation between objective measures and subjective reports are frequent. A single dose of citalopram increases the recognition of fearful faces without affecting subjective mood (Harmer et al. 2003b). Citalopram also affects physiological responses to emotional stimuli in the absence of conscious changes in subjective feelings (Kemp et al. 2004). We also reported that acute citalopram increased fear-potentiated startle to a threat cue and context-potentiated startle without affecting subjective fear/anxiety (Grillon et al. 2007). The most likely reason for the differential effect of citalopram on physiological and subjective reports in the present study is that startle was used to probe anxiety online, whereas the subjective anxiety measures were retrospective. The passage of time may have obscured subtle differences in responding because of the complexity of the design.

To summarize, consistent with the hypothesis of different neural mechanisms underlying fear-potentiated startle to a threat cue and context-potentiated startle, we found that two weeks of treatment with citalopram reduced the latter response without affecting the former. Using the same experiment, we recently reported that patients with panic disorder (who are characterized by substantial levels of anticipatory anxiety) show normal fear-potentiated startle but elevated context-potentiated startle. These findings confirm the relevance of contextual anxiety to clinical anxiety and have implications for psychopharmacological treatments. The demonstration of a pharmacological dissociation between fear and anxiety may encourage future efforts to develop novel treatments that target clinical manifestations of dissociable perturbations in these systems. For example, such treatments might relate to clinically-relevant perturbations in so-called “fear-related” systems, as manifest in simple phobia, or “anxiety-related” systems, as manifest in anticipatory anxiety in panic disorder. There is growing evidence from animal models for the role of CRH on the BNST in mediating sustained startle potentiated responses, and for interactions between serotonin and CRF in alleviating sustained anxiety states. Context-potentiated startle is therefore a useful integrative model to gain a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying the therapeutic effects of SSRIs and to detect the efficacy of potential anxiolytic compounds.

Table 2.

Mean (SEM) retrospective rating of anxiety during the cue and ITI across treatments and conditions


Neutral
Predictable
Unpredictable

Cue
ITI
Cue
ITI
Cue
ITI
Placebo pre-treatment 1.9 (.2) 1.8 (.2) 5.4 (.4) 3.6 (.4) 5.1 (.4) 5.4 (.4)
    post-treatment
1.4 (.1)
1.4 (.1)
5.5 (.4)
3.2 (.3)
5.5. (.4)
5.6 (.4)
Citalopram pre-treatment 1.7 (.2) 1.7 (.3) 5.9 (.4) 3.6 (.4) 5.9 (.5) 6.4 (.4)
    post-treatment 1.4 (.1) 1.4 (.1) 5.0 (.4) 2.9 (.3) 5.6 (.4) 5.4 (.4)

Acknowledgment

This research was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institutes of Mental Health

