Abstract
The ability to form associations between choice alternatives and their contingent outcomes is an important aspect of learning that may be sensitive to hippocampal dysfunction in memory disorders of aging such as amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCIa), or early Alzheimer Disease. In this preliminary study we examined brain activation using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in twelve healthy elderly participants and nine patients with MCIa during an associative learning task. Using a high-field 3.0 Tesla MRI scanner, we examined the dynamic neural response during associative learning over trials. The slope of signal attenuation associated with learning was analyzed for differences between groups within an a-priori defined hippocampal region. Results indicated dynamic signal attenuation associated with learning in the healthy elderly sample, but not in MCIa. The absence of an associative learning effect in the MCIa sample reaffirms an important link between the learning difficulties that are commonly encountered in MCIa and the mesial temporal region.
The hippocampal region, including hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, and nearby brain structures, is affected early in the course of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Jack et al., 2000; Jack et al., 1998; Jack et al., 1999). Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCIa) is a putative prodromal syndrome to AD in which learning and memory related cognitive abilities are the predominant impairment, but the patient has not yet lost the functional capacity to warrant a diagnosis of dementia (Petersen, 2004; Winblad et al., 2004). Characterizing the hippocampus in MCIa may yield important new ways to predict whether a patient will eventually develop AD (Grundman et al., 2003; Muller et al., 2006).
Quantitative structural MRI studies indicate that MCI patients exhibit volume loss in the hippocampal and other mesial temporal lobe structures (MTL) (Dickerson et al., 2001), but volumetric studies do not provide a means to make direct inferences about the functionality of this region. Functional MRI (fMRI) may be useful in this regard and has previously been used to assess the functionality of the MTL in people with suspected hippocampal compromise such as persons with MCIa (Dickerson et al., 2005; Johnson et al., 2006; Machulda et al., 2003).
A recent fMRI study with MCI has shown an adaptation response in the hippocampal region associated with stimulus repetition (Johnson et al., 2004). The effect was found for healthy elderly persons with intact learning abilities, but not in similarly aged persons who had exhibited memory decline. That study joins a growing body of functional imaging evidence suggesting that the intact hippocampus is active during encoding of new information (LePage, Habib, & Tulving, 1998; Saykin et al., 1999; Tulving & Markowitsch, 1998; Wagner, Koutstaal, & Schacter, 1999), a function that is compromised by MCIa.
In MCI populations, some studies have found that the MTL is less responsive relative to controls (Johnson et al., 2004; Johnson et al., 2006; Machulda et al., 2003; Petrella et al., 2006; Small, Perera, DeLaPaz, Mayeux, & Stern, 1999), though other studies have shown that the MTL is paradoxically more active in MCI than controls (Dickerson et al., 2004; Dickerson et al., 2005). Several explanations are possible for the discrepant findings including variation in the type of tasks that are used, differences in difficulty level between patient and control groups, differences in the choice of statistical analysis, and differences in the severity of deficit in the patient group. For example Celone et al., (2006) found that less impaired MCI subjects showed greater than normal magnitude of activation, while more impaired MCI subjects exhibited smaller signal change compared to controls. More work is needed to clarify the cerebral activation issues in MCI if fMRI is ever to be useful as a clinical tool in this population.
To date, the majority of fMRI studies examining MCI have utilized declarative memory tasks to measure MTL signal change over discrete trials of episodic stimuli (e.g. novel and previously learned items). However, a broad body of literature in animals and humans employing tasks of associative learning has implicated the mesial temporal lobe in forming relationships between choices and their contingent outcomes. For example, non-demented elderly with hippocampal atrophy can learn a series of object discriminations as well as non-atrophied peers, but they are seriously impaired on a subsequent generalization test in which familiar information is presented in novel recombinations or novel contexts (Myers et al., 2002; Myers et al., 2003). Myers et al., (2002) implemented an eight-pair concurrent visual discrimination task. On each trial, subjects saw a pair of objects that differed in color or shape but not both (e.g. red square vs. red triangle, or blue diamond vs. green diamond). The left-right ordering of the two objects was random; one member of each pair was arbitrarily designated as rewarding. Over repeated trial blocks, subjects learned to choose the rewarded member of each pair. Non-demented elderly with and without hippocampal atrophy could learn this at equivalent speeds. This learning phase was followed by an unsignaled transfer phase, in which relevant stimulus dimensions remained the same, but irrelevant features were altered (e.g. yellow square vs. yellow triangle, or blue circle vs. green circle). Individuals with no hippocampal atrophy transferred with near-ceiling accuracy, continuing to choose the previously-rewarded stimulus feature (e.g. square beats triangle, regardless of color). Individuals with atrophy performed significantly worse on transfer. Data from a subsequent study indicate that these atrophied subjects had not simply forgotten the earlier information, but were specifically impaired at generalizing that learning to new contexts (Myers et al., 2003). The results of these studies suggest that the hippocampal region may not be required for simple stimulus-response learning, but it normally participates during this learning, helping to establish stimulus representations that are sufficient to support subsequent generalization when task demands change (Gluck & Myers, 1993, 2001). Individuals with hippocampal atrophy are therefore able to learn but, without full benefit of hippocampal-region mediation during that learning, are impaired at subsequent transfer.
The objective of this fMRI study was to build upon the prior results of Myers et al., (2002; 2003) by examining MTL signal changes during associative learning over trials in a concurrent discrimination task adapted to fMRI that was similar to the learning phase of Myers et al., (2002). Given the results of the aforementioned fMRI studies, the right hippocampus was predicted to exhibit blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal attenuation associated with learning the correct response in this associative learning paradigm. Furthermore, the slope of attenuation was expected to differentiate MCIa patients from healthy controls.
Methods
Participants
Twenty-one participants were studied and are described in Table 1.
Table 1.
Demographic and Neuropsychological Data.
MCI | Controls | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Avg | SD | Avg | SD | ||
Age | 76.1 | 5.7 | 71.8 | 5.5 | |
Education | 15.6 | 3.1 | 17.6 | 2.0 | |
Gender f/m | 5/4 | -- | 8/4 | -- | |
MMSE | 26.25 | 3.1 | * | 29.5 | 1.0 |
WRAT-III reading | 111.6 | 9.5 | 114.8 | 3.9 | |
Boston Naming Test | 52.9 | 7.2 | 57.3 | 1.7 | |
Controlled Oral Word | 33.6 | 10.4 | * | 45.8 | 8.4 |
RAVLT total | 28.0 | 5.2 | ** | 47.0 | 7.8 |
RAVLT delayed recall | 2.7 | 1.6 | ** | 8.7 | 2.4 |
BVMT-R total | 13.3 | 4.6 | ** | 26.3 | 5.2 |
BVMT-R delay | 4.3 | 2.2 | ** | 9.9 | 1.9 |
Trail Making B | 90.0 | 14.8 | * | 69.4 | 17.1 |
p<.05;
p<.001 statistical significance.
MCI = Mild Cognitive Impairment; MMSE= Mini Mental State Exam; WRAT-III = Wide Range Achievement Test-3rd Edition; RAVLT= Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; BVMT-R = Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised.
Note: All cognitive scores are raw scores except WRAT-III standard score.
MCI
Nine participants met criteria for MCI diagnosis. Our diagnostic criteria for MCI were consistent with those proposed by Winblad et al. (2004), and they included: a) presence of memory complaints by participant and/or informant, b) cognitive deficits on memory testing together with subjective report of decline over time by participant and/or information, c) intact functional status, d) cognitive and functional status not consistent with a diagnosis of dementia. All MCI participants were classified as amnestic on the basis of objective cognitive testing, though some showed deficits in other domains as well. The patients were referred from the several memory disorders clinics at a large university-based medical center. These subjects, as well as the controls, received a battery of neuropsychological tests as part of this study to verify and document the extent of any selective memory impairment. In addition, T2-weighted images acquired as part of this study were required to be rated as normal by a neuroradiologist for final inclusion of the participants.
Prior to inclusion in this study, the MCI patients were presented to a diagnostic consensus panel (consisting of experienced physicians and neuropsychologists in dementia research and clinical practice) for support of the diagnosis. Clinical information presented to the panel included medical history, laboratory tests, brain imaging findings, neurologic examination and neuropsychological test results. Exclusion criteria for this study consisted of Hachinski score (Hachinski, 1990) greater than four, abnormal brain imaging findings, prior neurological disease or neurosurgery, prior major psychiatric disorder, chronic major medical conditions such as poorly controlled diabetes, poorly controlled hypertension, or cardiac disease.
Twelve controls were also studied. The same exclusion criteria applied. Additionally, the controls were required to demonstrate neuropsychological evidence of normal cognitive function at the time of the study (no cognitive score could be more than one standard deviation below the normative mean for the subject’s age). The demographic characteristics and neuropsychological data for the controls as well as the MCIs are presented in Table 1. The controls were recruited by advertisement, physician referral, and by selected mailings to a local registry of healthy elderly who had previously expressed an interest in participating in research.
Procedures
Neuropsychology
All participants received a cognitive battery including the Mini Mental State Exam (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (Benedict, 1997), Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Spreen & Strauss, 1998), Boston Naming Test (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1987), Controlled Oral Word Association Test, Trail Making Test (Reitan & Wolfson, 1993), and an estimate of verbal intellectual ability—The Wide Range Achievement Test-3rd Edition, Reading Recognition Subtest (Jastak Associates, 1993).
Functional MRI Setup
Participants were provided with instruction on the fMRI task and underwent practice prior to scanning. They were then situated on the bed of a GE long bore 3.0 Tesla MRI scanner, equipped with a two button MR-compatible response device, and fitted with a high-resolution goggle system containing vision correction and screen resolution set at 800 × 600 from Resonance Technology (Northridge, CA, USA). Head motion was constrained by foam padding. The software Presentation (http://www.neurobs.com) was used to deliver visual stimuli and response feedback from a PC computer via the goggle system and also record the responses through the button-box connected to the serial port of the stimulus-delivery computer. A BNC cable connecting the scanner to the computer enabled Presentation to detect each and every slice acquisition from the scanner, thereby enabling precise synchrony between slice acquisition and event-related stimulus delivery.
fMRI Task
We have adapted the concurrent discrimination task described by Myers et al. (2002) for event-related fMRI. Subjects were presented with six individually presented pairs of abstract shapes that repeated six times over the course of the scan. In addition to the object pairs, a control condition was presented in which subjects saw two gray squares side by side; one of the squares was clearly marked as the correct choice; the subject’s task was merely to choose that square. Feedback was provided for both the object and control trials, as shown in Figure 1. As in Myers et al. (2002), if the subject’s response was correct, the chosen object was raised to reveal a smiley face. If incorrect, the chosen object was raised but no smiley face appeared. Target locations were counterbalanced.
Figure 1.
Concurrent visual discrimination task. (A) On each trial the subject sees a pair of objects and is asked to choose the left or right object. (B) The chosen object is raised and, if the subject’s choice was correct a smiley face is revealed; (C) when the incorrect object is chosen the object is raised to reveal no smiley face; (D) In the control task, the correct choice was always apparent; (E) The chosen object is raised to reveal the smiley face.
Trials were presented as discrete events. The stimulus onset asynchrony ranged from 5 to 18 seconds (mean of 7 seconds). The trials were jittered around the TR (+/- 0, .15 or .5 TR). The concurrent discrimination (choice) trials were a maximum of 4 seconds and ended with response selection. The feedback trial immediately ensued indicating whether the response was correct and lasted for one second duration. In the rare event a choice was not made in the 4 second time frame, the feedback trial consisted of a reminder to respond quickly. Subjects were provided with instructions and practice prior to scanning to ensure they understood the task.
Imaging Protocol
MR imaging was performed using a General Electric 3.0 Tesla SIGNA (Waukesha, WI) MRI system. A T2* gradient-echo, echo-planar imaging (EPI) pulse sequence was used to obtain BOLD fMRI data. The homogeneity of the static magnetic field (B0) in the brain was optimized using higher order shims prior to the functional trials. The BOLD EPI parameters were as follows: echo time (TE) = 30 ms; repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms; flip angle = 90 degrees; acquisition matrix = 64 × 64 voxels; field of view (FOV) = 240 mm. Thirty sagittal slices of brain were acquired within each TR. Voxel resolution was 3.75 × 3.75 × 4 mm, with a 1 mm skip between slices. 152 temporal volume images were collected, of which the initial 3 image volumes acquired during the first 6 s were discarded.
Anatomic Imaging
In the same session as the functional scans, T1 and T2 weighted anatomic images were acquired and later viewed by a neuroradiologist for structural abnormalities including gliosis and ischemic disease that might have warranted exclusion and/or clinical follow-up.
The T2 weighted scan sequence was a 2D axially prescribed fast recovery fast spin echo pulse sequence with the following parameters: slice thickness 1.7 mm with .3 mm skip; TE= 90.8; TR=9000; NEX=2; FOV = 240 mm; matrix = 256×128. Image acquisition time was approximately 5 minutes.
A 3D IR-prepped fast gradient echo pulse sequence was employed to obtain high-resolution T1-weighted structural images. In order to obtain whole-brain coverage, the imaging parameters were as follows: inversion time= 600 ms, fast gradient echo read-out with TR/TE/flip = 9 ms/1.8 ms/20 degrees; acquisition matrix = 256 × 192 ×124 (axial 256 × 192 in-plane, interpolated to 256 × 256); FOV = 240 mm; slice thickness = 1.2 mm (124 slices); +/- 16 kHz receiver bandwidth; acquisition time 7.5 minutes.
Field Mapping
Even after high-order shimming, there are residual magnetic field (B0) inhomogeneities across the brain that cause regional image distortions in echo planar images such as in the inferior prefrontal regions near the mesial temporal lobe and in the frontal and ethmoid sinuses. Image distortions were corrected by measuring 3D field maps across the brain (co-planar with the fMRI slices). This was accomplished by measuring the phase of non-EPI gradient echo images at two echo times (7 and 10 ms). The phase difference between the two echo images is proportional to the static field inhomogeneity (Jezzard & Balaban, 1995). The warp correction was performed using FSL3.2 (Jenkinson, 2003).
Image Processing
Following echo-planar image reconstruction the files were converted to Analyze7.5 file format and reoriented to the axial plane. The 4D image time-series was motion-corrected to overcome minor head movement during the scan. The field map from each subject (described above) was then applied to each image in the time series to correct for echo-planar related distortions in image space. The images were then normalized into a standard atlas space (using the T2* weighted template provided through SPM2), and then smoothed with an 8 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel.
Functional Image Analysis
All image analyses were performed in SPM2 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Analyses of the time-series data were performed on individual participants using a boxcar model convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function. The statistical model included high frequency signal filtering (high pass filter = 128 seconds) and the AR1 method of accounting for temporal autocorrelation (Kiebel & Holmes, 2004). The time series data were statistically analyzed using the general linear model (Friston et al., 1995). Each participant’s performance accuracy scores were used in a subject-specific regressor (parametric modulation) according to the following rule. Presentation of the initial pair (regardless of whether or not the answer was guessed correctly) and all incorrect responses, were weighted with a score of 1 (meaning not learned; the initial items were always model as not learned because the subject had not yet received feedback by which learning could occur). The first two subsequent correct responses were weighted with a score of 0.5. All other subsequent and consecutive correct responses were weighted with a score of 0 (denoting the correct item in the pair had been learned). This rule explicitly equates learning with gradual signal attenuation and was used to feature brain locations where pair specific adaptation or signal attenuation occurs as learning takes place over trials. First-level contrast images, characterizing individual participant’s average slope of signal change associated with learning were then entered into a second-level random effects analysis.
Slope differences between groups were tested using a two-sample t-test model. For this analysis, an anatomical mask was selected based on prior results with other memory encoding tasks (Johnson et al., 2004; Johnson et al., 2006; Trivedi et al., 2006) implicating the right side for nonverbal encoding tasks. The a priori ROI was defined once only, on a template brain in standard atlas space, and included the right hippocampus and subiculum from the amygdala to the tail. The parahippocampal and fusiform gyri were not included. The results of the two sample t-test were thus only assessed within the mask region. The purpose of the mask was to impose a more stringent hypothesis driven restriction on the number of simultaneous comparisons based on our prior work. Because of the restricted region of interest (ROI) analysis, uncorrected p-values were used (p<.01).
Voxel-based morphometry
The T1 volume was subsequently used for voxel-based morphometry (VBM) to determine whether there were volumetric differences between groups that might account for any observed fMRI differences. The VBM procedure utilized a standard approach (Good et al., 2001) that included optimized normalization to standard atlas space (and resampled at 2 mm isotropic voxels), modulation of the normalized image by the Jacobian determinants in order to preserve volume information, followed by spatial smoothing to 12 mm. The Gaussian smoothing function differed from that of the functional images because the intrinsic smoothness of the high resolution structural scans was less than that of the functional scans. A comparable two sample t-test was performed between groups within the previously defined search region to determine if atrophy was accounting for the differences in the fMRI data.
Results
Neuropsychological Results
The cognitive data are presented in Table 1 for the MCI and control groups. Age, education and Wide Range Achievement Test Reading Standard Score (an estimate of maximal verbal IQ) were not statistically different. Group differences on the Boston Naming Test raw scores approach significance (p=.052). The learning and delayed recall components of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test and the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised were all significantly different between groups, with the MCI participants performing more poorly (p<.001). Similarly, MCI subjects were significantly different on the MMSE, COWAT, and Trailmaking Test B (p<.05).
Behavioral Results
Plots of reaction time and performance accuracy are shown in Figure 2 and Table 2. The plots reveal that both groups responded progressively quicker over trials and with greater accuracy. Repeated-measures ANOVAs were implemented to determine whether performance differed by group in magnitude or slope over trials. For reaction time (RT), the effect of group was non-significant (F [1,19]= 1.78, p=.19), though qualitatively the controls exhibited shorter RTs. The effect of trials was significant (F[1,19] = 13.12, p=.002), indicating a learning effect. There was no trial by group interaction (F[1,19]=1.55, p=.182). With regard to the accuracy data, there was a significant effect of group (F[1,19]= 11.41, p=.003), with controls being more accurate. There was also a significant effect of trials (F[1,19] = 21.27, p<.001), indicating that accuracy improved with repetition and feedback. The trial by group interaction was again not significant (F[1,19] =1.51, p=.195).
Figure 2.
Behavioral results. Plots of reaction time (Top) and accuracy (Bottom), for the control (squares) and MCIa (circles) groups. See also Table 2.
Table 2.
Task accuracy over trials
Controls | MCI | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Trial | MEAN | SD | MEAN | SD |
1 | 0.63 | 0.24 | 0.44 | 0.20 |
2 | 0.72 | 0.26 | 0.41 | 0.30 |
3 | 0.74 | 0.21 | 0.56 | 0.19 |
4 | 0.88 | 0.13 | 0.59 | 0.25 |
5 | 0.85 | 0.13 | 0.61 | 0.17 |
6 | 0.83 | 0.16 | 0.78 | 0.24 |
Note: scores represent mean proportion correct.
Imaging Results
The group analysis within the hippocampal ROI indicated significant differences in the slope of learning related BOLD signal over trials. The controls exhibited a steeper negative slope than did the MCI participants. This effect was observed in two clusters located in the hippocampal body (x,y,z: 30, -28, -8; t=3.29, punc=.002) and anterior hippocampus (x,y,z: 36, -8, -26; t=3.27, punc=.002). These results are shown in Figure 3. This figure depicts the statistical parametric map indicating the two clusters within the MTL search region where the control group exhibited a steeper slope associated with learning over trials than the MCI patients. Figure 4 illustrates the shape of the average hemodynamic response early in the task and again upon the final trial for a single subject. The hemodynamic response is observed to decrease as a function of trial in the hippocampal region.
Figure 3.
Statistical parametric map of the two-sample t-test delineating group differences between Controls and MCIa. The result is overlaid on an atlas brain in MCI space. The analysis was constrained to the right hippocampus based on prior studies. The ROI search region is in white on the images, and encompasses the statistical results (hot colors). The analysis resulted in two clusters in the body and head of the right hippocampus. See the text for coordinates and statistics. The right side of the brain is on the right side of the image.
Figure 4.
Peristimulus plots indicating a decrease in the hemodynamic response as a function of trial in the mesial temporal lobe for a healthy control subject. The mean time-course of the second trial (the first trial after feedback) and final trial are shown with solid lines. The 90% confidence intervals are represented by dashed lines. The coronal image inlay shows the subject’s SPM of learning over trials overlaid on the T1 volume. The color scale is the magnitude of the t-statistic.
In order to determine whether the group results were due to atrophy in the region of interest we conducted a voxel-based morphometry analysis with the same apriori ROI and extracted the VBM atrophy values underlying the activation coordinates. This was accomplished by extracting the principal component of a small sphere (2mm radius) at the maxima in the two activation clusters and then extracting the atrophy signal from the VBM data at the same locations in the same fashion. A small homogenous ROI was selected because we were only concerned with gray matter at the site of peak activity differences. This was a conservative approach for determining gray matter values at peak activation maxima; a larger ROI may have obfuscated volumetric findings under the fMRI maxima. The extracted ROI values were then entered into a correlation analysis. In the anterior cluster there was a relationship between fMRI signal and gray matter volume (GMV; r=.55, p=.009). The posterior hippocampal cluster did not show this relationship (r=.05, p=.86), suggesting that fMRI signal and GMV were not related at this location in these data.
Discussion
In this report we have examined the dynamic neural response during associative learning in a group of MCI patients and controls. Overall, the MCI patients expectedly performed worse than controls on a number of neuropsychological measures of encoding and recall abilities, as well as on the concurrent discrimination fMRI task itself. The imaging results were consistent with our initial hypotheses indicating a difference in learning-related BOLD attenuation in the right hippocampus. The controls exhibited a significant learning effect while the MCI patients exhibited very little learning effect. The results are consistent with prior work using repeating trials paradigms (Johnson et al., 2004; Rand-Giovannetti et al., 2006) in elderly adults. Furthermore, these findings fit well with other studies of dynamic hippocampal adaptation over successive trials during learning acquisition (Dolan & Fletcher, 1999; Henson, Cansino, Herron, Robb, & Rugg, 2003; Henson & Rugg, 2003; Strange, Fletcher, Henson, Friston, & Dolan, 1999).
