Abstract
Background
Whole-genome association studies in humans have enabled the unbiased discovery of new genes associated with human memory performance. However, such studies do not allow for a functional or causal testing of newly identified candidate genes. Since polymorphisms in Calsyntenin 2 (CLSTN2) showed a significant association with episodic memory performance in humans, we tested the C. elegans CLSTN2 ortholog CASY-1 for possible functions in the associative behavior of C. elegans.
Methodology/Principal Findings
Using three different associative learning paradigms and functional rescue experiments, we show that CASY-1 plays an important role during associative learning in C. elegans. Furthermore, neuronal expression of human CLSTN2 in C. elegans rescues the learning defects of casy-1 mutants. Finally, genetic interaction studies and neuron-specific expression experiments suggest that CASY-1 may regulate AMPA-like GLR-1 glutamate receptor signaling.
Conclusion/Significance
Our experiments demonstrate a remarkable conservation of the molecular function of Calsyntenins between nematodes and humans and point at a role of C. elegans casy-1 in regulating a glutamate receptor signaling pathway.
Introduction
The cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying learning and memory are the focus of intense research. Although many new components have been described that are conserved across different animal species, the exact mechanisms by which synaptic strength is regulated remain elusive [1] . Long-term potentiation (LTP) and depression (LTD), which are a key mechanisms underlying memory formation, involve plastic changes in synaptic strength through modulation of AMPA Glutamate receptor currents [2]. One frequently used mechanism by which neurons modulate synaptic strength is through the regulation of the number of neurotransmitter receptors at the surface of synapses [3]. Intracellular trafficking, exo- and endocytosis of receptors as well as surface dynamics also play important roles in regulating the exact number of receptors at the synapse [2], [4]. However, the exact mechanisms by which this is achieved are not completely understood.
Studies in both invertebrates and vertebrates have identified several genes and signaling pathways important for learning and memory. From this work it appears that many of the memory-related molecular mechanisms are conserved across different species. Despite the obvious differences in learning and memory tasks performed by different species and the anatomical differences between their nervous systems, recent human genetic studies suggest that genetic variability in the orthologs of related signaling molecules known from studies in model organisms contributes to inter-individual memory differences in humans [5]. Therefore, genes associated with human episodic memory identified in whole-genome association studies could provide new insights into the mechanisms underlying memory formation and storage.
Recently, an unbiased genome-wide screen for human hippocampus-dependent, episodic memory, which studied more than 500000 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), resulted in the identification of CLSTN2 (encoding calsyntenin 2) as a memory-related human gene [6]. Specifically, C allele carriers of a common T→C substitution within CLSTN2 had better episodic memory performance than TT genotype carriers in a verbal delayed recall task, which was performed by 341 Swiss young adults (median age 22 years). The better performance of the C allele carriers was observed 5 min and 24 h after learning, whereas immediate recall performance was similar between genotype groups, indicating that CLSTN2 is related to hippocampus-dependent memory performance and that the findings were not biased by possible differences in motivation, attention and working memory performance between groups. This association was not replicated in a second population of middle-aged participants from the US, which may be partially attributed to differences in ethnicity, in mean age between study populations, or in differences between cognitive tasks used [6]. However, a recent independent study in adolescents replicated the beneficial effect of the CLSTN2 C allele on verbal recall [7].
Even though there exists no direct equivalent of human episodic memory in the small nematode C. elegans, several forms of associative behaviour and long-term memory have been observed in this model organism [8]–[11]. For example, C. elegans is capable of pairing food deprivation sensation with olfactory cues [12], gustatory cues [9] and the temperature of its environment [11] by using different sensory neurons and integrating interneurons. Essentially, this type of learning is akin to some classical conditioning paradigms such as conditioned taste aversion (CTA) where an unconditioned stimulus (US) is paired with a conditioned stimulus (CS) [13]. Moreover, C. elegans is capable of distinguishing multiple cues based on past experience using a serotonin dependent mechanism [8]. Together with an easily modifiable genetic background and many available knock-out alleles, C. elegans allows a fast and systematic way to analyze genes implicated in associative memory.
