Abstract
We used in vivo voltage-sensitive dye optical imaging to examine the cortical representation of interaural time difference (ITD), which is believed to be involved in sound source localization. We found that acoustic stimuli with dissimilar ITD activate various localized domains in the auditory cortex. The main loci of the activation pattern shift up to 1 mm during the first 40 ms of the response period. We suppose that some of the neurons in each pool are sensitive to the definite ITD and involved in the transduction of information about sound source localization, based on the ITD. This assumption gives a reasonable fit to the Jeffress model in which the neural network calculates the ITD to define the direction of the sound source. Such calculation forms the basis for the cortex's ability to detect the azimuth of the sound source.
Keywords: auditory cortex, interaural time difference, optical imaging, voltage-sensitive dye
Introduction
As is well known, one of the main structural principles of the auditory cortex is tonotopicity (Ehret, 1997). In the auditory cortex of higher mammals, isofrequency areas appear in the form of elongated spots orthogonal to the rostro-caudal axis (Eggermont, 2006; Ehret, 1997; Sally and Kelly, 1988). In a predator's AI (primary auditory cortex), high-frequency representations are located in the rostral area and low-frequency representations in the caudal area. Primates demonstrate similar structure; low frequencies are represented anterolaterally, high frequencies posteromedially in AI. (Furukawa and Middlebrooks, 2002; Hosokawa et al., 2004; Tokioka et al., 2000).
Voltage-sensitive dyes (VSD) fluoresce proportional to changes in transmembrane potential, and thus can be used to detect neural activity (Grinvald et al., 2001). Using modern charge couple devices (CCD – cameras), functional mapping of the neocortex can achieve a very high spatial resolution (Grinvald et al., 2001; Inyushin et al., 2001). VSD imaging has been proven as a powerful tool for the investigation of the functional architecture of the auditory cortex (Grinvald et al., 2001; Nishimura et al., 2006; Song et al., 2006) and cochlear nucleus (Kaltenbach and Zhang, 2004).
In vivo VSD optical imaging allows us to make functional maps of the neocortex, and, in the case of imaging the auditory cortex, satisfy the question of where the neural pools responsible for sound source localization are located in the brain (Eggermont, 2006; Jenkins and Merzenich, 1984; Spitzer and Takahashi, 2006; Tsytsarev and Tanaka, 2002). In other words, the critical problem in neural network studies is how the computational ability of the neural network in the neocortex can be applied to define the location of sound sources.
Animals can usually localize sound clicks in the horizontal dimension. However, when two clicks presented to the left and right ears are separated by a short time delay, the animal can experience an illusion, failing to properly localize the sound (Figure 1). In the present study, we demonstrate that the spatial distribution of sound sources is not only reflected by cortical activity but also creates patterns similar to a set of moving activated spots, each representing a specific azimuth angle toward the object.
Materials and Methods
Imaging was performed in 12 adult rats (Sprague–Dawley, weight 250–300 g, age 2–3 months) that were anesthetized with a mixture of ketamine (90 mg/kg, i.p. Ketalar-50, Parke-Davis) and xylazine (12 mg/kg, i.p., Bayer Corp). Supplementary ketamine hydrochloride (40 mg/kg body weight, i.m.) was injected about 1 h later to maintain a constant level of anesthesia during the experiment (Tsytsarev and Tanaka, 2002). The anesthesia level was determined based on the heart rate, which was continuously monitored using an electrocardiomonitor. At the end of each experiment, the animal was sacrificed using an overdose of Nembutal (200 mg/kg body weight, i.p., Bayer Corp.). All procedures were carried out according to the standards of the Animal Care Committee of Kyoto Prefecture University of Medicine.
A detailed description of the experimental technique can be found in cited references Bonhoeffer and Grinvald (1996) and Grinvald et al. (2001). The animal was fixed on the stereotaxic frame using ear bars with channels that allowed sound transduction into the middle ear (Tsytsarev et al., 2004). The skin and muscles of the dorsolateral part of the head were surgically excised and the left lateral wall of the skull was exposed from the muscles. The cranium above the auditory cortex was removed using a dental drill, and a recording chamber made of dental wax (4–6 mm i.d., height 2–3 mm) was constructed above the hole in the skull (Bahar et al., 2006; Ojima et al., 2005). The dura mater over the auditory cortex was removed and the brain was covered with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) at a temperature of 30–35°C (Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2005; Takashima et al., 2005; Versnel et al., 2002). The VSD RH-795 (Molecular Probes, 0.6 mg/ml in ACSF), was applied to the exposed cortex for approximately 45 min. After staining, the cortex was washed with dye-free ACSF for approximately 15 min (Grinvald et al., 2001). A CCD camera (MiCAM-01, Brain Vision Inc., 2002, Japan, http://www.scimedia.com), (90 × 60 pixels, 0.7 ms/frame) then positioned above the chamber and directed such that its optical axis was perpendicular to the cortical surface.
