Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 2009 Mar;48(3):341–347. doi: 10.1097/MPG.0b013e318185998f

Impact of Hepatitis C Virus Infection on Children and their Caregivers: Quality of Life, Cognitive, and Emotional Outcomes

James R Rodrigue 1, William Balistreri 2, Barbara Haber 3, Maureen M Jonas 4, Parvathi Mohan 5, Jean P Molleston 6, Karen F Murray 7, Michael R Narkewicz 8, Philip Rosenthal 9, Lesley J Smith 10, Kathleen B Schwarz 11, Patricia Robuck 12, Bruce Barton 13, Regino P González-Peralta 14
PMCID: PMC2649743  NIHMSID: NIHMS87234  PMID: 19242286

Abstract

Objective

HCV infection is associated with decreased quality of life (QOL) and neurocognitive dysfunction in adults, but little is known about its impact on children and their caregivers.

Design and Patients

We studied the QOL, behavioral, emotional, and cognitive functioning of 114 treatment-naïve children with HCV enrolled in a placebo-controlled, randomized, multi-site clinical trial evaluating peginterferon alpa-2a alone or with ribavirin.

Outcome Measures

Baseline assessment included measures of children’s QOL, cognitive functioning, behavioral adaptation, and depression. Caregivers’ QOL was also assessed.

Results

Relative to published normative data, caregivers were more likely to believe that their children’s health was poor and would likely worsen (t = 3.93, p < 0.0001), reported higher concern about their children’s health status (t = 6.63, p < 0.0001) and that this concern limited family activities (t = 2.45, p < 0.01); they also viewed their children as having more internalizing behavioral problems (t = 1.98, p < 0.05). Only 2 (2%) children had a score in the clinically depressed range. Children with HCV had worse cognitive functioning than the normative sample, but significantly better functioning than children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Caregivers’ QOL scores did not differ significantly from the normative sample, but infected mothers had lower QOL than non-infected caregivers. Caregivers were highly distressed about their children’s medical circumstances.

Conclusion

While HCV infection, in its early stages, does not lead to global impairment in QOL, cognitive, behavioral, or emotional functioning in children, it is associated with higher caregiver stress and strain on the family system, and it may be associated with some cognitive changes in children.

Keywords: pediatrics, HCV, parents, clinical trial, quality of life

INTRODUCTION

The prevalence rate of hepatitis C virus (HCV) infection in children 5 to 14 years old ranges from 0.2% to 0.4% in the United States (1). Substantially higher rates of infection are observed in children who received blood products or clotting factor concentrates prior to the implementation of universal screen precautions (23). While an asymptomatic course during childhood can be expected for most pediatric patients, some children develop progressive liver disease with fibrosis, cirrhosis, and hepatocellular carcinoma (47), and some will eventually need liver transplantation.

There is growing evidence that HCV in adults is strongly associated with decrements in quality of life (QOL) (5,6,8,9), cognitive performance (10,11), and psychological functioning (8,12). For instance, studies have noted lower health-related QOL, deficits in attention and higher executive functioning, and heightened levels of anxiety and depression in adults with HCV compared to healthy controls and patients with other types of liver disease. Despite these findings, little is known about how HCV affects children along these same parameters. Iorio et al. (13) observed increased irritability, decreased quality of life, and other psychosocial problems in children on interferon therapy. Similarly, in a small group of asymptomatic children who acquired HCV in the first year of life, Nydegger et al. (14) found that their physical and mental QOL was significantly lower than children without HCV.

The aim of this study was to assess the impact of HCV on the QOL, cognitive functioning, and psychological status of children who were treatment-naïve. We hypothesized, based on findings in the adult HCV literature, that children with HCV would have lower QOL, more problems with executive functioning, and more psychological difficulties than children without HCV. In light of the impact of childhood chronic health conditions on adult caregivers, we also assessed the QOL of the primary caregiver for these children and its relationship to that of their children. In addition we analyzed QOL scores according to HCV status of the caregiver. Finally, we examined whether QOL, cognitive, and psychological outcomes were related to HCV duration.

