Skip to main content
Journal of General Internal Medicine logoLink to Journal of General Internal Medicine
. 2008 Dec 20;24(3):299–304. doi: 10.1007/s11606-008-0883-7

Asking the Right Questions: Views on Genetic Variation Research Among Black and White Research Participants

Jada Bussey-Jones 1,, Gail Henderson 2, Joanne Garrett 2, Mairead Moloney 2, Connie Blumenthal 2, Giselle Corbie-Smith 2
PMCID: PMC2642575  PMID: 19101773

Abstract

Background

Genetic variation research (GVR) may raise concerns about misuse of information and discrimination. Seemingly contradictory positive views about GVR have also been reported.

Objective

To dissect this inconsistency, our objectives were to: (1) explore open-ended views of GVR and (2) quantify views of and willingness to participate in GVR by race.

Design

Cross-sectional study.

Participants

801 African-American and white prior participants in a case-control genetic epidemiology study of colon cancer risks (NCCCS).

Measures

Qualitative measures evaluated responses to questions about good and bad things about GVR. Quantitative measures evaluated positive and negative perceptions, perceptions of discrimination, and likelihood of future participation by race.

Results

Open-ended queries about GVR resulted in few “negative” responses. In closed-ended questions, however, African Americans were more likely to feel that such research would: result in higher insurance (41% vs. 30%, p = 0.008), not benefit minorities (29% vs. 14%, p=<0.001), reinforce racism (32% vs. 20%, p = 0.002), and use minorities as guinea pigs (27% vs. 6%, p < 0.001). Overall, after adjustment for potential confounding factors, African-American race remained inversely associated with feeling “very positive” about GVR (46% vs. 57%, p = 0.035). In contrast, African Americans were as likely as whites to express willingness to participate in future GVR studies (46%).

Conclusions

Open-ended questions about GVR were unlikely to spontaneously generate “negative” responses. In contrast, when presented specific examples of potentially negative implications, more respondents agreed, and minorities were more likely to express concerns. This suggests that while participants appear generally positive about GVR, their inability to articulate views regarding these complex concepts may require that researchers engage lay audiences, ensure accurate understanding, and provide them with language to express concerns.

KEY WORDS: genetic variation research, discrimination, response evaluation

BACKGROUND

Researchers have increasingly focused efforts on genetic variations research (GVR), research that seeks to understand genetic differences by racial/ethnic group, often in the hopes of addressing racial and ethnic diparities. Supporters of such work suggest that it may lead to improved health for individuals and populations, proposing that a better understanding of disease risk and targeted pharmacogenomic innovations are examples of this potential.1,2 However, others express concern about the lack of clarity and consistency around the assignment of racial categories, along with fear of increased bias, discrimination, and reinforced stereotypes that may shift focus away from important social determinants of health.38 One study, in fact, demonstrated that lay audiences exposed to messages about genetics that are linked to either ‘Whites’ or ‘Blacks’ were more likely to endorse higher levels of racism, beliefs about a genetic basis for racism, and genetic discrimination.4

To date, the bulk of this debate is in the academic arena and focuses on genetic research in more general terms. Sterling et al. recently performed a systematic review of studies examining public opinions on GVR and found only one study that prompted discussion about research specifically on the relationships of race, genes, and health.9 Much less is known about the public perception of this type of investigation. While relevant cautions and concerns regarding general genetic testing and research have been elicited from researchers, minority organizations, and the public,1015 seemingly contradictory positive views about genetics research have also been reported.10,15

To dissect this inconsistency, we examined views of GVR among African-American and white participants in a genetic epidemiology study, using a mixed method approach – with both qualitative and quantitative components. We present findings from this study that evaluate qualitative open-ended responses to queries of the “good” and “bad” of GVR; quantify participant positive and negative views of and willingness to participate in GVR by race; and offer insight and implications for public discourse about this complex and controversial topic.