Taking advantage of the fact that the C. elegans genome encodes only one CLSTN gene (casy-1) homologous to vertebrate CLSTN2 and that a knock-out allele is available, we show that casy-1 plays an important role in associative learning in both thermotaxis and chemotaxis conditioning paradigms. While this work was in progress, an independent study has identified casy-1 in a forward genetic screen for behavioural mutants [14]. In addition to the reported behavioural defects of casy-1 mutants, we show here that the pan-neuronal expression of human CLSTN2 rescues the chemotaxis conditioning defect of casy-1(tm718), thus demonstrating a strong conservation between CLSTN2 and casy-1 at the level of their molecular function. Finally, we describe a putative mechanism for CASY-1 in regulating associative behaviour via glutamate receptor signalling based on neuron-specific rescue experiments and on the genetic interaction between casy-1 and the glutamate receptor subunit glr-1.
Results and Discussion
The C. elegans genome encodes a single CLSTN2 ortholog casy-1
To test a causal relationship between CLSTN2 function and learning and memory, we searched the genomes of invertebrate model organisms for CLSTN2 orthologs. While vertebrate genomes typically encode three Calsyntenin family members, the genomes of invertebrates like Drosophila melanogaster and C. elegans contain only a single Calsyntenin gene (Fig. 1A). Protein sequence alignment of the three vertebrate Calsyntenin family members with the invertebrate Calsyntenins indicates that the single C. elegans homolog CASY-1 as well as Drosophila Calsyntenin are most similar to vertebrate CLSTN2 (Fig. 1A).
CLSTN2 is a type I transmembrane protein with two extracellular calcium-binding cadherin domains and two intracellular kinesin light chain-binding domains [15], [16]. These domains are conserved in all three Calsyntenin family members including C. elegans CASY-1 [15]. Similar to mammalian Calsyntenins, a transcriptional casy-1 reporter is expressed in many head nerve ring neurons, some of which send processes into the ventral nerve cord (Fig. 2A and data not shown). Moreover, a GFP-tagged CASY-1 protein was reported to localize at synapses (Duan and Hedgecock, personal communication). Given the sequence similarity between human CLSTN2 and C. elegans CASY-1 and their neuronal expression in both organisms, we asked whether the casy-1 gene might function in regulating associative learning in C. elegans. The casy-1 deletion mutant tm718 (kindly provided by S. Mitani) contains a 601 bp deletion in exon 4, creating a frameshift followed by a premature stop codon. The tm718 allele results in the production of a protein truncated at position 117 that lacks most of the extracellular and the entire intracellular domain. We observed no obvious anatomical, behavioral or locomotory defects in naive casy-1(tm718) animals (Fig. 1B,C). Moreover, casy-1(tm718) animals appear healthy and are fertile.
Behavioral defects in C. elegans casy-1 mutants
To test associative learning in C. elegans, we used three established context-dependent behavioral paradigms that are based on olfactory, gustatory and thermosensory starvation conditioning, respectively [11], [17]. The chemotaxis of naive casy-1(tm718) animals to three different volatile attractants was comparable to the response of the wild-type strain (Fig. 1B and Fig. S1). We thus investigated the olfactory associative learning capacity of casy-1(tm718) animals by testing their ability to reverse the attraction to an odorant after associating this odorant with a negative stimulus such as starvation (see methods). After starvation conditioning, the chemotaxis index (CI) of unconditioned controls and conditioned animals was compared in a quantitative chemotaxis assay [17]. Unconditioned wild-type and casy-1(tm718) animals both displayed strong chemotaxis to 0.1% diacetyl (DA, Fig. 1D), indicating that casy-1(tm718) mutants have no sensory defects in DA olfaction under these conditions. After a one hour starvation period in the presence of DA, wild-type animals did not show any attraction to DA, while casy-1(tm718) mutants were still significantly attracted by DA (CI = 0.3, p<0.01 using a Student t-test, 6 replicates repeated three times), albeit less efficiently than unconditioned control animals (Fig. 1D). The behavioral difference between wild-type and casy-1(tm718) animals is not due to a defect in food detection, since we observed normal slowing of casy-1(tm718) locomotion compared to wild-type, when animals were deprived of food and replaced on a new bacterial lawn (Fig.1E) [18].