The focusing plane was manipulated 400 μm below the cortical surface. The recording area of the cortex was illuminated with a 540 nm wavelength light via a cube-system with two optic filters and a dichroic mirror. At the start of each optical recording, a gray-scale image of the region of interest was obtained. Each stimulus consisted of a pair of 30 μs square-pulse binaural clicks with interaural time difference (ITD) 0 or 100 μs (Figure 1). During recording, stimuli with different ITDs were presented in a quasi-random sequence starting at the tenth frame with a 4 s interval in between. Since heart rhythm produces sudden changes in fluorescence during a heart contraction, a light path shutter was synchronized with the electrocardiogram: the light shutter was opened only during the period between two consecutive heartbeats in order to minimize light exposure and prevent dye bleaching. We used a University constructed low-noise shutter and a special technique from our previous studies (Tsytsarev and Tanaka, 2002; Tsytsarev et al., 2006) for minimization of shutter noise. In short, after insertion of the earphones into the ears the auricles were filled with Vaseline and the ears were wrapped with tape. To prevent a sound advancing through the metal apparatus, a stereotaxic frame was placed on the foam-rubber plate. As was shown in previous electrophysiological studies, (Tsytsarev et al., 2006) such an experimental set up prevents the shutter sound from confounding the results. To set an electrocardiogram threshold the shutter was controlled by a differential alternating current amplifier (model 1700, AM Systems Inc, WA, USA) with window discriminator (WD01, Scientific Ltd, UK). Thus, the sequence of the stimulation and recording of evoked optical signal was as follows: Heart beat, shutter opening, start of the optical signal recording, 10 ms pause, sound stimulation, end of recording trial (after a total recording time of 82 ms), shutter closing.
Four pilot experiments with pure tone binaural stimulation and wideband sound clicks were performed to test our method. The sound stimuli had frequencies of 1, 5, and 7 kHz and a duration of 40 ms. The linearly increasing onset and decreasing offset of stimulus envelopes were set at 5 ms. All stimuli were calibrated before optical recording and the sound pressure was set at 45 dB. As in the previously described method, (Nishimura et al., 2007; Tsytsarev and Tanaka, 2002) the images were combined to construct a frequency map (Figure 2A). As expected, the tonotopic representation of sound frequencies in these experiments demonstrated that the location and direction of sound frequency arrangement closely agrees with previously obtained data (Kalatsky et al., 2005; Nishimura et al., 2007; Takahashi et al., 2004; Tsytsarev et al., 2006). On that ground, we concluded that our method was sufficiently reliable and proceeded to test the sound stimuli with various ITD.
Analysis of the recorded image data was performed using Brain Vision Analyzer (©, Brain Vision Inc., Tokyo, Japan). Presentation of sound stimuli took place after the first 10 frames so data prior to frame 10 contains only random non-sound-evoked fluctuations. To extract the images reflecting neural activity in response to sound stimuli, we applied “first-frame analysis,” in which signals in the first 10 prestimulus frames (averaged from multiple trials) were subtracted from signals in the subsequent frames (Bonhoeffer and Grinvald, 1996). In the simplest case of “first-frame analysis” the first prestimulus frame must be subtracted from each subsequent frame, but we used an averaged value of 10 prestimulus frames instead (Tsytsarev et al., 2004). This procedure was applied to the recorded signals at each trial, which were then averaged. We then reconstructed optical signals elicited in response to each acoustic stimulus using the generalized indicator function method, which projects the optical imaging data onto a subspace of high signal-to-noise ratio (Bonhoeffer and Grinvald, 1996).