METHODS

Study Sample

Learning About Research in North Carolina (LeARN) is a cross-sectional study of African Americans and whites who had previously participated in a case-control genetic epidemiology study of colon cancer risk factors, the North Carolina Colorectal Cancer Study (NCCCS). In NCCCS, cases had a diagnosis of invasive colorectal cancer. Controls were drawn from DMV records and Medicare beneficiary lists. Race was initially obtained from cancer registry records and DMV or HCFA files and further confirmed by self-identification during the interview. When there were conflicting data, the participants’ self-identified race was used. We used the NCCCS database to identify participants interested in participating in other studies. Those who expressed interest were contacted to take part in the LeARN telephone interviews. They were eligible to participate if they met the following criteria (1) self-reported race of African American or non-Latino white, (2) completed the entire interview required of NCCCS, (3) agreed to be contacted about future studies, (4) lived in the state of North Carolina at the time of the LeARN study, and (5) had sufficient cognitive functioning, as assessed by the interviewer, to allow successful completion of the telephone interview.

Recruitment Procedures and Data Collection

Potential participants were mailed a letter by the NCCCS investigators that introduced the LeARN study, described the telephone interview, and alerted them to expect a follow-up telephone call. We contracted with the professional survey group, FGI, Inc., to conduct the telephone interviews using Computer Assisted Telephone Interviewing (CATI) methods. Potential participants were contacted on an average of 4 months after completing the NCCCS interview. LeARN interviewers were racially diverse and trained to address barriers to research participation. We used a follow-up protocol with at least ten attempts made at different times of day and different days of the week to maximize response rates.

Participants provided verbal consent to participate in the telephone interview. All interviews were audio-taped and transcribed for content analysis of the open-ended questions. The final LeARN participant questionnaire took on average 45 min to complete. After completion of the LeARN interview, each participant was mailed an incentive of $25. All procedures were approved by both the UNC and Emory University Institutional Review Boards.

Quantitative Analysis

Views of GVR were assessed by examining the Likert responses to two questions, “How positive or negative do you feel about genetic research that looks into why different racial groups get different diseases?” and “How likely would you be to take part in such a study in the future?” Because a large majority of participants answered positively to both these questions, we dichotomized each response at the highest category, i.e., “Very positive” versus the other responses for the first question, and “Very likely” compared to the other responses for the second question. We examined racial and other participant characteristics for differences in responses to these two questions using Pearson’s chi-square tests. In addition, participants were given a list of five potential concerns about minority participation in genetic research and were asked whether they agreed or disagreed with these statements. We used Pearson’s chi-square tests to examine whether these responses differed by race. We were particularly interested in racial differences in responses to views of GVR. To examine this in more depth, we used logistic regression models to see if any observed differences remained after adjustment for potential confounding factors, including concerns about minority participation. We developed a separate model for each of the two outcomes. Each initial model included race as our main study factor and the other participant characteristics as potential confounding variables (case status, age, gender, education, income, how religious, health status, family history of cancer, how much they had heard about GVR, and concerns about minority participation). We used a “change in effect” elimination strategy to reduce the models. We considered dropping potential confounders when removing them from the model caused little change (<10%) in the estimate of the relationship between race and each outcome. The estimates for the differences between African Americans and whites were reported as adjusted percents, based on the beta coefficients from the final logistic regression models. We chose to report adjusted percents rather than odds ratios because of the common occurrence of both outcomes (54% and 45%); odds ratios would over-estimate the size of the effect.

Qualitative Analysis

Qualitative analysis was performed to more fully understand the views and responses of participants. Since our original sample was too large for an in-depth qualitative analysis, a smaller sub-sample (N = 194) was created. We included the first 51 African-American cases and 46 African-American controls. We then matched participants by case status, age, education, and sex to an equal number of white cases and controls. Most of the 97 African-American participants had only one clear match among the white participants. In cases where there was more than one potential match, a match was randomly chosen.