PATIENTS AND METHODS

Eligible study participants were children with HCV and their parent/guardian (hereafter designated as caregiver) who were enrolled in a placebo-controlled, randomized, multi-site clinical trial (PEDS-C)(15) evaluating peginterferon alpha-2a alone or with ribavirin/placebo. In brief, study eligibility criteria included HCV viremia on two tests ≥6 months apart and/or one positive test in a child with maternal-fetal transmission, chronic hepatitis consistent with HCV infection on liver biopsy within 36 months of screening, and compensated liver disease (Child-Pugh Grade A clinical classification). Exclusion criteria included any prior treatment with interferon or ribavirin, decompensated liver disease, major depression, or history of other severe illness.

At the time of study enrollment, children underwent a baseline assessment that included measures of QOL, cognitive functioning, and behavioral and psychological functioning. All measures were completed by each child’s parent or legal guardian. The Child Health Questionnaire – Parent Form 50 (CHQ-PF50)(16) measures functional status and well-being across several QOL domains, including physical functioning, role/social limitations, general health, bodily pain/discomfort, parent impact, self-esteem, mental health, general behavior, and family impact. The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF)(17) measures emotional and behavioral dysregulation, difficulties with response inhibition, working memory and the ability to quickly transition into new situations or tasks. The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)(18) measures behavioral and psychological functioning across multiple domains. The Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI)(19) assesses affective, cognitive, and behavioral symptoms of depression in children.

The child’s primary caregiver completed all questionnaires about his or her child’s functioning. Primary caregivers also completed the SF-36 Health Survey (20) to assess their own QOL. None of the caregivers refused to complete the baseline measures.

Written, informed consent was obtained from the child’s parent or legal guardian before participation in the study. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at all study sites.

Descriptive statistics were calculated to summarize the medical and sociodemographic characteristics of the sample and the outcome measures. The proportion of children with clinically significant scores on each of the outcome measures was calculated. Scores on all outcome measures were compared to normative and comparison samples using t-tests. We used the normative data, drawn from the U.S. general population, for the CHQ (n = 391) (16), BRIEF (n = 604) (17), CBCL (n = 1,753) (18), and SF-36 (n = 2,474) (20), as well as available data using children with other chronic conditions. Wilcoxon rank-sum test was used to test the difference between mothers with HCV who transmitted the virus to their enrolled child and all other caregivers. Spearman Rho correlation coefficients were calculated to assess the associations between the outcome measures and sociodemographic characteristics and HCV duration. The significance level was set at p < 0.01 for all analyses. Because we considered these analyses to be exploratory, we did not correct for multiple comparisons. We considered p-values less than 0.01 to indicate a significant association and p-values less than 0.001 to indicate strong associations.

RESULTS

Participants were 114 children who met study eligibility criteria. As noted in Table 1, the sample was predominantly white, with HCV genotype 1 and perinatal transmission. Caregivers who completed the baseline assessment were predominantly white (83%) and had a mean age of 46 ± 7 yrs. Forty-three (39%) caregivers who completed the questionnaires were biological mothers with HCV infection who transmitted the virus to their child.

Table 1.

Sociodemographic and medical characteristics of study patients

Characteristics
Total number studied 114
Sex, female 51 (45)
Age, yr 11 ± 3
Race
  White 85 (75)
  Black / African American 5 (4)
  Asian 5 (4)
  American Indian / Alaska Native 1 (1)
  More than one race 6 (5)
  Unknown 12 (11)
HCV genotype
  1 93 (82)
  2 7 (6)
  3 13 (11)
  6 1 (1)
  Unable to genotype 1(1)
Transmission mode
  Vertical/Perinatal 86 (75)
  Sexual contact 1 (1)
  IV drug use 1 (1)
  Transfusion 8 (7)
  Unknown 18 (16)
ALT 60 ± 49
Infection duration, mos 108 ± 55

Data are presented as Mean ± SD or No. (%)

HCV = Hepatitis C Virus; ALT = Alanine aminotransferase

Quality of Life

QOL scores for the study sample did not differ significantly from the normative sample, although they were consistently higher than scores reported by another group of children with HCV (14) and children with diabetes (21) (Table 2). However, two of the scaled scores, General Health Perceptions (p < 0.0001) and Parent Impact-Emotional (p < 0.0001), were significantly lower in the study sample than in the normative sample. CHQ scores did not vary significantly based on the caregiver’s own HCV status.