The temporal changes of the fluorescence after the stimulus onset were examined in a small rectangular domain of 2 × 2 pixels. This domain was chosen in order to avoid intersecting any blood vessels. To eliminate fluctuation of the signal strength between individual rats we normalized the signals using the maximum ΔF/F in response to contralateral stimulation before the intertrial averaging procedure. The temporal changes of the optical signal are shown in Figure 2B. To construct color-coded maps and determine the areas significantly activated by a particular ITD, the images of optical signals were thresholded by 50% of the maximum response inside the area of recording. Thus, we obtained maps of strongly activated areas that were color-coded according to the ITD of the stimuli.
Results
Optical signals evoked by a binaural click stimulus at a sound pressure level of 45 dB were recorded in each rat. Figure 3 displays the time courses of optical images in rat auditory cortex as a result of optical evoked activity produced with three different stimuli: ipsi- and contralateral click leading with ITD of 100 μs, and simultaneous click in both ears. Figure 3 shows three separate experiments with three different rats tracing the dynamic patterns of the evoked activity. As expected, each stimulus induced activity in several small cortex areas. These areas do not have definite form and can change in just tens of milliseconds. Such patterns were observed in all experiments.
Figure 4 shows the temporal evolution of the spatial pattern of optical signals evoked by one particular sound stimulus (ITD 100 μs, contralateral click leading). We can observe that the shape of the activated area changes over time following stimulus onset. For all stimuli, activated areas steadily increase from the 15th to 50th ms and then decrease; the activated domains appear as irregularly shaped spots.
In all cases, we found that sound stimuli with different ITD led to different cortical activation patterns. But, in an experiment with a single animal, various sessions produced the same patterns with each separate ITD. One ITD typically activated two spatially disconnected regions (Figure 3). The patterns were randomly distributed and had irregular shapes. The activated patterns moved around the cortex within 0.1–0.3 mm; there is no statistically significant difference between the movements of activations caused by different stimuli (P = 0.963). The mean velocity was 5.8 mm/s, which was found to be significantly differentially expressed [one way analysis of variance (ANOVA) P < 0.01]. The absolute displacement was close in all experiments with little statistical straggling. In all cases, the response reached its maximum value after 30–40 ms (Figures 5A–C). At the same time, we did not find any systematic character in the latency period changes of each pattern with different ITD. The results obtained in different sessions (which took place one after another) with the same animal were practically identical. These results suggest that any complete explanation of the cortical representation of the ITD must incorporate the temporal characteristics of the neuronal response patterns.
We found that each ITD usually corresponded to a common pattern. From the time of origination until the disappearance of the pattern, its spatial extent approximately doubled and then decreased. At the maximum size, the patterns covered an area of 0.2–0.4 mm.
Comparative analysis of the areas activated by two different stimuli or by the same stimulus at different times after stimulus onset was completed. After setting the threshold at 50% (see Materials and Methods), the sizes of the superposed areas were calculated and averaged. Results of this analysis are presented in Figure 5.
Figure 5 shows superimposition of the areas activated by different stimuli at the same time after a stimulus onset of 30 ms (Figure 5D), 50 ms (Figure 5E), and 70 ms (Figure 5F). The sizes of the cortical parts activated by different stimuli are very similar (0.01–0.02 mm). They remain stable at the 30th, 50th, and 70th ms after stimulus onset. The common area of the cortical parts, activated at the 30th and 70th ms after stimulus onset is much smaller than areas activated at the 30th and 50th ms or the 50th and 70th ms (Figures 5G–I). This regularity is significant for all stimuli, although the effect is smaller for stimuli with an ITD of 0 (ANOVA P < 0.01).
Discussion
Topographical representation is a main feature of the sensory cortices of mammals. The primary somatosensory cortex contains a somatotopic map of the body, the primary visual cortex contains a retinotopic map of the visual field, and the primary auditory cortex (AI) contains a tonotopic map of sound frequency. Based on this principle, many scientists expect the presence of a spatial auditory map.
In 1950 this hypothesis was proposed by Jeffress and has been discussed by many authors during the last decade (King et al., 2001; McAlpine and Grothe, 2003; McAlpine et al., 2001; Zador, 2001). The features of the representation of auditory space in the brain have been observed in the superior colliculus (King et al., 2001; Nelken et al., 2004) for mammals and in the thalamus (Proctor and Konishi, 1997) for avians. Nevertheless, spatial representation of sound source has not been demonstrated in the mammalian neocortex. It is hypothesized that ITD are calculated in the superior olivary nucleus of the brainstem (Tollin, 2003), where neurons with various connecting axon lengths receive innervations from each ear.