For this sub-sample of 194, we analyzed qualitatively responses to four open-ended questions, (1) “What are the good things for yourself and your family about participating in genetic research that looks at why different racial groups get different diseases;” (2) “what are the good things for society about participating in genetic research that looks at why different racial groups get different diseases?” These two questions were then repeated for a list of the “bad things.” Codes were initially developed by the team of LeARN investigators, applied, and validated through an iterative process. Two coders applied the codes to all responses to open-ended questions; periodic checks were undertaken to assure uniformity of application. Four LeARN investigators worked to further refine and validate codes for the four questions used in this paper. Additionally, there was a final review of each of the transcripts by race to determine if African Americans and whites articulated their responses differently even when coded similarly.

RESULTS

Participant Characteristics

The overall response rate for the LeARN study was 73%. This represents the percent of NCCCS participants who were eligible for LeARN and responded to our survey. There were 801 respondents in the sample, of which 153 were African Americans (19%). The mean age was 64.3 years (SD = 9.90). Cases from NCCCS made up 55% of the LeARN sample, 57% were male, and most had at least a high school education (Table 1). African Americans and whites were comparable in age and reported similar perceived health status. African Americans, however, were somewhat more likely than whites to have been cases in the NCCCS study (55% of African Americans vs. 43% of whites; p = 0.008), to be female (51% vs. 41%, p = 0.026), to report less education (31% vs. 11% had less than high school), to have lower income (48% vs. 15% made less than $20,000), to be more religious (64% vs. 51% were very religious), and to have heard more about GVR (29% vs. 16% heard “a lot”). A large majority of both African Americans and whites felt positive about GVR (84% and 93%), although African Americans were less likely to feel very positive (43% vs. 56%). Both groups expressed that they would be very likely or somewhat likely to participate in future studies (82% and 87%), and these responses did not differ by race.

Table 1.

Participant Characteristics, Overall and by African American (AA) vs. White Presented as Percents*

Participant Characteristic % Overall (n = 801) % AA (n = 153) %White (n = 648) p value
Case status in NCCCS
 Case 45 55 43 0.008
 Control 55 45 57
Age
 40 to 54 years 20 21 20 0.662
 55 to 69 years 42 44 42
 70 to 85 years 38 35 38
Gender
 Male 57 49 59 0.026
 Female 43 51 41
Education
 Less than high school 15 31 11 <0.001
 High school graduate 25 28 24
 Technical or some college 32 26 33
 College degree, graduate school 28 15 32
Income
 Less than $20,000 21 48 15 <0.001
 $20,000 to $40,000 27 26 26
 More than $40,000 52 26 59
How religious
 Not religious 5 4 5 0.001
 Somewhat religious 42 32 44
 Very religious 53 64 51
 Perceived health status
 Excellent 13 13 13 0.113
 Very good 37 30 39
 Good 30 32 29
 Fair 14 20 13
 Poor 6 5 6
Cancer Hx
 Case, family hx (relative) 6 8 6 0.027
Case, no family hx (relative) 39 47 37
 Control, family hx (self or relative) 18 11 20
 Control, no family hx (self or relative) 37 34 37
How much heard about genetic research and race
 A lot 18 29 16 0.001
 Little 71 59 73
 None 11 12 11
How positive about genetic research about racial differences
 Very positive 54 43 56 <0.001
 Positive 37 41 37
 Neutral or negative 9 16 7
How likely to take part in genetic research about racial differences
 Very likely 45 44 45 0.284
 Somewhat likely 41 38 42
 Neutral or unlikely 14 18 13

*Percents are column percents, e.g., 55% of African Americans are cases and 43% of whites are cases

p value for chi-square test comparing each characteristic to African Americans vs. whites

Qualitative Analysis

In the qualitative analysis, several themes emerged. Respondents were positive about the potential for GVR to contribute to knowledge, determine causes, prevent, and find the cures for diseases. Some felt that GVR might help determine differential group risk and identify culturally/racially specific lifestyle risks. One said, “Different groups have different lifestyles, they eat different, they do things different, and, of course, some ethnic groups are more prone to diseases, such as diabetes and high blood pressure.” Others discussed equity in research and felt it would allow researchers to prioritize research on certain groups to address prior inequalities. For example, “Well, it would benefit the racial groups who have not been targeted before.” “It’s like giving everyone the same starting block… Making sure that everyone has the same advantage.”