Table 2.

CHQ summary and scaled scores for study sample, normative sample, pediatric HCV sample, and diabetes comparison group

Study Sample (n=114) Normative Sample16 (n=391) Pediatric HCV Sample14 (n=19) Diabetes Sample21 (n=128)
Physical Summary 52 ± 6 53 ± 9 45 ± 11** 49 ± 8**
Psychosocial Summary 52 ± 9 51 ± 9 44 ± 12** 49 ± 10
Physical Functioning 96 ± 10 96 ± 14 85 ± 20** 94 ± 11
Role/Social Emotional/Behavioral 95 ± 16 93 ± 19 77 ± 35** 94 ± 14
Role/Social Physical 97 ± 10 94 ± 19 83 ± 28*** 92 ± 19
Bodily Pain 82 ± 19 82 ± 19 84 ± 15 73 ± 25
General Behavior 79 ± 17 76 ± 17 70 ± 21 71 ± 17**
Mental Health 80 ± 12 79 ± 13 72 ± 16 77 ± 13
Self Esteem 83 ± 17 80 ± 18 72 ± 19 83 ± 16
General Health Perceptions 66 ± 15 73 ± 17*** 50 ± 17*** 59 ± 16**
Parental Impact - Emotional 65 ± 27 80 ± 19*** 46 ± 31 62 ± 27
Parental Impact – Time 88 ± 17 88 ± 20 78 ± 24 76 ± 23***
Family Activities 85 ± 19 90 ± 19 75 ± 24 67 ± 23***
Family Cohesion 75 ± 22 72 ± 22 NA 75 ± 25

Data are expressed as Mean ± SD

Higher scores reflect better QOL

**

p ≤ 0.001

***

p ≤ 0.0001

Caregivers of children with HCV did not report QOL that was significantly different from the SF-36 normative sample (Table 3). However, their QOL scores were significantly higher than those reported by another sample of adults with newly diagnosed HCV (22). Table 4 showsthat mothers with HCV who transmitted the virus to the enrolled child had QOL scores that were significantly lower than caregivers without HCV on three SF-36 domains: role functioning-physical, role functioning-emotional, and general health. Scores on the other SF-36 scales also trended in the same direction. Higher child QOL, both physical and psychosocial, was significantly associated with higher caregiver QOL across most domains (Table 5).

Table 3.

SF-36 scaled scores for caregivers in the study sample, normative sample, and comparison groups

Study Sample (n=114) Normative Sample20 (n=2,474) Adult HCV Sample22 (n=70)
Physical Functioning 82 ± 25 84 ± 23 82 ± 17
Role-Physical 82 ± 26 81 ± 34 58 ± 45***
Bodily Pain 75 ± 27 75 ± 24 67 ± 26
General Health 69 ± 24 72 ± 20 43 ± 27***
Vitality 60 ± 20 61 ± 21 49 ± 20***
Social Functioning 83 ± 24 83 ± 23 65 ± 18***
Role-Emotional 84 ± 23 81 ± 33 64 ± 43***
Mental Health 72 ± 19 75 ± 18 66 ± 11*

Data are expressed as Mean ± SD

**

p ≤ 0.001

***

p ≤ 0.0001

Table 4.

SF-36 scaled scores (mean ± s.d.) for HCV-infected mothers who transmitted virus to the enrolled child versus non-infected caregivers

HCV-infected mothers (n=43) Non-infected caregivers (n=67)
Physical Functioning 77 ± 27 86 ± 23
Role-Physical 75 ± 28 87 ± 24*
Bodily Pain 68 ± 29 80 ± 24
General Health 57 ± 23 77 ± 21*
Vitality 56 ± 20 63 ± 20
Social Functioning 77 ± 24 87 ± 23
Role-Emotional 77 ± 23 90 ± 21*
Mental Health 68 ± 19 75 ± 19
*

p < 0.006

Table 5.

Spearman correlations between child (CHQ) and parent (SF-36) QOL

Caregiver QOL (SF-36)

Physical Functioning Role-Physical Bodily Pain General Health Vitality Social Functioning Role-Emotional Mental Health
CHQ – Physical Summary 0.20* 0.27** 0.31*** 0.17 0.22* 0.27** 0.33*** 0.31**
CHQ Psychosocial Summary 0.03 0.18 0.16 0.31** 0.21* 0.30** 0.27** 0.35***
*

p < 0.05

**

p < 0.01

***

p < 0.001

Cognitive Functioning

Children with HCV differed significantly from the normative (17) and pre-liver transplant (Rodrigue JR, Guenther R, unpublished data) samples on several clinical and index scales (Table 6). However, executive function ratings for the HCV study sample were significantly better than those for children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (23). T scores ≥ 65 are considered indicative of clinical impairment in executive function. Although the study sample means were in the normal range, 18% percent of children showed clinically significant impairment in executive function, as defined by elevated Global Executive Composite T scores.

Table 6.

BRIEF T scores for study sample, normative sample, and comparison groups. Percent (%) in clinical range provided for study sample only.

Study Sample (n=114) Normative Sample17 (n=1,078) Pre-Transplant Sample (n=67) ADHD Sample23 (n=130)
Behavioral Regulation 52 ± 10 (13) 50 ± 10 55 ± 14 59 ± 11***
Metacognition 54 ± 11 (18) 50 ± 10 57 ± 12 68 ± 8***
Global Executive Composite 54 ± 11 (18) 50 ± 10 58 ± 13 66 ± 9***
Inhibition 55 ± 10 (18) 50 ± 10 54 ± 11 59 ± 12
Shift 52 ± 10 (15) 50 ± 10 55 ± 15 59 ± 12***
Emotional Control 50 ± 10 (8) 50 ± 10 54 ± 15 58 ± 13***
Initiation 53 ± 11 (18) 50 ± 10 58 ± 11** 61 ± 10***
Working Memory 54 ± 11 (19) 50 ± 10 62 ± 12*** 70 ± 9***
Plan/Organization 54 ± 11 (18) 50 ± 10 56 ± 13 67 ± 10***
Organization of Materials 52 ± 11 (15) 50 ± 10 53 ± 10 63 ± 12***
Monitor 52 ± 10 (13) 50 ± 10 55 ± 13 63 ± 9***

Rodrigue JR, Guenther R, unpublished data based on a comprehensive questionnaire battery completed by parents as part of the required pre-liver transplant psychological assessment at the University of Florida. This sample includes children and adolescents (mean age 9.3±5; 59% female; 90% White) with different causes of liver disease who were referred, but not yet formally approved, for liver transplantation.

ADHD = Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

Data are expressed as Mean ± SD (% in clinical range for the study sample only)

Higher scores reflect worse cognitive functioning

**

p ≤ 0.001

***

p ≤ 0.0001

Behavioral/Emotional Functioning

Mean CBCL scores were all within the normal range. However, all of our HCV study patients’ CBCL clinical scales were significantly higher than the normative sample, although they did not differ significantly from children awaiting liver transplantation (Rodrigue JR, Guenther R, unpublished data) or those with HIV infection (24). Regarding clinical significance (defined as T score ≥ 65), 13% and 9% of children had clinically significant internalizing (somatic problems, depression, anxiety) and externalizing problems (aggressive behavior, social problems), respectively. Twenty percent reported clinical elevation on the Somatic Problems scale.