Many authors (McAlpine and Grothe, 2003; McAlpine et al., 2001; Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2005) suppose that if the Jeffress model is correct, the various cues of virtual auditory space might be integrated into a tonotopic map. Numerous scientists, however, have failed to find the auditory space map in the mammalian neocortex (Furukawa and Middlebrooks, 2002; Mickey and Middlebrooks, 2005). However, such a map being described in the owl's thalamus has given hope to the prospect of finding one in mammals (Proctor and Konishi, 1997). In spite of the fact that a representation of the sound source in the neocortex of mammals has not been found, work in recent decades has shown that separate neurons of the AI have the property of definite spatial selectivity (Doan and Saunders, 1999; Furukawa and Middlebrooks, 2002; Kelly and Phillips, 1991; Mickey and Middlebrooks, 2005; Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2005).
Jeffress developed the first model for sound localization using a projective interference circuit (Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2005; Nelken et al., 2004), noise originating from an external source to the right which interferes with leftward excitation and visa versa. Malhotra et al. (2004) used terms “what” and “where” to denote different processes of sound perception. It seems reasonable to say that the “what” and “where” processing in non-primary auditory cortex can be divided among the anterior and posterior auditory fields, as was demonstrated by Malhotra et al. (2004). It is therefore possible that the location of a sound source is inferentially represented by the tonotopically organized arrays of auditory neurons as a spatiotemporal population code.
Several lesion studies suggest that AI is required for sound source localization in carnivores (Kummierek et al., 2007), primates, (Kummierek et al., 2007) and martens (Smith et al., 2004), but the question of how different fields of the rodent's auditory cortex relate to each other in the sound localization process is still just beginning to be explored. Numerous studies using anesthetized animals have consistently found that many neurons of the auditory cortex are sensitive to sound source location, but topographical representation of space has not been demonstrated (Furukawa and Middlebrooks, 2002; Kalatsky et al., 2005; King et al., 2001).
However, our experimental data implies that the ITD-receptive patterns originate from the interaction between the frequency and non-frequency sensitivity of cortical neurons. One could say the ITD-activated patterns shift in ways that are directly related to individual acoustic cue response differences between animals.
It has been reported that the auditory cortical neurons encode some sound parameters, such as duration, frequency preference, and ipsi-, contra-or bilateral preference (Barbour and Wang, 2002; Ehret, 1997). We suppose that some neurons might be organized into an ITD-preferred pool. In light of the present study, the cortical areas involved in spatial localization clearly contain more than one region of auditory cortex. It has been shown previously that the neuronal spike patterns in the auditory cortex represent information about the azimuth of the sound source (Furukawa and Middlebrooks, 2002; Mickey and Middlebrooks, 2005) and that these neurons are distributed widely throughout the auditory cortex.
Our results demonstrate that patterns of neural activity recorded in response to binaural click presentations can encode ITD. Based upon the data presented here, we conclude that some neurons in the auditory cortex receive information from several neural pools, each of which processes separate stimuli. The ITD information is then converted into neural code and passed to the auditory cortex.
A standout feature of these domains is their tendency to shift in space over time. Even though these movements can become relatively large, the regions never overlap by more than approximately 25% of their total size. Additionally, no two regions ever occupy the exact same spot, even at different time points. These facts help show that the areas remain spatially segregated despite their tendency to move.
In 2001, we showed that acoustic stimuli with distinct ITD activated various domains localized in AI (Tsytsarev and Tanaka, 2002). It is important to note that the center of the activated domain shifted as the time difference was changed (Tsytsarev and Tanaka, 2002). In our opinion, this fact, taken together with the idea that ITD plays an important role in sound source localization, suggests that AI is involved in information transformation from ITD to sound source location. Obtained results show that the cortical representation of sound sources is reflected by neural activity and creates a pattern like a “moving (floating?) mosaic”. The data presented here enable us – despite the fact that the virtual map of auditory space in rat auditory cortex does not exist – to suppose that the neurons which are selectively sensitive to different ITDs are structured in definite groups. Overall, the present study demonstrates the effectiveness of the neuronal ensemble coding of a range of different ITDs. Further studies will be necessary to identify details of information-bearing features and the neural mechanisms of ITD and its transduction into source localization. Further investigation with more modern VSD and CCD cameras may help discover more about how the spatial-temporal patterns of the auditory cortex convey information that can be related to sound source localization.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We want to express our undying gratitude to Dr. Sonya Bahar for her great help in preparing and editing the manuscript. We also thank Dr. Shuji Higashi and Dr. Hitoshi Inokawa (Division of Neurophysiology, Kyoto Prefecture University of Medicine) for valuable advice and technical support in all our experiments, Dr. Zador, Dr. Liberman and Dr. Hiken for their advice in writing the manuscript and Dr. Astafiev for his help in labor management.
References
- Bahar S., Suh M., Zhao M., Schwartz T. H. (2006). Intrinsic optical signal imaging of neocortical seizures: the “epileptic dip”. Neuroreport 17, 499–503 10.1097/01.wnr.0000209010.78599.f5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbour D. L., Wang X. (2002). Temporal coherence sensitivity in auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 2684–2699 10.1152/jn.00253.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonhoeffer T., Grinvald A. (1996). Optical imaging based on intrinsic signals: the methodology. In Brain Mapping: The Methods. Toga A. W., Mazziotta J. C., eds (San Diego, Academic Press; ), p. 55 [Google Scholar]
- Doan D. E., Saunders J. C. (1999). Sensitivity to simulated directional sound motion in the rat primary auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 2075–2087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eggermont J. (2006). Properties of correlated neural activity clusters in cat auditory cortex resemble those of neural assemblies. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 746–764 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehret G. (1997). The auditory cortex. J. Comp. Physiol. 181, 547–557 10.1007/s003590050139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furukawa S., Middlebrooks J. C. (2002). Cortical representation of auditory space: information-bearing features of spike patterns. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 1749–1762 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grinvald A., Shoham D., Shmuel A., Glaser D., Vanzetta I., Shtoyerman E., Slovin H., Sterkin A., Wijnbergen C., Hildesheim R., Arieli A. (2001). In Vivo Optical Imaging of Cortical Architecture and Dynamics. The Grodetsky Center for Research of Higher Brain Functions, the Weizman Institute of Science. Technical Report GC-AG/99-6.
- Hosokawa Y., Sugimoto S., Kubota M., Taniguchi I., Horikawa J. (2004). Optical imaging of binaural interaction in multiple fields of the guinea pig auditory cortex. Neuroreport 15, 1093–1097 10.1097/00001756-200405190-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inyushin M. Y., Volnova A. B., Lenkov D. N. (2001). Use of a simplified method of optical recording to identify foci of maximal neuron activity in the somatosensory cortex of white rats. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 31, 201–205 10.1023/A:1005272526284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins W. M., Merzenich M. M. (1984). Role of primary auditory cortex for sound-localization behavior. J. Neurophysiol. 52, 819–846 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalatsky V. A., Polley D. B., Merzenich M. M., Schreiner C. E., Stryker M. P. (2005). Fine functional organization of auditory cortex revealed by Fourier optical imaging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 13325–13330 10.1073/pnas.0505592102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaltenbach J. A., Zhang J. S. (2004). In vivo optical imaging of tone-evoked activity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus with a voltage sensitive dye. J. Neurosci. Res. 78, 908–917 10.1002/jnr.20339 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly J. B., Phillips D. P. (1991). Coding of interaural time differences of transients in auditory cortex of Rattus norvegicus: implications for the evolution of mammalian sound localization. Hear Res. 55, 39–44 10.1016/0378-5955(91)90089-R [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King A. J., Schnupp J. W. H., Doubell T. P. (2001). The shape of years to come: dynamic coding of auditory space. Trends. Cogn. Sci. 5, 261–270 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01660-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kummierek P., Malinowska M., Kowalska D. (2007). Different effects of lesions to auditory core and belt cortex on auditory recognition in dogs. Exp. Brain Res. 180, 491–508 10.1007/s00221-007-0868-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malhotra S., Hall A. J., Lomber S. J. (2004). Cortical control of sound localization in the cat: unilateral cooling deactivation of 19 cerebral areas. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 1625–1643 10.1152/jn.01205.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAlpine D., Grothe B. (2003). Sound localization and delay lines – do mammals fit the model? Trends Neurosci. 26, 347–350 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00140-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAlpine D., Jiang D., Palmer A. R. (2001). A neural code for low-frequency sound localization in mammals. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 396–401 10.1038/86049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mickey B. J., Middlebrooks J. C. (2005). Sensitivity of auditory cortical neurons to the locations of leading and lagging sounds. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 979–989 10.1152/jn.00580.