Some people discussed dispelling racial myths through research as one potential positive consequence. “A good thing would be to give you honest answers … There are certain myths and many perhaps untruths about genetic passing down. I feel very strongly that there are too many myths about it, and I believe this would expose and expel some of it.” “You create a better understanding among all people. Then what you do if the research is genuine… you eliminate the greater chance of discrimination. Because you have actual facts.”

In contrast, some respondents disagreed with the premise of race as the central issue. While the frequencies of these qualitative responses coded as “race is not the issue” did not vary by race, African Americans and whites articulated their responses differently in some cases. Whites in several cases reported that we are all the same, should be treated equally, and such distinctions should not be made. For example, “I feel like everybody’s created equal.” ‘It doesn’t make any difference the race… I know that scientists want to separate the racial groups… I don’t know.” “To me a man’s a man whether regardless of his race… I think we’re all one people even though African American, white, red, yellow, whatever.”

While African Americans expressed similar views of equality, they also offered alternatives to race as grounds for disease disparities. Examples such as money, health care, and food were offered as more relevant issues than race. One person stated, “I don’t think it has to do with race itself. I think it has to do with your access to proper foods and proper medical attention more so than race. But I don’t think it has to with the genetic makeup of those races.” One respondent stated, “Uh, I think as being African American... that if we had the resources like money and good health-care provider we wouldn’t have the genetic diseases like we would because we would know more about taking care of ourselves.” Finally, one respondent explicitly offered bias as a cause. “And there are certain diseases that African Americans get that whites don’t and it could be because of prejudice.”

Few voiced concerns about the negatives of GVR. The respondents who did raised issues of abuse of information (“If this information was used against you for employment purposes or being able to get insurance coverage”); discrimination (“Whatever racial group had a particularly strong indication of a particular problem that may be discriminated against because of it"); stereotyping (“Sometimes people are stereotyped that they’re bad because of the fact that maybe this race is affected by a certain disease more so than others”); and increased anxiety (“The only thing is knowing ahead of time that you’re going to catch something or get something terrible”).

Finally, when our sample of respondents who had participated in a study collecting genetic data were asked open-ended questions about the positives or negative about GVR, many expressed limited knowledge. When asked about the positives and negatives of GVR for themselves, their family and society, 20% reported “did not know” of any positives/negatives. Similarly, 10% of the sample said they did not understand these questions or responded in a manner that made us doubt their comprehension.

Quantitative Analysis

In contrast to our open-ended questions, in which many were unable to articulate either general responses or specific negatives, when given a list of potential implications of research that collects information about genes, most were able to articulate concerns in response to closed-ended questions. African Americans were more likely than whites to feel that such research would: result in higher insurance premiums (41% vs. 30%, p = 0.008), not benefit minorities (29% vs. 14%, p=<0.001), reinforce racism (32% vs. 20%, p = 0.002), and use minorities as guinea pigs (27% vs. 6%, p < 0.001) (Table 2). African-American controls were the most likely to express concerns about minorities’ participation in genetic research when compared to African-American cases, white cases, and white controls.

Table 2.

Concerns About Minority Participation in Genetic Research by Race

  African American (n-153) White (n = 648) P value*
% Discrimination against minorities 33 29 0.277
% Higher insurance to minorities 41 30 0.008
% Minorities less likely to benefit 29 14 <0.001
% Research could reinforce racism 32 20 0.002
% Minorities used as guinea pigs 27 6 <0.001

*P value for chi-square test comparing race (African Americans vs. whites) to each concern about minority participation question