The mean CDI T score (44 ± 6) was in the normal, non-depressed range. Only 2 (2%) children had a CDI score in the clinically depressed range (defined as T score ≥ 65). Higher CDI scores were significantly associated with more behavioral problems (Rho = 0.34, p = 0.0003), more impairment in global executive function (Rho = 0.40, p < 0.0001), lower physical health-related QOL (Rho = −0.46, p < 0.0001), and higher emotional impact for parents (Rho = −0.22, p =0.02).

HCV Duration of Infection and Outcomes

HCV duration of infection was significantly correlated with the BRIEF Metacognition score (Rho = −0.22, p = 0.02). Children with longer HCV duration had less impairment in metacognitive processes. Illness duration was not associated with CDI or CBCL scores.

DISCUSSION

Findings from this study indicate that QOL, cognitive, behavioral, and emotional functioning are not globally impaired in children with HCV infection. As a group, the children showed no deficits in QOL, behavioral problems, or clinical depression, relative to normative samples. Furthermore, their functioning in these domains was comparable to or better than that of other children with chronic health conditions. While HCV diagnosis appears to cause stress, depression, and anxiety in newly diagnosed adults (912), there is little evidence that these symptoms are pervasive in children and adolescents. When compared to rates reported in the adult HCV literature (as high as 75%)(25), relatively few children have clinical levels of depression, behavioral disturbances, or QOL impairment.

Collectively, primary caregivers also do not appear to experience any significant QOL decrements. However, mothers who vertically transmitted HCV to the enrolled child reported more compromised QOL than caregivers who did not have HCV. This finding is consistent with research showing that QOL deteriorates in the context of chronic HCV (5,6,9,25,26), although there is some evidence that QOL improves with successful antiviral therapy (8,26,27). We did not assess mothers’ HCV liver disease status or treatment history, so we are unable to examine the relationship between these parameters and QOL in this study. Clearly, more research is needed to better understand the psychological sequelae associated with parents who have transmitted HCV to their children. In such circumstances, mothers may be simultaneously dealing with the demands of their own illness and coping with issues of disclosing and discussing viral transmission to their child, feelings of guilt or shame, and depression. While some of these issues have been the focus of study within the HIV literature (28,29), they have not yet been examined in the context of HCV.

The QOL of children HCV who are treatment-naïve does not appear to be negatively affected, relative to otherwise healthy children. Indeed, QOL in some areas is higher than that reported for children with HIV or diabetes. Moreover, as reported by their caregivers, the QOL of children in our sample was generally higher than that reported by Nydegger et al (14). The disparate QOL findings in these two studies may be explained, in part, by cultural factors, sample size differences, and sociodemographic and medical factors. For instance, Nydegger et al. studied only 19 children (83% male) with predominantly transfusion-acquired HCV. Of particular note, Nydegger et al. asked 10 adolescents to self-report on their QOL and found that their perceptions, in striking contrast to their parents’ assessment, did not differ significantly from that of otherwise health children.

Our findings suggest that some caregivers of children with HCV are distressed about their children’s medical circumstances, which tends to support Nydegger et al.’s recent findings. In both studies, caregivers report relatively high concern and worry about the children’s current and future physical health, emotional well-being, and general behavior. Parents may also be concerned about their child will be treated by family members, friends, and teachers because of their HCV status (14). We found that parents of children with more advanced HCV infection seem to be more stressed, which may be due to both the implications of liver disease progression and the behavioral symptoms exhibited by the children. Regardless, these findings highlight the importance of assessing parental adjustment and adaptation throughout their child’s HCV management.