To investigate the possibility that the chemotactic association defect of casy-1(tm718) could be due to adaptation (i.e. a decrement in response due to sensory fatigue that cannot be dishabituated [19]) rather than to an associative learning defect, we pre-exposed both strains to concentrated DA in the presence of abundant food before measuring their CI to 0.1% DA (gray bars in Fig. 1F). DA-adapted wild–type and casy-1(tm718) animals showed a similar partial reduction in their CI to DA , indicating that casy-1(tm718) mutants can adapt to high concentrations of DA. We thus conclude that a loss of casy-1 function predominantly reduces associative learning without significantly impairing olfactory adaptation.
Next, we tested the performance of casy-1(tm718) mutants in an “gustatory” NaCl chemotaxis conditioning paradigm [9]. Wild-type worms display a strong attraction to 25 mM NaCl that is reversed when worms are first starved in the presence of NaCl in liquid cultures for 1 hour (Fig. 3A) [9]. Unconditioned casy-1(tm718) worms displayed a chemotaxis index (CI) that was similar to naive wild-type animals. However, when starved in the presence of NaCl casy-1(tm718) mutants did not show an aversion but only a partial decline in their attraction towards NaCl (Fig. 3A).
To test the associative behavior in the context of a third sensory system, we examined the performance of casy-1(tm718) mutants in a thermotaxis conditioning paradigm. Wild-type animals typically migrate towards the temperature at which they had been previously fed, but they avoid this temperature after a 3 hour starvation period [11]. We used a modified version of this conditioning paradigm by training groups of worms at specific temperatures and placing them on thin agar plates with a steep temperature gradient to measure their Thermotaxis Index (TTI) [20]. Wild-type worms grown at 15°C showed a TTI close to zero after 3 hours of starvation conditioning at 15°C, whereas casy-1(tm718) animals continued to exhibit significant albeit reduced thermotaxis to 15°C after starvation conditioning at this temperature (Fig. 3B).
In conclusion, casy-1(tm718) mutants exhibit strong associative learning defects in the context of three different sensory stimuli with no sensory impairment of the naive animals when compared to wild-type. These results point at a central function of CASY-1 in promoting associative learning downstream of different sensory stimuli.
Expression of human CLSTN2 rescues the behavioral defects of casy-1 mutants
To confirm that the olfactory and thermotaxis association defects observed in tm718 animals are due to the loss of casy-1 function, we introduced a casy-1 minigene composed of 5 kb of 5′ regulatory sequences fused to 3 kb cDNA of the long casy-1 isoform (B0034.3a) and 3 kb of 3′ non-coding sequences into casy-1(tm718) animals. A transgenic line carrying the casy-1 minigene on an extrachromosomal array (zhE242.1[casy-1 minigene]) was tested in the olfactory and thermotaxis conditioning paradigms. We calculated a learning index (%LI) as the difference between the CI or TTI of unconditioned and conditioned animals divided by the CI or TTI, respectively, of the unconditioned animals [8] (see methods). In both paradigms, the transgenic animals showed significant rescue of the %LI, while their non-transgenic siblings (casy-1(tm718) sibs without array) that were simultaneously scored on the same assay plates exhibited behavioral defects comparable to the parental casy-1(tm718) strain (Fig. 4A, B).
To test the functional conservation between human CLSTN2 and C. elegans CASY-1 at the molecular level, we expressed human CLSTN2 cDNA under the control of the pan-neuronal unc-119 promoter and with the C. elegans casy-1 3′UTR in place of the CLSTN2 3′UTR in casy-1(tm718) mutants and measured the %LI of CLSTN2 transgenic animals using the olfactory conditioning assay. All three transgenic lines that were tested showed a significant rescue of the behavioral defects (Fig. 4C). Control transgenic animals carrying the unc-119 promoter-casy-1 3′UTR vector lacking the CLSTN2 cDNA insert exhibited no significant increase in the %LI when compared to non-transgenic casy-1(tm718) animals (Fig. S2). Thus, human CLSTN2 can functionally replace C. elegans CASY-1 in an associative learning paradigm.
CASY-1 acts in a GLR-1 Glutamate receptor pathway
Human CLSTN1 and CLSTN2 form a complex with the MINT2/X-11-like neuronal adaptor protein and kinesin light chain (KLC1), suggesting a function for CLSTNs in the transport or sorting of synaptic vesicles [15], [16], [21]. Since mutations in the C. elegans ortholog of Mint2 (lin-10) cause defects in the clustering of the AMPA-type glutamate receptor subunit GLR-1 at the synapses of ventral cord interneurons and LIN-10 can bind to the PDZ binding motif at the C-terminus of GLR-1 [22], we hypothesized that CASY-1 might regulate the synaptic function or transport of GLR-1. Even though we did not observe a significant mislocalization of a translational GLR-1::GFP reporter in ventral cord motorneurons of casy-1(tm718) mutants (data not shown), glr-1(n2461) mutants showed similar association defects in the olfactory conditioning assays as casy-1(tm718) mutants (Fig. 5A). Notably, GLR-1 has been previously shown to be important for olfactory assocation and critical for long-term memory in C. elegans [23], [24]. To test a possible function of CASY-1 in a GLR-1 signaling pathway, we examined the genetic interaction between casy-1(tm718) and glr-1(n2461). We found no further reduction in the %LI in the casy-1(tm718); glr-1(n2461) double loss-of-function mutant compared to either single mutant, suggesting that casy-1 and glr-1 may act in the same pathway regulating olfactory conditioning (Fig. 5A). We thus tested if increased levels of GLR-1 could rescue the behavioral defects of casy-1(tm718) mutants. For this purpose, we introduced a rescuing multicopy extrachromosomal array containing a 6 kb fragment spanning the glr-1 locus (zhEx243.1[glr-1(+)]) into the casy-1(tm718) background. casy-1(tm718); zhEx243.1[glr-1(+)] animals showed a similar %LI in the olfactory conditioning assay as wild-type animals (Fig. 5A). Thus, increasing the GLR-1 gene dosage can compensate for the behavioral defects of casy-1(tm718) mutants, suggesting that CASY-1 positively regulates GLR-1 signaling during olfactory conditioning.
Some of the head neurons expressing the casy-1 transcriptional reporter also expressed the glr-1::gfp reporter (Fig. 2B). Strongest co-expression was seen in the RMDD, SMDD, RMD and SMDV motor/interneurons that regulate head turning, and weaker casy-1p::rfp expression was observed in the glr-1-positive AVE command interneurons (not visible in Fig. 2B). We therefore tested if expression of casy-1 under control of the glr-1 promoter was sufficient to rescue the olfactory learning defects of casy-1 (tm718) mutants. In two out of four lines tested, the DA starvation conditioning defect was completely and in the remaining two lines weakly rescued (Fig. 5B and Table S1). Thus, casy-1 acts at least in part in glr-1 positive neurons during olfactory associative learning. It is interesting to note that Ikeda et al.[14] found that during salt chemotaxis conditioning, expression of casy-1 in glr-1 positive neurons was not sufficient to rescue the associative learning defects. Accordingly, a recent study by Kano et al. [25] showed that associative learning as well as short-term memory using the salt chemotaxis conditioning paradigm are not glr-1 dependent. Thus, casy-1 may perform another, glr-1 independent function during gustatory (salt) chemotaxis learning, as casy-1 may act in multiple, distinct pathways depending on the type of sensory inputs that need to be associated with the starvation signal.
Conclusions
In summary, our study reveals an important role of C. elegans casy-1 calsyntenin in associative learning in response to different environmental stimuli. It should be noted that in all the association assays shown, the behavior of conditioned casy-1(tm718) mutants still significantly differed from the naive controls (i.e. the %LI of casy-1 mutants was always greater than 0), indicating that loss of casy-1 function does not result in a complete loss of all associative behavior. Thus, there must exist multiple parallel pathways controlling associative learning in C. elegans. For example, components of the insulin signaling pathway have been implicated in salt chemotaxis learning, and casy-1 was found to act in parallel to the insulin pathway during salt chemotaxis learning [14].