Footnotes

Financial disclosure: The authors declare that, except for income received from my primary employer, no financial support or compensation has been received from any individual or corporate entity over the past three years for research or professional service and there are no personal financial holdings that could be perceived as constituting a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Andrews W, Parker G, Barrett E. The SSRI antidepressants: exploring their ”other” possible properties. J Affect Disord. 1998;49:141–4. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(97)00203-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baldwin DS, Anderson IM, Nutt DJ, Bandelow B, Bond A, Davidson JR, den Boer JA, Fineberg NA, Knapp M, Scott J, Wittchen HU. Evidence-based guidelines for the pharmacological treatment of anxiety disorders: recommendations from the British Association for Psychopharmacology. J Psychopharmacol. 2005;19:567–96. doi: 10.1177/0269881105059253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Blanchard DC, Griebel G, Blanchard RJ. Mouse defensive behaviors: pharmacological and behavioral assays for anxiety and panic. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25:205–18. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00009-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blanchard DC, Griebel G, Blanchard RJ. The Mouse Defense Test Battery: pharmacological and behavioral assays for anxiety and panic. Eur J Pharmacol. 2003;463:97–116. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(03)01276-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Burghardt NS, Sullivan GM, McEwen BS, Gorman JM, LeDoux JE. The selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram increases fear after acute treatment but reduces fear with chronic treatment: a comparison with tianeptine. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:1171–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.02.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Davis M, Cassella JV, Kehne JH. Serotonin does not mediate anxiolytic effects of buspirone in the fear-potentiated startle paradigm: comparison with 8-OH-DPAT and ipsapirone. Psychopharmacology. 1988;94:14–20. doi: 10.1007/BF00735873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. De Bellis MD, Gold PW, Geracioti TD, Listwak SJ, Kling MA. Association of fluoxetine treatment with reductions in CSF concentrations of corticotropin-releasing hormone and arginine vasopressin in patients with major depression. The American journal of psychiatry. 1993;150:656–7. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.4.656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. First MB, Spitzer RI, Williams JBW, Gibbon M. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-V (SCID) American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  9. Gorman JM, Liebowitz MR, Fyer AJ, Goetz D, Campeas RB, Fyer MR, Davies SO, Klein DF. An open trial of fluoxetine in the treatment of panic attacks. Journal of clinical psychopharmacology. 1987;7:329–332. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Grillon C. Models and mechanisms of anxiety: evidence from startle studies. Psychopharmacology. 2008;199:421–437. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-1019-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Grillon C, Baas JMP, Pine DS, Lissek S, Lawley M, Ellis V, Levine J. The benzodiazepine alprazolam dissociates contextual fear from cued fear in humans as assessed by fear-potentiated startle. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:760–766. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Grillon C, Baas JP, Lissek S, Smith K, Milstein J. Anxious responses to predictable and unpredictable aversive events. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118:916–24. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.5.916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Grillon C, Levenson J, Pine DS. A single dose of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram exacerbates anxiety in humans: a fear-potentiated startle study. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007;32:225–231. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Grillon C, Lissek S, Rabin S, McDowell D, Dvir S, Pine DS. Increased anxiety during anticipation of unpredictable but not predictable aversive stimuli as a psychophysiologic marker of panic disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:898–904. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07101581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hammack SE, Richey KJ, Watkins LR, Maier SF. Chemical lesion of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis blocks the behavioral consequences of uncontrollable stress. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118:443–8. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.2.443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Harmer CJ, Bhagwagar Z, Perrett DI, Vollm BA, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM. Acute SSRI administration affects the processing of social cues in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003a;28:148–52. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Harmer CJ, Mackay CE, Reid CB, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM. Antidepressant drug treatment modifies the neural processing of nonconscious threat cues. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:816–20. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Harmer CJ, Rogers RD, Tunbridge E, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM. Tryptophan depletion decreases the recognition of fear in female volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2003b;167:411–417. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1401-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Harmer CJ, Shelley NC, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM. Increased positive versus negative affective perception and memory in healthy volunteers following selective serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibition. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1256–63. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kehne JH, Cassella JV, Davis M. Anxiolytic effects of buspirone and gepirone in the fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Psychopharmacology. 1988;94:8–13. doi: 10.1007/BF00735872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kemp AH, Gray MA, Silberstein RB, Armstrong SM, Nathan PJ. Augmentation of serotonin enhances pleasant and suppresses unpleasant cortical electrophysiological responses to visual emotional stimuli in humans. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1084–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Klemenhagen KC, Gordon JA, David DJ, Hen R, Gross CT. Increased fear response to contextual cues in mice lacking the 5-HT1A receptor. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:101–111. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Malagié I, David DJ, Jolliet P, Hen R, Bourin M, Gardier AM. Improved efficacy of fluoxetine in increasing hippocampal 5-hydroxytryptamine outflow in 5-HT(1B) receptor knock-out mice. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2002;443:99–104. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(02)01604-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Matar MA, Cohen H, Kaplan Z, Zohar J. The effect of early poststressor intervention with sertraline on behavioral responses in an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:2610–8. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Nutt DJ, Forshall S, Bell C, Rich A, Sandford J, Nash J, Argyropoulos S. Mechanisms of action of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in the treatment of psychiatric disorders. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1999;9(Suppl 3):S81–6. doi: 10.1016/s0924-977x(99)00030-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Otto T, Poon P. Dorsal hippocampal contributions to unimodal contextual conditioning. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6603–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1056-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Parks CL, Robinson PS, Sibille E, Shenk T, Toth M. Increased anxiety of mice lacking the serotonin1A receptor. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95:10734–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.18.10734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Romano P, van Beek N, Cucchi M, Biffi S, Perna G. Anxiety sensitivity and modulation of the serotonergic system in patients with PD. J Anxiety Disord. 2004;18:423–31. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(02)00295-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Seligman MEP, Binik YM. The safety signal hypothesis. In: Davis H, Hurwitz HMB, editors. Operant-Pavlovian interactions. Hillsdale; New York: 1977. pp. 165–187. [Google Scholar]
  30. Spielberger CD. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologist Press, Consulting Psychologist Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  31. Sullivan GM, Apergis J, Bush DE, Johnson LR, Hou M, Ledoux JE. Lesions in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis disrupt corticosterone and freezing responses elicited by a contextual but not by a specific cue-conditioned fear stimulus. Neuroscience. 2004;128:7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Tyrka AR, Carpenter LL, McDougle CJ, Kirwin PD, Owens MJ, Nemeroff CB, Strong DR, Price LH. Increased cerebrospinal fluid corticotropin-releasing factor concentrations during tryptophan depletion in healthy adults. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;56:531–4. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Waddell J, Morris RW, Bouton ME. Effects of bed nucleus of the stria terminalis lesions on conditioned anxiety: Aversive conditioning with long-duration conditional stimuli and reinstatement of extinguished fear. Behavioral neuroscience. 2006;120:324–336. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.2.324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Walker DL, Davis M. Role of the extended amygdala in short-duration versus sustained fear: a tribute to Dr. Lennart Heimer. Brain Struct Funct. doi: 10.1007/s00429-008-0183-3. (in press) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Walker D, Davis M. Double dissociation between the involvement of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and the central nucleus of the amygdala in startle increases produced by conditioned versus unconditioned fear. Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:9375–9383. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-23-09375.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES