Skip to main content
Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment logoLink to Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment
. 2007 Dec;3(6):949–954. doi: 10.2147/ndt.s2234

Effects of psycho-educational training and stimulant medication on visual perceptual skills in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

Antigone S Papavasiliou 1,, Irene Nikaina 1, Ioanna Rizou 1, Stratos Alexandrou 1
PMCID: PMC2656339  PMID: 19300632

Abstract

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is treated with stimulants and psycho-educational remedial programs despite limited literature support for the latter. This study aimed to examine changes in a “Test of Visual Perceptual Skills” (TVPS) that has not been previously reported in children with ADHD enrolled in such a program.

Methods

Sixteen children, 7–11 years old, with ADHD were involved in occupational therapy and special education geared towards attention training. Six months later methylphenidate 1 mg/kg/day was prescribed. It was not taken by eight children because of family choice. The TVPS was given twice, upon diagnosis, and 8 months post-intervention. The groups were compared by a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with medication as a between groups factor and test-retest scores as within factor.

Results

All children demonstrated increases in total scores in the second measurement. Medicated children scored higher but ANOVA showed a nonsignificant F for the two groups, medicated and unmedicated (F = 0.0031, p = 0.9563), indicating a nondifferential effect of the two levels of treatment. It revealed a significant F for the pre- and post-treatment total TVPS scores (F = 30.91, p < 0.0001) indicating a significant difference between pre- and post-treatment tests. The interaction between pre-post treatment and level of treatment (medicated–unmedicated) was nonsignificant (F = 2.20, p = 0.1604).

Conclusion

TVPS scores improved in all children following intervention. Medicated children did better, but differences were nonsignificant.

Keywords: ADHD, stimulants, psycho-educational therapy, TVPS

Introduction

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most frequent chronic conditions of childhood, occurring in 4%–12% of school age children (AAP 2001); it has a significant impact on child mental health and education. Accurate diagnosis, measurement of impairment, and monitoring of symptoms overtime are important for successful management. The treatment of ADHD includes stimulants and/or psycho-educational interventions. Stimulants are effective in controlling ADHD core symptoms in 70%–80% of the patients; these are considered the safest of all the medications utilized in this condition (Olfson et al 2003). The evidence supporting medication-based interventions is strong; consensus treatment algorithms to guide the multimodal treatment of ADHD, alone and in combination with common co-morbidities, have been discussed (Remschmidt 2005). Methylphenidate, the most frequently used stimulant, gives better motivation and drive than the others making patients work more intensely; the duration of its effect is short, but the breadth of its impact is impressive (Conners 2002). Treatment with stimulants is more effective in the reduction of ADHD symptoms than an intensive program of behavioral and cognitive intervention; the combination of stimulants with psychosocial interventions offers few advantages over medication alone (Santosh and Taylor 2000). The Multimodal Treatment Study concluded that careful medication management with good follow-up is more effective than intensive behavioral treatment (The MTA cooperative group 1999); combination of medication and intensive behavioral treatment was also significantly superior to psychosocial treatments alone in reducing ADHD symptoms. Nevertheless, many societies and child health specialists prefer to rely on psycho-educational interventions such as occupational therapy, special education, and psychotherapy, rather than stimulants, despite the cost and the time investment involved (Nianiou et al 2003). The literature provides little support for expanded psycho-educational treatment options and stresses the need for well-designed rigorous studies (AAP 2001). Neuropsychological evaluation of children with ADHD is often requested on diagnosis along with behavioral rating scales for parents and teachers (Biederman et al 2004). However, measuring progress of children involved in treatment is not routinely done. The reason for this could be related to assessment difficulties; lengthy testing sessions are counterproductive because attention problems interfere with the process and create difficulties in the interpretation of incorrect results. Testing for treatment response in individual children with ADHD has not been adequately evaluated (Mollica et al 2004). The clinical effectiveness of stimulant medications is no longer in doubt but the efficacy of psycho-educational interventions and the patterns of changes in ADHD need to be explored. Perceptual and spatial disorders have been much less investigated than other domains. Visual perception is the capacity to interpret or give meaning to what is seen; it includes recognition, insight and interpretation of what is seen, at the higher levels of the central nervous system (Buktenica 1968). Visual perceptual skills have not been studied in children with ADHD. They have been investigated in children with reading problems and have been found to probably contribute to them (Griffin et al 1993) as well as in children with cerebral palsy (Menken et al 1987; Stiers et al 2002).