Finally, we demonstrate that the molecular function of human CLSTN2 and C. elegans CASY-1 is conserved, as human Calsyntenin-2 can functionally replace CASY-1 during olfactory learning. Given the relatively large evolutionary distance between these two species and the anatomical dissimilarity of their nervous systems, this degree of conservation at the molecular level is remarkable. Thus, Calsyntenin might be a key component of conserved molecular pathways regulating different aspects of learning and memory in diverse species.
Methods
Strains were maintained and grown according to standard procedures [26]. Wild-type refers to C. elegans Bristol, variety N2. casy-1(tm718) mutants were kindly provided by the Mitani Lab and backcrossed three times before use in all assays. All transgenic animals were generated by microinjection of the indicated DNAs into the syncytial gonads as described. Alleles and transgenes used: LGI: unc-29(x29) (kind gift of A.V. Maricq); LGII: casy-1(tm718); LGIII: glr-1(n2461); LGV: acr-16(ok789); transgenes: zhEx242.1[casy-1 minigene; sur-5::gfp], zhEx243.1[glr-1(+), lin-48 ::gfp], zhEx282.1 to 282.3[unc-119p::CLSTN2::casy-1 3′UTR, sur-5::gfp], zhEx285.1 to zhEx285.3[unc-119p::no insert::casy-1 3′UTR, sur-5::gfp], zhEx245[casy-1p::rfp], nuIs24[glr-1::gfp], Ex[glr-1p::casy-1].
PCR fusion constructs
All DNA fragments were amplified using proof reading polymerase from C. elegans genomic DNA or total N2 cDNA. Individual fragments were fused by PCR fusion [27]. A 6 kb genomic glr-1 fragment was amplified (forward: 5′-ccggtcatacgggagataga-3′, reverse: 5′- taaattttcctgggggcttc-3′) to generate zhEx243.1. 5 kb of the 5′ UTR region of casy-1 (forward outer: 5′- ggatattggtcaccttcccta-3′, nested forward: 5′- ttctagattattctgacaaccatttg-3′, reverse: 5′-cgagcagcatggtgatgtttg-3′) were fused to 2995 bp casy-1 cDNA (B0034.3a, 5′ fusion primer: 5′-actcacgcacacaaaaccaatcatgcgaactgcgtactttatttttgtc-3′, reverse: 5′- ggagggagtcatgaatgttga-3′) and 1.6 kb of 3′UTR (forward 3′UTR: 5′-gttcgtttgacaagccgttt-3′, nested forward 3′UTR: 5′- agccgtttggtttttcaatg-3′, cDNA fusion primer: 5′- aattccttcaggcatgttgc-3′). This PCR construct was used together with the transformation marker sur-5::gfp to generate zhEx242.1. Details on the construction of the glr-1p::casy-1, the casy-1p:.rfp and the unc-119p::CLSTN2::casy-1 3′UTR rescue and control (without insert) constructs are available upon request.
Olfactory conditioning
All assays were conducted with 50–200 well-fed synchronized young adult worms, using 10 cm Petri CTX agar dishes (2% agar, 5 mM KPO4 pH = 6.0, 1 mM CaCl2, 1 mM MgSO4). Except for agar composition, chemotaxis assays were performed as described previously [17]. Adaptation and starvation conditioning assays were performed as previously described [12], except that animals were washed three times with M9 buffer (22 mM KH2PO4, 22 mM Na2HPO4, 85 mM NaCl, 1 mM MgSO4) for 20 min each, resulting in 1 hour pre-starvation before the olfactory conditioning was performed.
NaCl conditioning
Salt chemotaxis and salt chemotaxis learning assays were assessed as described before with some modifications [28], [29],. Briefly, synchronized and well-fed young adult nematodes were washed 3 times in CTX buffer. 100–200 worms were placed at the intersection of a four-quadrant CTX plate to test chemotaxis and liquid was removed with a tissue paper. Chemotaxis plates were prepared one day in advance. Pairs of opposite quadrants of four-quadrant Petri plates (Falcon X plate, Becton Dickinson Labware) were filled with 16 ml buffered agar (2% agar, 5 mM KPO4 pH 6, 1 mM CaCl2 and 1 mM MgSO4), either containing 25 mM NaCl or not. Adjacent quadrants were connected with a thin layer of molten agar 1 h before the assay. The chemotaxis index was calculated 10 min after the worms were placed on the CTX plates: (A–C)/ total number of worms), where A is the number of worms at the quadrants with, and C is the number of worms at the quadrants without NaCl.
For NaCl chemotaxis learning assays, the collected nematodes were transferred after the washing procedure into 30 ml CTX buffer containing 20 mM NaCl for 1 h at room temperature [14], and chemotaxis was tested immediately afterwards. All experiments were performed in triplicates at least three times.
Thermotaxis conditioning
We created a thermotaxis setup as described previously using a steep thermal gradient on a thin agar plate [20]. A 2–3 mm thick CTX agar plate 130 mm long 90 mm wide was rested on heated and cooled metal blocks, respectively, such that 13°C was measured at one end and 33° at the other end of the plate. 200–400 worms were spotted along the 22°C isothermic line measured shortly before applying the worms. The worms were then left to migrate for 45 min. At the end of the assay, the plate was separated into a cold region and a warm region along the 22°C isothermic line, and the worms were immediately counted to determine the TTI as described [20].
Microscopy
For the image shown in Fig. 2, animals were anesthetized with 10 mM NaN3 and mounted in M9 buffer on 3% agarose pads. Optical sections through the left hemisphere were recorded on a Leica SP2 confocal microscope using a 63× N.A. 1.4 objective and a z-step size of 0.73 µm. 3D reconstructions were generated using the volocity 2.3. software package (Improvision) and a lateral view is shown.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We wish to thank all lab members for critical discussion of this work. We are grateful to Attila Stetak for help with primer design and behavioral assays, the Maricq, Mitani and Kaplan labs and the C. elegans Genetics Center for strains and the Fire lab for plasmid vectors and Gert Jansen for help with the salt chemotaxis assays.
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: This work was supported by grants from the Swiss National Science Foundation to A. P., D.Q. and A. H. and by the Kanton of Zürich. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Kandel ER, Abrams T, Bernier L, Carew TJ, Hawkins RD, et al. Classical conditioning and sensitization share aspects of the same molecular cascade in Aplysia. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol. 1983;48 Pt 2:821–830. doi: 10.1101/sqb.1983.048.01.085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Malinow R, Malenka RC. AMPA receptor trafficking and synaptic plasticity. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2002;25:103–126. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Groc L, Choquet D. AMPA and NMDA glutamate receptor trafficking: multiple roads for reaching and leaving the synapse. Cell Tissue Res. 2006;326:423–438. doi: 10.1007/s00441-006-0254-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Choquet D, Triller A. The role of receptor diffusion in the organization of the postsynaptic membrane. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:251–265. doi: 10.1038/nrn1077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.de Quervain DJ, Papassotiropoulos A. Identification of a genetic cluster influencing memory performance and hippocampal activity in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:4270–4274. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0510212103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Papassotiropoulos A, Stephan DA, Huentelman MJ, Hoerndli FJ, Craig DW, et al. Common Kibra alleles are associated with human memory performance. Science. 2006;314:475–478. doi: 10.1126/science.1129837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Jacobsen LK, Picciotto MR, Heath CJ, Mencl WE, Gelernter J. Allelic Variation of Calsyntenin 2 (CLSTN2) Modulates the Impact of Developmental Tobacco Smoke Exposure on Mnemonic Processing in Adolescents. Biol Psychiatry. 2008 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Zhang Y, Lu H, Bargmann CI. Pathogenic bacteria induce aversive olfactory learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature. 2005;438:179–184. doi: 10.1038/nature04216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Saeki S, Yamamoto M, Iino Y. Plasticity of chemotaxis revealed by paired presentation of a chemoattractant and starvation in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. J Exp Biol. 2001;204:1757–1764. doi: 10.1242/jeb.204.10.1757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Rankin CH, Beck CD, Chiba CM. Caenorhabditis elegans: a new model system for the study of learning and memory. Behav Brain Res. 1990;37:89–92. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(90)90074-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mohri A, Kodama E, Kimura KD, Koike M, Mizuno T, et al. Genetic control of temperature preference in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 2005;169:1437–1450. doi: 10.1534/genetics.104.036111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Colbert HA, Smith TL, Bargmann CI. OSM-9, a novel protein with structural similarity to channels, is required for olfaction, mechanosensation, and olfactory adaptation in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurosci. 