Overall, African Americans were less likely than whites to feel very positive about GVR (43% vs. 56%; p = 0.003) (Table 3). Other participant characteristics associated with feeling very positive about GVR were older age, male gender, more education, higher income, not being religious, better perceived health status, having heard more about GVR, and disagreeing that minorities are used as guinea pigs. When we included these other factors in a logistic regression model, they had a neglible effect on the relationship between race and feeling very positive. Even after adjustment, African-American race remained inversely associated with feeling “very positive” about genetic research (46% AA vs. 57% white, p = 0.035). In contrast, in both the bivariate analysis and in the logistic regression model adjusting for potential confounding variables, African Americans were as likely as whites to express willingness to participate in future studies “that look at why different racial groups get different diseases” (45% for both groups in the bivariate analysis; 46% for both groups in the logistic regression model).

Table 3.

Bivariate Comparisons of Participant Characteristics and Percent “Very Positive” About GVR, and Percent “Very Likely” to Participate in Future GVR Studies

Participant characteristic % Very positive p value* % very likely p value*
About GVR To participate in GVR
Race
 African American 43 0.003 45 0.942
 White 56 45
Case status in NCCCS
 Case 51 0.171 41 0.068
 Control 56 48
Age
 40 to 54 42 0.006 41 0.142
 55 to 69 60 49
 70 to 85 58 42
Gender
 Male 57 0.037 48 0.043
 Female 50 41
Education
 Less than high school 42 <0.001 40 0.065
 High school grad 48 41
 Technical or some college 53 43
 College degree, graduate school 65 52
Income
 Less than $20,000 48 0.009 36 0.014
 $20,000 to $40,000 50 50
 More than $40,000 61 49
How religious
 Not religious 65 0.028 63 0.002
 Somewhat religious 49 38
 Very religious 58 49
Perceived health status
 Excellent 67 0.028 54 0.105
 Very good 56 48
 Good 49 39
 Fair 49 44
 Poor 48 40
Cancer Hx
 Case, family hx (relative) 43 0.325 37 0.223
 Case, no family hx (relative) 52 42
 Control, family hx (self or relative) 55 50
 Control, no family hx (self or relative) 56 47
How much heard about genetic research and race (GVR)
 A lot 65 <0.001 62 <0.001
 Little 53 43
 None 37 29
Discrimination against minorities
 Agree 55 0.555 43 0.429
 Disagree 53 46
Higher insurance to minorities
 Agree 54 0.966 43 0.515
 Disagree 54 46
Minorities less likely to benefit
 Agree 55 0.687 40 0.188
 Disagree 53 46
Research could reinforce racism
 Agree 53 0.848 44 0.876
 Disagree 54 45
Minorities used as guinea pigs
 Agree 38 0.003 36 0.110
 Disagree 56 46

*P value for chi-square tests comparing each characteristic to each outcome

DISCUSSION

Our research indicates that the majority of participants in a genetic epidemiology study of colorectal cancer in North Carolina were generally positive about GVR. In fact, when asked open-ended questions about their views of GVR, participants were unlikely to spontaneously generate “negative” responses. Additionally, despite having previously participated in genetic research that examined differences in genetic and other risk factors by race (NCCCS), a surprising number reported having no knowledge of GVR or did not understand the question. In contrast, when presented with specific examples of potentially negative implications of such research, more respondents endorsed these views, and minorities were significantly more likely to express concerns.

Broad views on GVR among a diverse study sample should not be unexpected given the complexity of this material. Our sample of prior participants in a genetic study expressed views that demonstrate both remarkable insight as well as limited understanding in some cases. Participants were often affirmative and optimistic on the one hand yet guarded on the other. These differing responses seem inconsistent, but may instead characterize multifaceted and emerging views on this challenging topic. The inability to freely articulate responses to open-ended queries, even among those that have previously participated in genetic research, points to the difficulty lay audiences may have communicating the concepts and language of genetic research. The complexity of genetic concepts for lay audiences has been corroborated in prior survey research that found that very few respondents (11%) considered themselves “very informed” about scientific discoveries. In this survey, 48% had not heard of the Human Genome Project.16 Conversely, other researchers have demonstrated that the public is capable of articulating complex views of genetic research irrespective of advanced education or coursework.10,17 Importantly, our work suggests that the content of questions and method of inquiry may also play a role in the responses generated.

In addition to the complexity of the subject matter by itself, wide-ranging individual perspectives—shaped by experiences, media, and other cultural influences—have also been reported. Prior research suggests that the public is positive about the potential of genetic research to contribute to improved health.10,11,15 Our work parallels prior findings that these generally optimistic public perceptions are often multifaceted, nuanced, and may not be equally expressed by all groups.10 Earlier studies of lay perceptions of genetic research have demonstrated increased concerns among minorities regarding potential loss of privacy, genetic discrimination, and potentially undesirable or dangerous genetic research.12,18 African Americans’ negative views are often considered in the context of a historical legacy of discrimination often based on the assertion of genetic inferiority as well as government-funded research, such as the Tuskegee study.19 Even so, African Americans and whites generally share similar optimism regarding the promise of genetic research.10,15,18,20

Limitations

This study does have limitations that should be noted. First, while the overall NCCCS sample is large, it still represents one state; regional variations may limit generalizability. Next, we chose to examine African-American and white views only. It is likely that other groups may have differing concerns and views. This sample represents the opinions of subjects that had previously participated in genetic research. Their willingness to participate in at least two research projects (NCCCS and LeARN) suggests an openness to genetic research that, perhaps, would exceed that of the general population. Additionally, while the LeARN response rate was quite good (73%), there are biases inherent in the sample that further limit its generalizability. The response rate differed by race: 65% of African Americans participated compared to 75% of whites. It is likely that African Americans who agreed to participate in LeARN were more likely to feel positive toward genetic research then those who did not agree to participate. Differences between African Americans and whites about how positive they felt about genetic research conceivably could have been larger than we observed had we had been able to recruit both races equally. Even so, the difference between these groups in LeARN was still statistically significant.

Implicatons/Future Research

Despite these limitations, a number of relevant themes and information emerged that may have implications and add direction to future work in this area. The aforementioned discussions of genetic research in general terms has generated complex opinions from lay audiences.20,21 As greater attention is focused on population-specific genetic variation research, additional questions will emerge about the application and implications of the results of this work. Given historical abuses18 and concerns about collective risks, such as disease associations, stigmatization, and discrimination for socially identifiable groups,22 explorations of lay perceptions of population-specific genetic variation are likely to heighten this complexity. As funding and eventually treatment and clinical practice recommendations are considered, assessing the public’s and particularly minority groups’ concerns, fears, and perceptions of GVR is critically important.

Given the paucity of literature examining lay perceptions of GVR, this work, demonstrating wide-ranging views within one diverse study population, begins to lay the foundation for further investigation of public perceptions. Importantly, it suggests that ongoing queries of opinions of GVR are likely to generate diverse, occasionally conflicting views that should be expected, accounted for, and explored. To that end, unidimensional research models may become increasingly inadequate. Rather, research models should utilize a range of approaches and be communicated in a variety of forums. Approaches that combine quantitative and qualitative responses can be expected to enhance researchers’ understanding of public opinion regarding risks and benefits of GVR — the former allowing a deeper canvasing of public perceptions in their own words and the latter providing them with the language to express their concerns.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the NIH-NHGRI grant 1-R01-HG002830. This project was also supported by grant no. P50HG004488 from the National Human Genome Research Institute.

Conflict of Interest None disclosed.

Contributor Information

Jada Bussey-Jones, Phone: +1-404-7781610, FAX: +1-404-7781601, Email: jcbusse@emory.edu

Gail Henderson, Email: gail_henderson@med.unc.edu.

Joanne Garrett, Email: joanne_garrett@med.unc.edu.

Mairead Moloney, moloney@email.unc.edu.

Connie Blumenthal, Email: connie_blumenthal@unc.edu.

Giselle Corbie-Smith, Email: gcorbie@med.unc.edu.