The objective of this investigation was to examine changes in a TVPS that has not been previously utilized in children with ADHD, before and after intervention with individualized psycho-educational training and stimulants. We hypothesized that all children were going to have significant changes in their TVPS scores in the second time of measurement.

Methods

Patients

Sixteen children, 13 boys and 3 girls, 7–11 years of age (mean age 8 years and 3 months) were enrolled in the study. The patients were included on the basis of fulfilling DSM-IV ADHD diagnostic criteria (APA 1994) and being students of regular schools. They were all considered good candidates for stimulant medication. Comorbidity with other conditions commonly seen in children with ADHD, resource class, or other type of tutoring within the school system were not exclusion criteria. According to our treatment protocol for children with ADHD, all of them were referred for comprehensive psycho-educational testing and intervention prior to the initiation of stimulant treatment. After the testing was completed they became involved in a 5-hour/week remedial program of occupational therapy and special education that was carried out by the same team of occupational therapists, special educators, and clinical psychologists. The psycho-educational training was individually tailored to each child’s needs and flexible enough to accommodate for changes. Occupational therapy was geared towards attention training; it was based on therapeutic principles of motor-perceptual training, cognitive-perceptual training, and sensory integration. Special education included psycho-paedagogic sessions based on behavioral techniques; additionally, an educational program in accordance to the grade level of the child was followed. After 6 months of psycho-educational activities methylphenidate was prescribed at a dose of 1 mg/kg/day, divided in 3 daily doses, to be given at 8 am, 12 noon, and 4 pm. Eight children (6 boys and 2 girls, mean age 7 years and 11 months) took the medication as prescribed and 8 (7 boys and 1 girl, mean age 8 years and 6 months) did not, because the families chose not to proceed with medication due to their own fears and beliefs regarding stimulant use. All children regardless of medication status continued the psycho-educational intervention.

Measures

The TVPS (nonmotor) Revised (TVPS-R) was utilized (Gardner 1996). This test consists of seven subtests: 1. Visual discrimination, which refers to a subject’s ability to match or determine exact characteristics of two forms when one of the forms is among similar forms. 2. Visual memory, which refers to a subject’s ability to remember for immediate recall (after 4–5 seconds) all of the characteristics of a given form, and being able to find this form from an array of similar forms. 3. Visual spatial-relationships, which refers to a subject’s ability to determine the correct direction of forms (spatial relations). 4. Visual form-constancy, which refers to a subject’s ability to see a form, and being able to find it, even though the form is rotated, reversed, or hidden among others. 5. Visual sequential-memory, which refers to a subject’s ability to remember a number of forms in a series. 6. Visual figure-ground, which refers to a subject’s ability to perceive a form visually, and to find this form hidden in a conglomerated ground of matter. 7. Visual closure, which refers to a subject’s ability to determine, from among 4 incomplete forms, the one that would be the same as the completed one.

TVPS-R measures to what extent a subject (4–13 years) can perform in the above subtests. Directions require a minimum amount of language and can be given in any language. In fact it is described as a test nonbiased for culture, gender, and education. From the original raw scores determined for each subtest the following derived scores are obtained: Standard scores for each age level, percentile ranks, and perceptual ages. These derived scores allow comparisons of the subjects’ performance to that of normal children of the same age-group. The test was given twice; first, upon diagnosis and prior to any intervention and subsequently, eight months after occupational therapy and special education were initiated. In the medicated group the test was carried out one hour after methylphenidate administration.

Statistical analysis

In our study we had two experimental conditions (medicated and nonmedicated children) and different subjects have been used in each condition. In order to compare the mean total scores, an analysis of variance has been performed. Because we had a pre-treatment and a post-treatment test and one experimental condition with two levels (medicated and nonmedicated) we performed a two-way analysis of variance (mixed model) with one between variable having two levels (a. education plus medication and b. education without medication) and one within variable also having two levels (a. pre-treatment test and b. post-treatment test). The dependent variable was the total TVPS scores which was composed of 7 sub-scores.

Results

In Table 1 the age, sex, ADHD type, co-morbidity, and medication status of the children are listed. The comprehensive psycho-educational testing that preceded the remedial treatment program revealed that in addition to the ADHD, four children had borderline intelligence, six were dyspraxic, and three dysgraphic. Three children did not demonstrate associated co-morbidity.

Table 1.

Age, sex, ADHD type, co-morbidity, and medication status of the children

Patient number Age (months) Sex ADHD type Co-morbidity Medication status
1 84 F Combined Dysgraphia Methylphenidate
2 103 M Combined Borderline Intelligence Methylphenidate
3 93 M Combined Borderline Intelligence Methylphenidate
4 93 M Combined Dyspraxia Methylphenidate
5 115 M Combined Methylphenidate
6 103 M Combined Dyspraxia Methylphenidate
7 87 F Combined Dysgraphia Methylphenidate
8 84 M Combined Methylphenidate
9 123 M Combined No
10 99 M Combined Dyspraxia No
11 119 M Combined Dyspraxia No
12 88 M Combined Dysgraphia No
13 92 F Combined Borderline Intelligence No
14 120 M Combined Dyspraxia No
15 94 M Combined Dyspraxia No
16 96 M Combined Borderline Intelligence No

Abbreviation: ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

After all data were collected and the standard scores were obtained, we performed descriptive statistics on each variable. Table 2 indicates the means, standard deviations, and ranges of the total and sub-category scores in both tests, in medicated and nonmedicated patients. As we can see, both medicated and nonmedicated children appear to have an increase in their score means in the second time of measurement with the exception of closure category in the nonmedicated group where a small decrease was noticed (from mean 5.375 ± 3.02 to mean 4.25 ± 3.732).

Table 2.

Means, standard deviations, and ranges of the total and sub-category TVPS scores in tests in medicated and nonmedicated patients

Pre-test
Post-test
Medicated Nonmedicated Medicated Nonmedicated
Mean ± SD (range) Mean ± SD (range) Mean ± SD (range) Mean ± SD (range)
Total TVPS score 39.25 ± 21.829 (12–72) 43.125 ± 24.098 (7–71) 60.625 ± 25.690 (30–105) 55.5 ± 20.853 (15–87)
Discrimination 5.875 ± 4.086 (1–12) 7 ± 5.01 (1–14) 8.375 ± 5.370 (1–16) 9.5 ± 3.854 (1–13)
Memory 5 ± 3.891 (1–213) 7.625 ± 4.479 (1–13) 9.875 ± 4.190 (2–15) 8 ± 4.175 (2–14)
Spatial Relationships 6.375 ± 6.116 (1–17) 7. 25 ± 5.09 (1–13) 9 ± 7.091 (1–18) 10.25 ± 4.301 (1–14)
Form Constancy 5.5 ± 3.464 (2–12) 6 ± 3.464 (1–11) 8.375 ± 5.041 (1–16) 9.25 ± 2.964 (5–13)
Sequential Memory 5.25 ± 4.097 (1–12) 5.6 ± 4.44 (1–13) 7.625 ± 4.34 (2–14) 6.25 ± 3.196 (1–12)
Figure ground 7.25 ± 3.575 (1–11) 5.375 ± 4.406 (1–14) 10.125 ± 3.482 (5–16) 8 ± 3.817 (1–13)
Closure 4 ± 2.878 (1–9) 5.375 ± 3.02 (1–8) 7.25 ± 3.327 (3–13) 4.25 ± 3.732 (1–12)

Abbreviations: SD, standard deviation; TVPS, Test of Visual Perceptual Skills.

Subsequently, in order to compare all groups a 2X2 mixed analysis of variance was performed with medication as a between groups factor and test-retest scores as within factor. At first, a t-test was performed for the purpose of detecting any differences between the total TVPS scores of the two groups (medicated and nonmedicated) in the pre-treatment condition. The t-test value was nonsignificant (t = 0.0031, p = 0.741), indicating that the two groups (medicated and nonmedicated) were equivalent. The analysis of variance (Table 3) resulted in a nonsignificant F for the two groups, medicated and nonmedicated (F = 0.0031, p = 0.9563), indicating that there was no differential effect of the two levels of treatment. Furthermore, the analysis of variance revealed a significant F for the pre-treatment and the post-treatment total TVPS scores (F = 30.91, p < 0.0001) indicating that there was a significant difference between the pre-treatment and the post-treatment tests. The interaction between pre-post treatment and level of treatment (medicated – nonmedicated) was nonsignificant (F = 2.20, p = 0.1604). As we can observe from the mean values on Table 2; both groups (medicated and nonmedicated) started with an equivalent total score on TVPS and, in the post-treatment test, both groups improved considerably, and the medicated children did better. The sample was smaller than required in order to allow for analysis of the subcategory scores.

Table 3.