Adults with HCV have been shown to experience cognitive deficits, particularly in attention and higher executive functioning (10). These deficits may be secondary to the direct effect of HCV on the central nervous system, rather than the indirect effects of fatigue or depression that often accompany HCV (30,31). Executive function in the HCV-infected children was slightly impaired relative to normative data and relative to children evaluated for liver transplantation. Those with HCV were rated as having more difficulty with planning and organizational skills, as well as inhibiting one’s own behavior. However, they fare considerably better than children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, whose condition is known to significantly impair executive functioning. One-fifth of the HCV study sample showed evidence of problems in executive function. Because we did not directly assess cognitive performance in this sample of children, it is possible that HCV contributes to more subtle neuropsychological difficulties that are not apparent to their parents and, therefore, not detected in this study. Additional research is necessary to more systematically examine whether early stage HCV infection directly affects central nervous system function and impairs academic performance.

There are only limited data on the natural history of HCV in children.(32,33) However, available evidence suggests that HCV may represent a milder disease process in children than in adults.(34) Therefore, HCV, especially in the early stages, may not cause any significant impairment in physical functioning, social activities, and bodily discomfort. Moreover, in the absence of functional impairment, children and adolescents may not experience any behavioral or emotional sequelae that can be linked to their medical diagnosis. Only two children had clinical depression, but it is important to emphasize that those with consistent mood disturbance were not enrolled in the study. Per study protocol, enrolled children with clinically high CDI scores were further interviewed by their pediatrician investigator, who initiated consultation with a mental health professional

It is possible that introducing antiviral treatment may precipitate changes along these behavioral and emotional parameters. For instance, Iorio et al. (13) reported that QOL deteriorated significantly during antiviral treatment in children. While QOL returned to baseline within 3 months of stopping interferon, it is possible that a very different clinical profile than the one found in our study will emerge for children on antiviral therapy. Notwithstanding the findings of Iorio et al. (13) and Nydegger et al. (14), the favorable QOL of children in our study may argue for aggressive antiviral treatment before the development of the cognitive and psychological problems seen in adults with HCV.

Study findings should be examined within the context of a few methodological limitations. First, this is a study of children in the early stages of HCV who are treatment-naïve. Therefore, findings should not be generalized beyond these medical parameters. Second, we compared children with HCV to normative samples of predominantly healthy children, rather than to a matched control group. Third, we relied exclusively on the report of primary caregivers about the functioning of children in this study. Such reports can be biased for many reasons and may not accurately capture the true functional status of the child. Some studies have shown that as children get older, the gap between parent and child ratings of behavior and emotional adjustment widens (35). Future studies should use multiple informants, including the children themselves, their primary caregivers, and teachers.

In conclusion, children with HCV in its early stages may not show signs of significant cognitive, behavioral, or emotional impairment. Nevertheless, we encourage the close monitoring of these children over time, especially once antiviral treatment has been initiated.

Table 7.

CBCL T scores for study sample, normative sample, and comparison groups. Percent (%) in clinical range provided for study sample only.

Study Sample (n=114) Normative Sample18 (n=1,753) Pre-transplant Sample (n=67) HIV Sample24 (n=36)
Internalizing 52 ± 10 (13) 50 ± 10 53 ± 13 50 ± 11
Externalizing 50 ± 10 (9) 50 ± 10 52 ± 10 53 ± 11
Total Behavior Problem 51 ± 10 (10) 50 ± 10 53 ± 11 NA
Anxious/Depressed 54 ± 5 (7) 50 ± 10*** 55 ± 11 NA
Withdrawn/Depressed 54 ± 6 (10) 50 ± 10*** 52 ± 8 NA
Somatic Problems 58 ± 7 (20) 50 ± 10*** 57 ± 9 NA
Social problems 55 ± 6 (7) 50 ± 10*** 53 ± 10 NA
Thought Problems 55 ± 6 (9) 50 ± 10*** 51 ± 12 NA
Attention Problems 55 ± 6 (10) 50 ± 10*** 54 ± 7 NA
Rule-Breaking Behavior 54 ± 5 (5) 50 ± 10*** 52 ± 7 NA
Aggressive Behavior 54 ± 7 (8) 50 ± 10*** 51 ± 9 NA

Data are expressed as Mean ± SD (% in clinical range for the study sample only)

Higher scores reflect more behavioral problems

Rodrigue JR, Guenther, R, unpublished data

**

p ≤ 0.001

***

p ≤ 0.0001

Acknowledgments

Grant Support: This study is supported by a cooperative agreement between the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases and the Food and Drug Administration, contract number 1UO1DK067767-01. Additional support is provided by Hoffman-La Roche for the study medications and central laboratory costs.