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mrsic-Flogel T. D., King A. J., Schnupp J. W. H. (2005). Encoding of virtual acoustic space stimuli by neurons in ferret primary auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 93, 3489–3503 10.1152/jn.00748.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelken I., Bizley J. K., Nodal F. R., Ahmed B., Schnupp J. W. H., King A. J. (2004). Large-scale organization of ferret auditory cortex revealed using continuous acquisition of intrinsic signal. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 2574–2588 10.1152/jn.00276.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nishimura M., Shirasawa H., Kaizo H., Song W. J. (2007). New field with tonotopic organization in guinea pig auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 927–932 10.1152/jn.00689.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nishimura M., Shirasawa H., Song W. J. (2006). A light-emitting diode light source for imaging of neural activities with voltage-sensitive dyes. Neurosci. Res. 54, 230–234 10.1016/j.neures.2005.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ojima H., Takayanagi M., Potapov D., Homma R. (2005). Isofrequency band-like zones of activation revealed by optical imaging of intrinsic signals in the cat primary auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1497–1509 10.1093/cercor/bhi028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Proctor L., Konishi M. (1997) Representation of sound localization cues in the auditory thalamus of the barn owl. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 10421–10425 10.1073/pnas.94.19.10421 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sally S. L., Kelly J. B. (1988). Organization of auditory cortex in the albino rat: sound frequency. J. Neurophysiol. 59, 1627–1638 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith A. L., Parsons C. H., Lanyon R. G., Bizley J. K., Akerman C. J., Baker G. E., Dempster A. C., Thompson I. D., King A. J. (2004). An investigation of the role of auditory cortex in sound localization using muscimol-releasing Elvax. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 3059–3072 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03379.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song W. J., Kawaguchi H., Totoki S., Inoue Y., Katura T., Maeda S., Inagaki S., Shirasawa H., Nishimura M. (2006). Cortical intrinsic circuits can support activity propagation through an isofrequency strip of the guinea pig primary auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 16, 718–729 10.1093/cercor/bhj018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spitzer M. W., Takahashi T. T. (2006). Sound localization by barn owls in a simulated echoic environment. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 3571–3584 10.1152/jn.00982.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi H., Nakao M., Kaga K. (2004). Distributed representation of sound in the rat auditory cortex. Neuroreport 15, 2061–2065 10.1097/00001756-200409150-00013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takashima I., Kajiwara R., Iijima T. (2005). Voltage-sensitive dye imaging of intervibrissal fur-evoked activity in the rat somatosensory cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 381, 258–263 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.02.062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tokioka R., Kawaguchi H., Fukunishi K. (2000). Spatio-temporal analyses of stimulus evoked and spontaneous stochastic neural activity observed by optical imaging in guinea pig auditory cortex. Brain Res. 861, 271–280 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)01991-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tollin D. J. (2003). The lateral superior olive: a functional role in sound source localization. Neuroscientist 9, 127–143 10.1177/1073858403252228 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsytsarev V., Taketani M., Schottler F., Tanaka S., Hara M. A. (2006) New planar multielectrode array: recording from a rat auditory cortex. J. Neural. Eng. 3, 293–298 10.1088/1741-2560/3/4/006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsytsarev V., Tanaka S. (2002). Intrinsic optical signal from rat primary auditory cortex in response to sound stimuli presented to contralateral, ipsilateral and bilateral ears. Neuroreport 13, 1661–1665 10.1097/00001756-200209160-00019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsytsarev V., Yamazaki T., Ribot J., Tanaka S. (2004). Sound frequency representation in cat auditory cortex. NeuroImage 23, 1246–1255 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Versnel H., Mossop J. E., Mrsic-Flogel T. D., Ahmed B., Moore D. R. (2002). Optical imaging of intrinsic signals in ferret auditory cortex: responses to narrow-band sound stimuli. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 1545–1558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zador A. M. (2001). Synaptic connectivity and computation. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 1157–1158 10.1038/nn1201-1157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]