1997;17:8259–8269. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08259.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Spear NE, Kucharski D, Hoffmann H. Contextual influences on conditioned taste aversions in the developing rat. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1985;443:42–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb27062.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Ikeda DD, Duan Y, Matsuki M, Kunitomo H, Hutter H, et al. CASY-1, an ortholog of calsyntenins/alcadeins, is essential for learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:5260–5265. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0711894105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Konecna A, Frischknecht R, Kinter J, Ludwig A, Steuble M, et al. Calsyntenin-1 docks vesicular cargo to kinesin-1. Mol Biol Cell. 2006;17:3651–3663. doi: 10.1091/mbc.E06-02-0112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hintsch G, Zurlinden A, Meskenaite V, Steuble M, Fink-Widmer K, et al. The calsyntenins–a family of postsynaptic membrane proteins with distinct neuronal expression patterns. Mol Cell Neurosci. 2002;21:393–409. doi: 10.1006/mcne.2002.1181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Nuttley WM, Atkinson-Leadbeater KP, Van Der Kooy D. Serotonin mediates food-odor associative learning in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:12449–12454. doi: 10.1073/pnas.192101699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Sawin ER, Ranganathan R, Horvitz HR. C. elegans locomotory rate is modulated by the environment through a dopaminergic pathway and by experience through a serotonergic pathway. Neuron. 2000;26:619–631. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81199-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Bernhard N, van der Kooy D. A behavioral and genetic dissection of two forms of olfactory plasticity in Caenorhabditis elegans: adaptation and habituation. Learn Mem. 2000;7:199–212. doi: 10.1101/lm.7.4.199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Cassata G, Kuhn F, Witmer A, Kirchhofer R, Burglin TR. A steep thermal gradient thermotaxis assay for the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Genesis. 2000;27:141–144. doi: 10.1002/1526-968x(200008)27:4<141::aid-gene20>3.0.co;2-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Araki Y, Tomita S, Yamaguchi H, Miyagi N, Sumioka A, et al. Novel cadherin-related membrane proteins, Alcadeins, enhance the X11-like protein-mediated stabilization of amyloid beta-protein precursor metabolism. J Biol Chem. 2003;278:49448–49458. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M306024200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Rongo C, Whitfield CW, Rodal A, Kim SK, Kaplan JM. LIN-10 is a shared component of the polarized protein localization pathways in neurons and epithelia. Cell. 1998;94:751–759. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81734-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Rose JK, Kaun KR, Chen SH, Rankin CH. GLR-1, a non-NMDA glutamate receptor homolog, is critical for long-term memory in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurosci. 2003;23:9595–9599. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-29-09595.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Morrison GE, van der Kooy D. A mutation in the AMPA-type glutamate receptor, glr-1, blocks olfactory associative and nonassociative learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115:640–649. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.115.3.640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kano T, Brockie PJ, Sassa T, Fujimoto H, Kawahara Y, et al. Memory in Caenorhabditis elegans is mediated by NMDA-type ionotropic glutamate receptors. Curr Biol. 2008;18:1010–1015. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.05.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Brenner S. The genetics of Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 1974;77:71–94. doi: 10.1093/genetics/77.1.71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hobert O. PCR fusion-based approach to create reporter gene constructs for expression analysis in transgenic C. elegans. Biotechniques. 2002;32:728–730. doi: 10.2144/02324bm01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wicks SR, de Vries CJ, van Luenen HG, Plasterk RH. CHE-3, a cytosolic dynein heavy chain, is required for sensory cilia structure and function in Caenorhabditis elegans. Dev Biol. 2000;221:295–307. doi: 10.1006/dbio.2000.9686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hukema RK, Rademakers S, Dekkers MP, Burghoorn J, Jansen G. Antagonistic sensory cues generate gustatory plasticity in Caenorhabditis elegans. Embo J. 2006;25:312–322. doi: 10.1038/sj.emboj.7600940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.