Two by two mixed analysis of variance

Source of variation SS df MS F Prob > F
Between subjects variable
Medication status 3.125 1 3.125 0.0031 0.9563
Subjects (within medication status) 14032.375 14 1002.312
Within subjects variable
Pre-test/Post-test 2278.125 1 2278.125 30.91 0.0001
Interaction 162 1 162 2.20 0.1604
Residual 1031.875 14 73.705
Total 17507.5 31 564.758

Discussion

The findings of this study supported our initial hypothesis; all children enrolled in intervention had significant changes in the TVPS total scores in the second time of measurement. This was noted irrespective of medication status with one exception in the score means in closure category in the non-medicated group. These improvements were not merely the result of normal development because the analysis was done on standard scores for each age level that allowed comparisons of the subjects’ performance to that of normal children of the same age-group. The aim of this study was not to examine the effect of the stimulants versus that of the remedial program and it was not anticipated that half of the subjects were going to be retested without medication. For this reason a control group was not included and retesting of the medicated children prior to stimulant onset was not planned. It was subsequently ascertained that medicated and nonmedicated children were not significantly different at the outset through a t-test on the pretreatment TVPS scores. Comorbidity with other conditions commonly seen in children with ADHD was present in the majority of the subjects. It was not an exclusion criterion as this is very common, occurring in over 50% of patients; in fact, ADHD overlaps with other disorders at an extremely high rate (Gillberg 2003). Four patients were found to be of borderline intelligence at the initial evaluation. IQ testing may not be reliable in untreated children with ADHD because of inattention and it is often underestimated; moreover, it has been shown that visual perception impairment and intelligence impairment may coexist as two separate and irreducible deficits (Stiers 1999). For these reasons we did not exclude these children from the study.

The importance for a consensus on the processes that are necessary in monitoring children with ADHD after a treatment plan has been established, has been previously stressed (Foy and Earls 2005). The TVPS was chosen as an outcome measure because it is brief and easy to administer, important factors when assessing children with ADHD. It is frequently utilized by occupational therapists to monitor the progress of children involved in training. The test-retest reliability of the total TVPS scores has been demonstrated in learning disabled children (McFall et al 1993; Burtner et al 2002) and in Chinese preschoolers (Chan and Chow 2005). In contrast, the test-retest reliability estimates scores on the subtests were low and should be used with extreme caution. In the present study the TVPS was useful in measuring the progress of children with ADHD following a treatment program.

How can we explain the improvement of visual perceptual skills when applying techniques that enhance attention? Attention has a critical role in information processing. Its adequate functioning is required for the development of cognitive abilities including the complex ones involved in scholastic progress. As a result, children with attention deficits often have difficulties in reading, writing, and arithmetic. Attention comprises different processes and is fundamentally involved in various cognitive functions. According to Posner and Petersen (1990), attention encompasses the following subsystems: orientation to sensory stimuli, filtering and selection of inputs, and maintenance of the thought on a stimulus, mental activity, or goal. Impaired attention hinders information processing at multiple levels and has been used as an explanation for the functional decline that occurs in aging and dementia. Impairments of attention in early dementia may contribute to performance reductions in other cognitive domains, including memory and executive functions (Rizzo 2000). It is possible therefore that improvements in the tested visual perceptual skills were the by-product of the enhanced attentional processing skills that was accomplished through this treatment schedule. We did not however document a significant effect of methylphenidate even though medicated children scored higher in the second measurement (Table 2). Methylphenidate improves vigilance performance, short-term memory performance, and visual problem solving (Swanson et al 1993). There is short-term benefit of stimulants regarding increased concentration and improving on-task behavior, whereas the long-term studies are difficult to evaluate because of methodological problems (Douglas et al 1988). In one study the longer term stimulant-treated group had better executive function performance (Vance et al 2003). Nevertheless, stimulants and other medications are unable to completely ameliorate all the difficulties for all children with ADHD. Therefore, attention needs to be directed toward psycho-educational interventions and their ability to increase the successful functioning of children with ADHD (Hoffman and DuPaul 2000). The deficit in working memory in children with ADHD can be improved by training; this also improved response inhibition and reasoning and resulted in a reduction of the parent-rated inattentive symptoms of ADHD (Klingberg 2005).

Conclusion

Visual perceptual skills improved in children enrolled in individualized psycho-educational intervention, irrespective of stimulants. The effect of the stimulants could have been investigated if the same children had been examined serially, after psycho- educational training, before and after methyl-phenidate administration. A larger sample of patients and a carefully executed design is important for this purpose.