References

  • 1.Tate S, Goldstein D. Will tomorrow’s medicines work for everyone? Nature Genetics Supplement. 2004;36(11):534–42. November [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 2.Bloche G. Race-Based Therapeutics. N Engl J Med. 2004;351(20):2035–7. November 11 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 3.Bamshad M, Wooding S, Salisbury B, Stephens C. Deconstructuring the relationship between genetics and race. Nat Rev, Genet. 2004;5:596–609. August. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 4.Condit C, Parrott R, Bates B, Bevan J, Achter P. Exploration of the impact of messages about genes and race on lay attitudes. Clin Genet. 2004;66:402–8. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 5.Foster M, Sharp R. Beyond race: towards a whole-genome perspetive on human populations and genetic variation. Nat Rev, Genet. 2004;5:790–6. October [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 6.Goodman A. Why Genes Don’t Count (for Racial Differences in Health). Am J Public Health. November 2000;90(11):1699–702. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 7.Shields A, Fortun M, Hammonds E, et al. The Use of Race Variables in Genetic Studies of Complex Traits and the Goal of Reducing Health Disparities. Am Psychol. 2005;60(1):77–103. January [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 8.Sankar P, Cho MK, Condit CM, et al. Genetic research and health disparities. JAMA. 2004;291(24):2985–9. Jun 23 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 9.Sterling R, Henderson GE, Corbie-Smith G. Public willingness to participate in and public opinions about genetic variation research: a review of the literature. Am J Public Health. 2006;96(11):1971–8. Nov [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 10.Bates BR, Lynch JA, Bevan JL, Condit CM. Warranted concerns, warranted outlooks: a focus group study of public understandings of genetic research. Soc Sci Med. 2005;60(2):331–44. Jan [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 11.Dickson D. Public views genetic research cautiously. Nat Med. 2001;7(4):391. Apr [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 12.Furr LA. Perceptions of genetics research as harmful to society: differences among samples of African-Americans and European-Americans.[see comment]. Genetic Testing. 2002;6(1):25–30. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 13.Haddow G, Laurie G, Cunningham-Burley S, Hunter KG. Tackling community concerns about commercialisation and genetic research: a modest interdisciplinary proposal. Soc Sci Med. 2007;64(2):272–82. Jan [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 14.Lassen J, Jamison A. Genetic technologies meet the public: the discourses of concern. Science Technology & Human Values. 2006;31(1):8–28. Jan [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 15.Schulz A, Caldwell C, Foster S. What are they going to do with the information? Latino/Latina and African American Perspectives on the Human Genome Project. Health Educ Behav. 2003;30(2):151–69. April [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 16.Siang S. Americans Concernced About Ethics, Morality of Scientific Research, Survey Shows. Journal of the National Cancer Institute. 2001;93(24):1841. December 19 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 17.Henderson BJ, Maguire BT. Three lay models of disease inheritance. Soc Sci Med. 2000;50:293–301. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 18.Achter P, Parrott R, Silk K. African Americans’ opinions about human-genetics research. Polit Life Sci. 2004;23(1):60–6. Mar [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 19.Corbie-Smith G. The continuing legacy of the Tuskegee Syphilis Study: considerations for clinical investigation.[see comment]. Am J Medical Sciences. 1999;317(1):5–8. Jan [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 20.Bevan J, Lynch J, Dubriwny T, et al. Informed lay preferences for delivery of racially varied pharmacogenomics. Genet Med. 2003;5(5):393–9. Sept/Oct [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 21.Condit C. How the public understands genetics Non-deterministic and non-discrimanatory interpretations of the "blueprint" metaphor. Pub Understand Sci. 1999;8:169–80. [DOI]
  • 22.Foster M, Sharp R, Freeman W, Chino M, Bernsten D, Carter T. The Role of Community Review in Evaluating the Risks of Human Genetic Variation Research. Am J Hum Genet. 1999;64:1719–27. May 4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Articles from Journal of General Internal Medicine are provided here courtesy of Society of General Internal Medicine

RESOURCES