List of Abbreviations

HCV

hepatitis C virus

QOL

quality of life

CHQ

Child Health Questionnaire

BRIEF

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function

CBCL

Child Behavior Checklist

CDI

Child Depression Inventory

ALT

Alanine aminotransferase

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest: None

REFERENCES

  • 1.Alter MJ, Kruszon-Moran D, Nainan OV, et al. The prevalence of hepatitis C virus infection in the United States: 1988 through 1994. New Engl J Med. 1999;341:556–562. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199908193410802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Zellos A, Thomas DL, Mocilnikar C, et al. High viral load and mild liver injury in young hemophiliacs compared to other children with hepatitis C virus (HVC) infection. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 1999;29:418–423. doi: 10.1097/00005176-199910000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Vogt M, Lang T, Frösner G, et al. Prevalence and clinical outcome of hepatitis C infection in children who underwent cardiac surgery before the implementation of blood-donor screening. New Engl J Med. 1999;341:866–870. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199909163411202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Chang MH, Ni YH, Hwang LH, et al. Long term clinical and virologic outcome of primary hepatitis C virus infection in children: a prospective study. Pediatr Infect Dis J. 1994;13:769–773. doi: 10.1097/00006454-199409000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Younossi Z, Kallman J, Kincaid J. The effects of HCV infection and management on health-related quality of life. Hepatology. 2007;45:806–816. doi: 10.1002/hep.21565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kallman J, O'neil MM, Larive B, et al. Fatigue and health-related quality of life (HRQL) in chronic hepatitis C virus infection. Dig Dis Sci. 2007;52:2531–2539. doi: 10.1007/s10620-006-9708-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.González-Peralta RP, Langham MR, Andres JM, et al. Hepatocellular carcinoma in two young adolescents with chronic hepatitis C. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 2008 doi: 10.1097/MPG.0b013e318170af04. In press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lim JK, Cronkite R, Goldstein MK, et al. The impact of chronic hepatitis C and comorbid psychiatric illnesses on health-related quality of life. J Clin Gastroenterol. 2006;40:528–534. doi: 10.1097/00004836-200607000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Spiegel BM, Younossi ZM, Hays RD, et al. Impact of hepatitis C on health related quality of life: a systematic review and quantitative assessment. Hepatology. 2005;41:790–800. doi: 10.1002/hep.20659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Forton DM, Thomas HC, Murphy CA, et al. Hepatitis C and cognitive impairment in a cohort of patients with mild liver disease. Hepatology. 2002;35:433–439. doi: 10.1053/jhep.2002.30688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hilsabeck RC, Hassanein TI, Carlson MD, et al. Cognitive functioning and psychiatric symptomatology in patients with chronic hepatitis C. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003;9:847–854. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703960048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Goulding C, O'Connell P, Murray FE. Prevalence of fibromyalgia, anxiety and depression in chronic hepatitis C virus infection: relationship to RT-PCR status and mode of acquisition. Eur J Gastroenterol Hepatol. 2001;13:507–511. doi: 10.1097/00042737-200105000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Iorio R, Pensati P, Botta S, et al. Side effects of alpha-interferon therapy and impact on health-related quality of life in children with chronic viral hepatitis. Pediatr Infect Dis J. 1997;16:984–990. doi: 10.1097/00006454-199710000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Nydegger A, Srivastava A, Wake M, et al. Health-related quality of life in children with hepatitis C acquired in the first year of life. Hepatology. 2008;23:226–230. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1746.2007.04859.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Murray KF, Rodrigue JR, González-Peralta RP, et al. Design of the PEDS-C Trial: Pegylated Interferon +/− Ribavirin for children with chronic hepatitis C viral infection. Clin Trials. 2007;4:661–673. doi: 10.1177/1740774507085445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Landgraf JM, Abetz L, Ware JE. The CHQ User's Manual. Boston: New England Medical Center; The Health Institute. 1996