References

  1. [AAP] American Academy of Pediatrics; Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Clinical Practice Guideline: Treatment of the school-aged child with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2001;108:1033–43. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.4.1033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. [APA] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  3. Biederman J, Faraone SV, Monuteaux MC, et al. How informative are parent reports of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms for assessing outcome in clinical trials of long-acting treatments? A pooled analysis of parents’ and teachers’ reports. Pediatrics. 2004;113:1667–71. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.6.1667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Buktenica NA. Visual learning. San Rafael CA: Dimensions; 1968. [Google Scholar]
  5. Burtner PA, Qualls C, Ortega SG, et al. Test-retest reliability of the Motor-Free Visual Perception Test Revised (MVPT-R) in children with and without learning disabilities. Phys Occup Ther Pediat. 2002;22:23–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chan PL, Chow SM. Reliability and validity of the Test of Visual-Perceptual Skills (Non-Motor)-Revised for Chinese preschoolers. Am J Occup Ther. 2005;59:369–76. doi: 10.5014/ajot.59.4.369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Conners CK. Forty years of methylphenidate treatment in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Atten Disord. 2002;6:S17–30. doi: 10.1177/070674370200601s04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Douglas VI, Barr R, Amin K, et al. Dosage effects and individual responsivity to methylphenidate in attention deficit disorder. J of Child Psych and Psychiatry. 1988;29:453–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1988.tb00737.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Foy JM, Earls MF. A process for developing community consensus regarding the diagnosis and management of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2005;115:e97–104. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gardner MF. Test of Visual-Perceptual Skills (n-m) Revised. Psychological and Educational Publications, Inc.; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  11. Gillberg C. Deficits in attention, motor control, and perception: a brief review. Arch Dis Childhood. 2003;88:904–10. doi: 10.1136/adc.88.10.904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Griffin JR, Birch TF, Bateman GF, et al. Dyslexia and visual perception: is there a relation? Optom Vis Sci. 1993;70:374–9. doi: 10.1097/00006324-199305000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hoffman JB, DuPaul GJ. Psychoeducational interventions for children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2000;9:647–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD-a randomized, controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:177–86. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. McFall SA, Deitz JC, Crowe TK. Test-retest reliability of the test of visual perceptual skills with children with learning disabilities. Am J Occup Ther. 1993;47:819–24. doi: 10.5014/ajot.47.9.819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Menken C, Cermak SA, Fisher A. Evaluating the visual-perceptual skills of children with cerebral palsy. Am J Occup Ther. 1987;41:646–51. doi: 10.5014/ajot.41.10.646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Mollica CM, Maruff P, Vance A. Development of a statistical approach to classifying treatment response in individual children with ADHD. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2004;19:445–56. doi: 10.1002/hup.624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Nianiou E, Mama-Agapiou P, Kotsalis C, et al. Difficulties in recognizing ADHD in an urban population. Europ J Paediatr Neurol. 2003;7:310. [Google Scholar]
  19. Olfson M, Gameroff MJ, Marcus SC, et al. National trends in the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1071–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Ann Rev Neurosci. 1990;13:25–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Remschmidt H for the Global ADHD Working Group. Global consensus on ADHD/HKD. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;14:127–37. doi: 10.1007/s00787-005-0439-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Rizzo M, Anderson SW, Dawson J, et al. Visual attention impairments in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 2000;54:1954–9. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.10.1954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Santosh PJ, Taylor E. Stimulant drugs. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;1:I27–43. doi: 10.1007/s007870070017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Stiers P, Decock P, Vandenbussche E. Separating visual perception and non-verbal intelligence in children with early brain injury. Brain Dev. 1999;21:397–406. doi: 10.1016/s0387-7604(99)00050-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Stiers P, Vanderkelen R, Vanneste G, et al. Visual-perceptual impairment in a random sample of children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2002;44:370–82. doi: 10.1017/s0012162201002249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Swanson J, Cantwell D, Lerner M, et al. Effects of stimulant medication on learning in children with ADHD. J Learn Disabilities. 1993;24:219–30. doi: 10.1177/002221949102400406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. The MTA cooperative group. A14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:1073–86. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Vance AL, Maruff P, Barnett R. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, combined type: better executive function performance with longer-term psychostimulant medication. Aust NZJ Psychiatry. 2003;37(5):570–6. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2003.01238.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment are provided here courtesy of Dove Press

RESOURCES