The earlier study by Myers et al. (2002) found that individuals with hippocampal atrophy, a risk factor for subsequent cognitive decline, performed worse than non-atrophied controls on the transfer portion of the concurrent discrimination task. The authors attributed impairment to reduced hippocampal-region processing during the initial learning stages. Since hippocampal atrophy is a risk factor for cognitive decline, including MCI, we expected that MCI patients would show reduced learning-related hippocampal signal change. Our results confirmed this prediction. The findings are consistent with the idea that the hippocampal-region is normally involved in stimulus-response learning, even if such simple learning is possible without hippocampal involvement. Similarly, in animals, classical conditioning may be spared following hippocampal lesion (e.g. Schmaltz & Theios, 1972), but electrophysiological recordings show that, when present, the hippocampus is active during this simple stimulus-response learning (Berger, Rinaldi, Weisz, & Thompson, 1983). Our results are also consistent with the idea that hippocampal-region activation during this simple learning task may facilitate subsequent transfer when task demands change; this is consistent with the idea that learning in animals and humans with hippocampal-region damage is often “inflexible” or “hyperspecific” (Eichenbaum, Otto, & Cohen, 1994; Gluck & Myers, 1993, 2001; Schacter, 1985).
Others have examined the hippocampal response during transitive inference—after initial learning of stimulus response associations have taken place. Heckers et al. (2004) found the anterior hippocampal region was active while making relational or transitive inferences on information already in declarative memory (see also Preston, Shrager, Dudukovic, & Gabrieli, 2004). A more recent study had extended that paradigm to schizophrenia and determined that transitive inference, or transfer, was impaired in patients compared to controls (Ongur et al., 2006). These studies all conducted fMRI after acquisition of information had already occurred, whereas the current study imaged the brain during the dynamic temporal course of acquisition over repetition and feedback.
Our results show that hippocampal attenuation during concurrent discrimination learning was detected in the right MTL for this visuospatial associative encoding task. We expected effects in the right-hemisphere due to the visuospatial modality of item presentation (Golby et al., 2001). Further, Henson, et al. (2003) reported a series of studies on repeating stimuli and concluded that across a variety of stimulus materials, the right MTL was more sensitive to stimulus repetitions than the left. Strange et al. (1999) also reported specific right hippocampal attenuation in healthy young adults during learning of visuospatial material.
Functional MRI studies in patient groups are prone to a number of potentially confounding factors. The coupling between neural activity and the hemodynamic response may be variably reduced in the patient group such that the hemodynamic response is no longer an accurate measure of neural activity. Another possible issue is that patients with cognitive impairment may approach the task with different strategies than controls. Examination of Figure 2 indicates that the MCI subjects exhibited a similar learning slope to controls, but indeed their accuracy was significantly lower. In this study we attempted to deal with this possible issue by incorporating the learning slope directly into the analysis. The input contrast images into the group analysis represented learning-associated signal change where learning of the correct choice in each pair was associated with signal reduction in the MTL. Another possible approach (but only useful at the group level) would have been to use the behavioral learning data as a covariate in the statistical analysis.
The samples in this preliminary study were small and thus raise the possibility of vulnerability to outliers, and simultaneously, low statistical power to detect true effects. The small number of subjects prevented more elaborate regression analyses modeling other demographic or general cognitive status in the fMRI analysis. Another limitation may have been incurred by the relatively small number of fMRI acquisitions from which event averages were calculated. More acquisition time points would likely improve detection of subtle BOLD adaptation over trials. Finally, others have pointed out that the accuracy of spatial normalization of the hippocampus to standard MNI space may be diminished in MCI and AD (Dickerson et al., 2005; Krishnan, Slavin, Tran, Doraiswamy, & Petrella, 2006) and this might have introduced spatial variability, affecting the final result.
Despite these limitations, the findings presented in this study are consistent with the hypothesis that hippocampal signal decreases with acquisition of stimulus-response associations derived through feedback. Although prior learning-based fMRI studies in MCI have modeled repetition (Johnson et al., 2004; Rand-Giovannetti et al., 2006), none have modeled feedback-based learning over trials as was done in the present report. Whether the fMRI-measured hippocampal encoding response can predict subsequent successful transfer is still an unanswered question that will be addressed by our laboratories. Additionally, further studies with MCI patients are needed to differentiate the role of the MTL during encoding of specific stimulus-response associations from its role in transfer of relevant information to new items.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by R01 AG021155 and a Merit Review grant from the Department of Veterans Affairs to SCJ. We thank Andrew Alexander, Ph.D., Howard Rowley, MD, Britta Torgerson, Justin Dunker, Michael Anderle and Ron Fisher for their assistance with this research.
References
- Benedict R. Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Berger TW, Rinaldi PC, Weisz DJ, Thompson RF. Single-unit analysis of different hippocampal cell types during classical conditioning of rabbit nictitating membrane response. J Neurophysiol. 1983;50(5):1197–1219. doi: 10.1152/jn.1983.50.5.1197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Celone KA, Calhoun VD, Dickerson BC, Atri A, Chua EF, Miller SL, et al. Alterations in memory networks in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: an independent component analysis. J Neurosci. 2006;26(40):10222–10231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2250-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson BC, Goncharova I, Sullivan MP, Forchetti C, Wilson RS, Bennett DA, et al. MRI-derived entorhinal and hippocampal atrophy in incipient and very mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2001;22(5):747–754. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(01)00271-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson BC, Salat DH, Bates JF, Atiya M, Killiany RJ, Greve DN, et al. Medial temporal lobe function and structure in mild cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol. 2004;56(1):27–35. doi: 10.1002/ana.20163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson BC, Salat DH, Greve DN, Chua EF, Rand-Giovannetti E, Rentz DM, et al. Increased hippocampal activation in mild cognitive impairment compared to normal aging and AD. Neurology. 2005;65(3):404–411. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000171450.97464.49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dolan RJ, Fletcher PF. Encoding and retrieval in human medial temporal lobes: an empirical investigation using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) Hippocampus. 1999;9(1):25–34. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:1<25::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H, Otto T, Cohen NJ. Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1994;17(3):449. [Google Scholar]
- Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatry Research. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, Poline JP, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach. Human Brain Mapping. 1995;2:189–210. [Google Scholar]
- Gluck MA, Myers CE. Hippocampal mediation of stimulus representation: a computational theory. Hippocampus. 1993;3(4):491–516. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450030410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gluck MA, Myers CE. Gateway to Memory: An Introduction to Neural Network Modeling of the Hippocampus in Learning and Memory. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Golby AJ, Poldrack RA, Brewer JB, Spencer D, Desmond JE, Aron AP, et al. Material-specific lateralization in the medial temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex during memory encoding. Brain. 2001;124(Pt 9):1841–1854. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.9.1841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage. 2001;14(1 Pt 1):21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodglass H, Kaplan E. The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. 2. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Grundman M, Jack CR, Jr, Petersen RC, Kim HT, Taylor C, Datvian M, et al. Hippocampal volume is associated with memory but not nonmemory cognitive performance in patients with mild cognitive impairment. J Mol Neurosci. 2003;20(3):241–248. doi: 10.1385/jmn:20:3:241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hachinski VC. The decline and resurgence of vascular dementia. Can-Med-Assoc-J. 1990;142(2):107–111. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heckers S, Zalesak M, Weiss AP, Ditman T, Titone D. Hippocampal activation during transitive inference in humans. Hippocampus. 2004;14(2):153–162. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henson RN, Cansino S, Herron JE, Robb WG, Rugg MD. A familiarity signal in human anterior medial temporal cortex? Hippocampus. 2003;13(2):301–304. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henson RN, Rugg MD. Neural response suppression, haemodynamic repetition effects, and behavioural priming. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41(3):263–270. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00159-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack CR, Petersen RC, Xu Y, O’Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, et al. Rates of hippocampal atrophy correlate with change in clinical status in aging and AD. Neurology. 2000;55(4):484–489. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.4.484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack CR, Petersen RC, Xu Y, O’Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, et al. Rate of medial temporal lobe atrophy in typical aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1998;51(4):993–999. doi: 10.1212/wnl.51.4.993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack CR, Petersen RC, Xu YC, O’Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, et al. Prediction of AD with MRI-based hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 1999;52(7):1397–1403. doi: 10.1212/wnl.52.7.1397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jastak Associates. Wide Range Achievement Test-Third Edition. Wilmington: Wide Range Inc; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M. Fast, automated, N-dimensional phase-unwrapping algorithm. Magn Reson Med. 2003;49(1):193–197. doi: 10.1002/mrm.10354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jezzard P, Balaban RS. Correction for geometric distortion in echo planar images from B0 field variations. Magn Reson Med. 1995;34(1):65–73. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SC, Baxter L, Susskind-Wilder L, Connor DJ, Sabbagh MN, Caselli RJ. Hippocampal adaptation to face repetition in healthy elderly and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(7):980–989. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SC, Schmitz TW, Moritz CH, Meyerand ME, Rowley HA, Alexander AL, et al. Activation of brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease: the effect of mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27(11):1604–1612. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiebel S, Holmes A. The general linear model. In: Frackowiak R, Friston K, Frith C, Dolan R, Price C, Zeki S, Ashburner J, Penny W, editors. Human Brain Function. 2. London: Elsevier; 2004. pp. 725–760. [Google Scholar]
- Krishnan S, Slavin MJ, Tran TT, Doraiswamy PM, Petrella JR. Accuracy of spatial normalization of the hippocampus: implications for fMRI research in memory disorders. Neuroimage. 2006;31(2):560–571. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LePage M, Habib R, Tulving E. Hippocampal PET activations of memory encoding and retrieval: The HIPER model. Hippocampus. 1998;8:313–322. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:4<313::AID-HIPO1>3.0.CO;2-I. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Machulda MM, Ward HA, Borowski B, Gunter JL, Cha RH, O’Brien PC, et al. Comparison of memory fMRI response among normal, MCI, and Alzheimer’s patients. Neurology. 2003;61(4):500–506. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000079052.01016.78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muller MJ, Greverus D, Weibrich C, Dellani PR, Scheurich A, Stoeter P, et al. Diagnostic utility of hippocampal size and mean diffusivity in amnestic MCI. Neurobiol Aging. 2006 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers CE, Kluger A, Golomb J, Ferris S, de Leon MJ, Schnirman G, et al. 2002Hippocampal atrophy disrupts transfer generalization in nondemented elderly J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 15\282–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers CE, Shohamy D, Gluck MA, Grossman S, Kluger A, Ferris S, et al. Dissociating hippocampal versus basal ganglia contributions to learning and transfer. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003;15(2):185–193. doi: 10.1162/089892903321208123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ongur D, Cullen TJ, Wolf DH, Rohan M, Barreira P, Zalesak M, et al. The neural basis of relational memory deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(4):356–365. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.4.356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrella JR, Krishnan S, Slavin MJ, Tran TT, Murty L, Doraiswamy PM. Mild cognitive impairment: evaluation with 4-T functional MR imaging. Radiology. 2006;240(1):177–186. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2401050739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preston AR, Shrager Y, Dudukovic NM, Gabrieli JD. Hippocampal contribution to the novel use of relational information in declarative memory. Hippocampus. 2004;14(2):148–152. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rand-Giovannetti E, Chua EF, Driscoll AE, Schacter DL, Albert MS, Sperling RA. Hippocampal and neocortical activation during repetitive encoding in older persons. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27(1):173–182. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reitan RM, Wolfson D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and clinical interpretation. 2. Tucson: Neuropsychology Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Saykin AJ, Johnson SC, Flashman LA, McAllister TW, Sparling MB, Darcey TM, et al. Functional differentiation of medial temporal and frontal regions involved in processing novel and familiar words: An fMRI study. Brain. 1999;122:1963–1971. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.10.1963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL. Multiple forms of memory in humans and animals. In: Weingberger N, McGaugh JL, Lynch G, editors. Memory Systems of the Brain: Animal and Human Cognitive Processes. New York: Guilford Press; 1985. pp. 351–379. [Google Scholar]
- Schmaltz LW, Theios J. Acquisition and extinction of a classically conditioned response in hippocampectomized rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1972;79(2):328–333. doi: 10.1037/h0032531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Small S, Perera GM, DeLaPaz R, Mayeux R, Stern Y. Differential regional dysfunction of the hippocampal formation among elderly with memory decline and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol. 1999;45(4):466–472. doi: 10.1002/1531-8249(199904)45:4<466::aid-ana8>3.0.co;2-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreen O, Strauss E. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. New York: Oxford; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Strange BA, Fletcher P, Henson R, Friston K, Dolan R. Segregating the functions of the human hippocampus. Procedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1999;96:4034–4039. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.7.4034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trivedi MA, Schmitz TW, Ries ML, Torgerson BM, Sager MA, Hermann BP, et al. Reduced hippocampal activation during episodic encoding in middle-aged individuals at genetic risk of Alzheimer’s disease: a cross-sectional study. BMC Med. 2006;4:1. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-4-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E, Markowitsch HJ. Episodic and declarative memory: role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 1998;8(3):198–204. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3<198::AID-HIPO2>3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner AD, Koutstaal W, Schacter DL. When encoding yields remembering: insights from event-related neuroimaging. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1999;354(1387):1307–1324. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1999.0481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L, Wahlund LO, et al. Mild cognitive impairment--beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]