  • 17.Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, et al. Psychological Assessment Resources. Odessa FL: 2000. Behavior rating inventory of executive function. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kovacs M. Children’s Depression Inventory manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems Inc. 1992
  • 20.Ware JE. SF-36 health survey: manual and interpretation guide. Lincoln, RI: QualityMetric, Inc. 1993
  • 21.Wake M, Hesketh K, Cameron F. The Child Health Questionnaire in children with diabetes: cross-sectional survey of parent and adolescent-reported functional health status. Diabet Med. 2000;17:700–707. doi: 10.1046/j.1464-5491.2000.00360.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Rodger AJ, Jolley D, Thompson SC, et al. The impact of diagnosis of hepatitis C virus on quality of life. Hepatology. 1999;30:1299–1301. doi: 10.1002/hep.510300504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Di Pinto M. Doctoral dissertation. Drexel University; 2006. May, The ecological validity of the BRIEF in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bachanas PJ, Kullgren KA, Schwartz KS, et al. Predictors of psychological adjustment in school-age children infected with HIV. Pediatr Psychol. 2001;26:343–352. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/26.6.343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Dbouk N, Arguedas MR, Sheikh A. Assessment of the PHQ-9 as a screening tool for depression in patients with chronic hepatitis C. Dig Dis Sci. 2008;53:1100–1106. doi: 10.1007/s10620-007-9985-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Foster G, Goldin R, Thomas H. Chronic hepatitis C infection causes a significant reduction in quality of life in the absence of cirrhosis. Hepatology. 1998;27:209–212. doi: 10.1002/hep.510270132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.McHutchison JG, Ware JE, Jr, Bayliss MS, et al. Hepatitis interventional therapy group. The effects of interferon alpha-2b in combination with ribavirin on health related quality of life and work productivity. J Hepatol. 2001;34(140):147. doi: 10.1016/s0168-8278(00)00026-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Bennetts A, Shaffer N, Manopaiboon C, et al. Determinants of depression and HIV-related worry among HIV-positive women who have recently given birth, Bangkok, Thailand. Soc Sci Med. 1999;49:737–749. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(99)00108-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Murphy DA. HIV-positive mothers' disclosure of their serostatus to their young children: a review. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;13:105–122. doi: 10.1177/1359104507087464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Forton DM, Taylor-Robinson SD, Thomas HC. Central nervous system changes in hepatitis C virus infection. Eur J Gastroenterol Hepatol. 2006;18:333–338. doi: 10.1097/00042737-200604000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Weissenborn K, Krause J, Bokemeyer M, et al. Hepatitis C virus infection affects the brain-evidence from psychometric studies and magnetic resonance spectroscopy. J Hepatol. 2004;41:845–851. doi: 10.1016/j.jhep.2004.07.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Mohan P, Colvin C, Glymph C, et al. Clinical spectrum and histopathologic features of chronic hepatitis C infection in children. J Pediatr. 2007;150:168–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2006.11.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Vogt M, Lang T, Frosner G, et al. Prevalence and clinical outcome of hepatitis C infection in children who underwent cardiac surgery before the implementation of blood-donor screening. N Engl J Med. 1999;341:866–870. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199909163411202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Murray KF, Finn LS, Taylor SL, et al. Liver histology and alanine aminotransferase levels in children and adults with chronic hepatitis C infection. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 2005;41:634–638. doi: 10.1097/01.mpg.0000179758.82919.1f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Informant, gender and age differences in ratings of adolescent problem behaviour. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;14:117–126. doi: 10.1007/s00787-005-0438-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES