Abstract
A key part of the ecological theory of adaptive radiation is disruptive selection during periods of sympatry. Some insight into this process might be gained by studying populations that are bimodal for dual-context traits, i.e. those showing adaptive divergence and also contributing to reproductive isolation. A population meeting these criteria is the medium ground finch (Geospiza fortis) of El Garrapatero, Santa Cruz Island, Galápagos. We examined patterns of selection in this population by relating individual beak sizes to interannual recaptures during a prolonged drought. Supporting the theory, disruptive selection was strong between the two beak size modes. We also found some evidence of selection against individuals with the largest and smallest beak sizes, perhaps owing to competition with other species or to gaps in the underlying resource distribution. Selection may thus simultaneously maintain the current bimodality while also constraining further divergence. Spatial and temporal variation in G. fortis bimodality suggests a dynamic tug of war among factors such as selection and assortative mating, which may alternatively promote or constrain divergence during adaptive radiation.
Keywords: natural selection, ecological speciation, adaptive divergence, reproductive barriers, divergent selection, incipient speciation
1. Introduction
The ecological theory of adaptive radiation hypothesizes that diversification is driven by adaptation to different ecological conditions (i.e. environments or resources). The process starts with divergent/disruptive selection causing adaptive divergence between conspecific groups that occupy different environments or use different resources (Schluter 2000). Reproductive isolation then evolves as a by-product of adaptive divergence (i.e. ‘ecological speciation’, Schluter 2000). Likely reproductive barriers arising in this manner include assortative mating and selection against migrants and hybrids. This basic theory has garnered considerable support from mathematical models and empirical studies, to the extent that adaptive divergence and ecological speciation are thought to play a central role in the evolution of biological diversity (Skúlason & Smith 1995; Schluter 2000; Rundle & Nosil 2005).
Recent theoretical work relevant to the above ecological theory has called particular attention to the possible importance of disruptive selection in sympatry (Dieckmann & Doebeli 1999; Rueffler et al. 2006; Bolnick & Fitzpatrick 2007; Doebeli et al. 2007; Abrams et al. 2008). Empirical evidence supporting this view has emerged from several different contexts. First, disruptive selection can act between competing species to accentuate their divergence, as has been recently confirmed for Darwin's finches (Grant & Grant 2006) and Spea tadpoles (Pfennig & Rice 2007; Pfennig et al. 2007). Second, disruptive selection can result from competition between hybrids and parental forms, as demonstrated in threespine stickleback (Schluter 1994, 2003). Third, disruptive selection may occur within unimodal populations that show no evidence of incipient divergence (Kingsolver et al. 2001), such as Anolis lizards (Calsbeek & Smith 2007) and most stickleback populations (Bolnick 2004; Bolnick & Lau 2008). Fourth, disruptive selection may occur in randomly mating bimodal populations, such as African Pyrenestes finches (Smith 1993). These studies show that disruptive selection can occur in nature, suggesting the value of further considering its role in adaptive radiation.
Additional support for the idea that disruptive selection contributes to adaptive radiation would come from evidence of its action in conspecific populations that are bimodal for traits that undergo adaptive divergence and also influence reproductive isolation. The reasons are twofold. First, such dual-context traits may catalyse progress towards speciation (Dieckmann & Doebeli 1999; Schluter 2000), and as such have been called ‘magic’ traits (Gavrilets 2004). Second, bimodality without complete separation of the modes suggests an intermediate stage of divergence, and might therefore reveal conditions near the tipping point between species fission or fusion. Our work focuses on a medium ground finch (Geospiza fortis) population that shows bimodality in beak size, a critical dual-context trait in this taxon (Grant 1986; Podos 2001; Huber et al. 2007; Grant & Grant 2008). Importantly, the observed intraspecific bimodality is a small-scale version of the differences between G. fortis and its granivorous congeners (see below). The evolutionary forces acting within the bimodal population might therefore inform those that contributed to the initial adaptive radiation.
(a) Darwin's finches
The 14 recognized species of Darwin's finches in Galápagos are thought to have originated in the following manner (Lack 1947; Grant 1986; Grant & Grant 2008). At the outset, an ancestral species colonized multiple islands characterized by ecological differences, such as different seed size distributions. Resulting divergent selection among allopatric populations then caused their adaptive divergence, most notably in beak and body dimensions (Bowman 1961; Abbott et al. 1977; Schluter & Grant 1984; Grant & Grant 2000, 2008). Next, further dispersal among the islands brought some allopatric forms back into secondary contact (Grant 1986; Petren et al. 2005; Grant & Grant 2006), where several outcomes were possible. First, the allopatric divergence may have been too modest to generate major barriers to gene flow, thereby leading to fusion after secondary contact. Second, the allopatric divergence may have essentially completed speciation, and largely eliminated competition and gene flow in sympatry. Given that Darwin's finches do not show genetic incompatibilities, any such reproductive isolation would probably include ecological selection against hybrids and assortative mating based on beak/body dimensions (Ratcliffe & Grant 1983; Grant & Grant 1993, 2008; Podos 2001; Huber et al. 2007). Third, the allopatric divergence may have been sufficient to prevent complete fusion following secondary contact, but not to eliminate between-group competition and gene flow. In these cases, divergence and reproductive isolation might be accentuated in sympatry owing to character displacement and ‘reinforcement’ (Schluter et al. 1985; Grant & Grant 2006, 2008).
The third outcome described above is expected to involve disruptive selection that favours increasing divergence in beak dimensions. This hypothesis has recently been confirmed for interactions between two ground finch species (Grant & Grant 2006) and we here ask whether it also occurs within one of them. The relevance of this particular analysis is that sympatric interactions between partially divergent conspecific groups probably contributed to the adaptive radiation of Darwin's finches (Grant & Grant 2008). Our work focused on a G. fortis population (El Garrapatero, Santa Cruz Island) characterized by two modes along the morphological axes (beak and body size) that also separate this species from its smaller (Geospiza fuliginosa) and larger (Geospiza magnirostris) granivorous congeners (Foster et al. 2008; figure 1). The initial origins of bimodality within G. fortis remain uncertain, with possibilities including interspecific hybridization, allopatric divergence followed by secondary contact (as above), entirely sympatric or parapatric divergence, or phenotype-biased immigration/emigration (Huber et al. 2007; Grant & Grant 2008).
Figure 1.
The frequency distribution of beak sizes for all individual G. fuliginosa, G. fortis (small and large) and G. magnirostris captured at El Garrapatero in 2004–2006. Beak size is represented by PC1 calculated from all the birds combined. PC1 explained 95.6% of the total variation in beak length (loading=0.964), beak depth (0.986) and beak width (0.983). The species and mode names identify general ranges only, owing to ambiguity and possible hybrids in the areas of overlap. Although strong bimodality in beak size characterizes G. fortis at this site (for details see Hendry et al. 2006; figures 3 and 4), the numerous intermediates facilitate our test for disruptive selection.
We are interested in the factors that influence bimodality in G. fortis, because these factors might influence fission/fusion when partially divergent forms meet (or originate) in sympatry. In previous work, we showed that birds in the two modes show adaptive divergence and partial reproductive isolation. Specifically, they have different bite forces (Herrel et al. 2005), sing different songs (Huber & Podos 2006), respond differently to each other's songs (J. Podos 2006, unpublished data), mate assortatively (Huber et al. 2007) and show partially restricted gene flow (Huber et al. 2007). Birds with intermediate beak sizes are still present, however, perhaps owing to the few cross-type matings (Huber et al. 2007) and phenotype-biased immigration (see below). The continued maintenance of bimodality might therefore require the selective loss of individuals with intermediate beak sizes. We test for such disruptive selection by relating individual beak sizes to interannual recapture probabilities.
2. Material and methods
Our study site/population differs in several ways from Daphne Major, the only other site at which selection has been estimated for G. fortis. El Garrapatero is part of a continuous tract of low-elevation, arid-zone forest (Bursera graveolens and Cordia lutea) and is located within 1 km of coastal-zone vegetation. Daphne Major, by contrast, is isolated and open, and does not have coastal-zone vegetation (Grant 1986). The El Garrapatero G. fortis population shows a wide range of beak sizes in a bimodal distribution (figure 1; Hendry et al. 2006), whereas Daphne Major G. fortis have small beak sizes in a unimodal distribution (Grant 1986). Other ground finch species at El Garrapatero include a large population of G. fuliginosa (figure 1), a few G. magnirostris (figure 1) and a few Geospiza scandens (J. Podos, unpublished data). Other ground finch species established at Daphne Major include G. magnirostris and G. scandens (Grant 1986; Grant & Grant 2006). We draw these contrasts to point out that selection at El Garrapatero is probably different from that at Daphne Major.
Our fieldwork at El Garrapatero took place in 2004 (28 January–27 March), 2005 (15 January–12 May) and 2006 (26 January–7 March). These 3 years saw very little precipitation (figure 2), including less total precipitation during the ‘wet’ season (230 mm from January through May 2004–2006) than in any other 3-year period on record. Our study thus took place during one of the most prolonged droughts on record, resulting in very little reproduction by plants, insects and finches. These conditions are known to cause high mortality, strong selection and character displacement in Darwin's finches (Grant 1986; Grant & Grant 2002, 2006).
Figure 2.
Total rainfall in each year of record at Academy Bay on Santa Cruz Island. The variation at this site should generally parallel that at El Garrapatero, because the two sites are nearby (approx. 10 km apart) and at roughly the same elevation on the same side of Santa Cruz (for a map see Hendry et al. 2006).
Mist nets were used to capture the birds, which were then banded to allow the unambiguous identification of individuals. Each time a bird was captured, we followed previous work (Grant 1986) in characterizing it as an adult or juvenile (based on beak and plumage colour) and in measuring beak length (anterior edge of nostril to tip of upper mandible), beak depth (at the nares) and beak width (base of lower mandible). All of our subsequent analyses used adult birds only, because beak size does not change appreciably after maturity (Grant 1986). The three beak dimensions were combined into a single measure of overall beak size (PC1—see §3 for details). All subsequent analyses focus on this measure because PC1 is the standard measure of overall beak size for Darwin's finches (e.g. Grant 1986; Grant & Grant 2002, 2006). Moreover, the two G. fortis modes and the three granivorous ground finch species (G. fortis, G. fuliginosa and G. magnirostris) do not differ in beak shape after controlling for beak size (Foster et al. 2008).
To estimate selective losses from the local population, we collected data on interannual recaptures, a commonly used method for estimating selection in birds (e.g. Smith 1993; Grant & Grant 2002; Benkman 2003; Clegg et al. 2008). More integrated fitness measures (e.g. lifetime reproductive success) would require multigeneration pedigrees, which could not be obtained. Regardless, previous work on G. fortis has confirmed that ‘the most important determinant of fitness is the ability of an individual to survive to breed in many years’ (Grant & Grant 2000). One of the reasons is that periodic droughts (and other factors) can eliminate reproduction and cause very high adult mortality in a given year. These were the very conditions that characterized El Garrapatero G. fortis during our study period (see also Huber 2008).
Birds banded in one year that were resighted or recaptured in a subsequent year clearly survived and did not emigrate (local absolute fitness=1). Birds that were not resighted or recaptured in any subsequent year had probably died or emigrated (local absolute fitness=0). Exceptions would occur if banded birds were present at the site but not recorded. We minimized this possibility by increasing our sampling effort from each year to the next. For example, the total number of G. fortis captures (including recaptures) increased from 122 in 2004 to 208 in 2005 and 237 in 2006, despite high mortality and minimal recruitment. (Note that this increasing effort prevented useful estimates of recapture probabilities from formal mark–recapture models.) Given our very large effort in 2005 and 2006, we anticipate that very few banded birds at the site went undetected.
Although most of the birds missing from one year to the next had probably died, some could simply have emigrated beyond the study site. We attempted to reduce this possibility by (i) analysing adults only, because they usually remain faithful to breeding localities (S. Huber 2004–2006, unpublished data), and (ii) conducting regular searches for banded finches beyond our main tagging area (very few were thereby discovered). At the end, however, our selection estimates necessarily conflate mortality with emigration, a situation characterizing many studies of selection in birds (e.g. Smith 1993; Benkman 2003). Fortunately, this possible conflation does not impact our main inference, because both survival and emigration represent losses from the local population. That is, birds that leave the site take their genes with them, and so no longer contribute to the next generation at the site. In short, the evolution of the local population should be similarly affected by phenotype-biased emigration and phenotype-biased mortality.
We estimated selection on beak size from 2004 to 2005, from 2005 to 2006 and from 2004 to 2006, relating local absolute fitness to PC1 in each of these ‘intervals’. The standard approach to testing for disruptive selection is to fit a quadratic regression across the entire range of data (Kingsolver et al. 2001; Bolnick 2004; Calsbeek & Smith 2007; Bolnick & Lau 2008). This approach would not be appropriate in our study because selection was expected to be more complex. For example, selection might disfavour not only intermediates but also very large G. fortis (perhaps owing to competition with G. magnirostris; Grant & Grant 2006) or very small G. fortis (perhaps owing to competition with G. fuliginosa; Schluter et al. 1985). Such complicated patterns of selection would be misrepresented by a simple quadratic fit across the entire data range. We therefore first used non-parametric cubic splines (Schluter 1988) to visualize how selection changed across the range of beak sizes (for other examples see Smith (1993) and Benkman (2003)).
Our a priori hypothesis was selection against intermediate beak sizes, which would help to maintain the two G. fortis beak size modes. In addition to cubic splines, we therefore tested for statistically significant quadratic selection between the modes. This prediction was tested by excluding individuals more extreme than each mode and then using logistic regression to relate local relative fitness (local absolute fitness divided by local mean fitness) to PC1 and PC12. A positive coefficient for PC12 would be consistent with disruptive selection. The results of logistic regressions based on the entire data range, where disruptive selection is not necessarily expected (see above), are presented in the electronic supplementary material.
3. Results
Mortality/emigration was substantial. Out of the 112 adult G. fortis banded and measured in 2004, 33 remained in 2005 and 19 remained in 2006. Out of the 184 adult G. fortis banded and measured in 2005, 41 remained in 2006. These high mortality/emigration rates are consistent with other studies of G. fortis during prolonged droughts (Grant 1986; Grant & Grant 2002, 2006). For adult G. fortis in the selection intervals that started in 2004, PC1 explained 84.8 per cent of the variation in beak length (loading=0.876), beak depth (0.958) and beak width (0.926). For adult G. fortis in the selection interval that started in 2005, PC1 explained 90.1 per cent of the variation in beak length (loading=0.912), beak depth (0.973) and beak width (0.962).
Mortality/emigration was highly non-random with respect to beak size. Cubic splines revealed that mortality/emigration was often the lowest for those birds closest to the large or small beak size modes (figures 3 and 4). Individuals between the modes always suffered higher mortality/emigration than did those near the modes, a pattern consistent with disruptive selection. In addition, individuals smaller than the small mode, and (sometimes) those larger than the large mode, suffered higher mortality/emigration than did those near the modes. The resulting two-peak pattern of selection would help to maintain the existing beak size modes.
Figure 3.
Selection on beak size (PC1) for El Garrapatero G. fortis measured and banded in 2004. (a) The frequency distribution of PC1 scores in banded birds at the start of the selection interval (2004). (b) Selection on these birds from 2004 to 2005. (c) Selection on them from 2004 to 2006. Selection is depicted as a cubic spline with bootstrapped 95% CIs (Schluter 1988). Cubic splines were calculated using a smoothing parameter of λ=−4 for the 2004–2005 interval and λ=−8 for the 2004–2006 interval. Note that the x-axis is not directly comparable with those in figures 1 and 4 because the PC analysis is based on different birds in each case.
Figure 4.
Selection on beak size (PC1) for El Garrapatero G. fortis measured and banded in 2005. (a) The frequency distribution of PC1 scores in banded birds at the start of the selection interval (2005). (b) Selection on these birds from 2005 to 2006. Selection is depicted as a cubic spline with bootstrapped 95% CIs (λ=−4). Note that the x-axis is not directly comparable with those in figures 1 and 3 because the PC analysis is based on different birds in each case.
Our inference of disruptive selection between the beak size modes was strengthened through quadratic regressions that excluded individuals smaller than the small mode and larger than the large mode (table 1). The quadratic term was positive in all three intervals and significant (one-tailed) in two of them (table 1). The generally lower statistical support for the intervals starting in 2004, relative to the one starting in 2005, was most likely due to the smaller sample sizes in the former. Supporting this assertion, the estimated quadratic coefficient (i.e. effect size) was higher for the intervals starting in 2004 than for the interval starting in 2005 (table 1).
Table 1.
Logistic regressions testing for quadratic selection between the G. fortis beak size modes at El Garrapatero. (Note that our hypothesis (disruptive selection on PC12) is one-tailed but p-values from regressions are two-tailed, so the reported p-values here should be halved for comparison to a two-tailed alpha of 0.05. ‘Interval’ gives the starting and ending years for each selection interval. ‘Data range’ is the range of PC1 values used for analyses, selected to represent the range where disruptive selection is expected, i.e. between the beak size modes—see figures 3 and 4. ‘N (local absolute fitness)’ gives the number of marked individuals that did (1) or did not (0) remain at the study site at the end of the selection interval (i.e. response variable in the regressions). The remaining columns give the logistic regression statistics for the linear (PC1) and quadratic (PC12) terms. Disruptive selection is consistent with positive coefficients for the PC12 terms.)
N (local absolute fitness) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
interval | data range | 1 | 0 | term | coefficient | Wald | p-value |
2004–2005 | −0.75 to 1.75 | 22 | 56 | PC1 | −1.159 | 3.84 | 0.050 |
PC12 | 0.925 | 2.85 | 0.091 | ||||
2004–2006 | −0.75 to 1.75 | 12 | 66 | PC1 | −0.813 | 1.30 | 0.255 |
PC12 | 0.632 | 0.89 | 0.345 | ||||
2005–2006 | −1.00 to 1.80 | 37 | 119 | PC1 | −0.146 | 2.33 | 0.127 |
PC12 | 0.678 | 4.19 | 0.041 |
4. Discussion
The bimodal population of G. fortis at El Garrapatero shows several signatures of ecologically maintained adaptive divergence and reproductive isolation. First, the two modes differ in beak size (Hendry et al. 2006) and bite force capacities (Herrel et al. 2005), presumably owing to specialization on small/soft seeds versus large/hard seeds. Second, the two modes sing songs of different structure (Huber & Podos 2006) and respond differently to each other's songs (J. Podos 2006, unpublished data). Third, gene flow between the modes is somewhat limited (inferred from DNA microsatellites), at least partly owing to assortative mating (Huber et al. 2007). Here we demonstrate that bimodality might also be maintained by the selective loss of birds with intermediate beak sizes, i.e. disruptive selection between the modes (figures 3 and 4). These phenomena all parallel those expected to be important during the radiation of ground finches in the Galápagos (Grant 1986; Grant & Grant 2008).
What are the proximate causes of the disruptive selection documented here? One possibility is that competition for shared resources reduces the success of individuals specializing on intermediate seeds, causing them to die or emigrate. Another possibility is that intermediate birds fall into a gap in the underlying resource distribution (i.e. independent of competition), such as relatively few intermediate-sized seeds. Both of these mechanisms can cause disruptive selection in theoretical models (Dieckmann & Doebeli 1999; Schluter 2000; Rueffler et al. 2006; Doebeli et al. 2007; Abrams et al. 2008) and have previously been implicated in the divergence of Darwin's finches (Lack 1947; Bowman 1961; Abbott et al. 1977; Schluter & Grant 1984; Schluter et al. 1985; Grant & Grant 2006). We cannot presently address how bimodality in G. fortis might have been driven by competition, but we can at least speculate on the role of the underlying resource distribution. In particular, spatial and temporal variation in G. fortis bimodality across the island is at least roughly consistent with the differences in available food types (Hendry et al. 2006). Disentangling the relative influences of underlying resource distributions versus competition for shared resources will be a subject of future work.
(a) The dynamics of divergence
Given that El Garrapatero G. fortis manifest multiple processes (disruptive selection and assortative mating) thought to be important during sympatric phases of adaptive radiation, why is divergence incomplete? Indeed, individuals with intermediate beak sizes are so numerous that frequency histograms are not particularly effective ways of confirming bimodality (for a discussion see Hendry et al. 2006). One possibility is that the current bimodality has developed only recently and will become more prominent in the future. Although this is certainly possible, it is not consistent with the observations that bimodality has periodically characterized G. fortis at other sites on Santa Cruz Island for at least 100 years (Hendry et al. 2006; Grant & Grant 2008). Recent divergence therefore does not seem to be cumulative, which suggests ongoing constraints on diversification in sympatry. Many such constraints are theoretically possible and have been discussed in detail elsewhere (e.g. Coyne & Orr 2004; Bolnick & Fitzpatrick 2007). We focus here on a subset of the constraints that are particularly intriguing in the context of our study system.
One constraint might be temporal variation in selection that hampers cumulative progress towards speciation. For example, disruptive selection might be strong during droughts but not in wet years. As a possible interspecific analogue, hybrids between G. fortis and G. scandens had low survival during dry periods but not during wet periods (Grant & Grant 1993, 1996), the latter thus precipitating their current trend towards species fusion (Grant et al. 2004). Something similar may have caused the loss of intraspecific bimodality in Academy Bay G. fortis, although the culprit here may have been the recent intensification of local human influences (Hendry et al. 2006).
Another constraint might be ongoing immigration of intermediate-sized birds from other locations. Such immigration could explain why substantial numbers of intermediate birds are present at El Garrapatero (figure 1) despite both strong assortative mating (Huber et al. 2007) and divergent selection (figures 3 and 4). It would also explain why bimodality did not appear to decrease from 2004 to 2005 despite very little local reproduction. We could not directly test this hypothesis of phenotype-biased immigration because we did not band all of the birds at our site, so new immigrants could not be confirmed. Recent work on other systems, however, is certainly drawing attention to the potential importance of phenotype-biased migration (Edelaar et al. 2008).
A final constraint we wish to discuss is that further adaptive divergence and reproductive isolation might actually be impeded by selection. Specifically, mortality/emigration was also high for the smallest G. fortis and sometimes also for the largest G. fortis (figures 3 and 4). Overall, then, selection in this population might be considered roughly stabilizing around each beak size mode, a situation similar to that seen in African Pyrenestes finches (Smith 1993). This two-peak pattern of selection might be expected if the adaptive landscape for beak size has reasonably discrete peaks corresponding to different food types (Schluter 2000). As noted above, such landscapes for Darwin's finches might be driven by gaps in the underlying resource distribution, as well as by interspecific competition (Abbott et al. 1977; Schluter & Grant 1984; Schluter et al. 1985; Grant & Grant 2006, 2008). These two possibilities cannot be discriminated at present, but it is nonetheless tempting to speculate that competition might be more important at the small end of the G. fortis distribution (given that G. fuliginosa are common; figure 1), whereas constraints of the underlying resource distribution might be more important at the large end (given that G. magnirostris are rare; figure 1).
(b) Food for thought
Geospiza fortis on Santa Cruz have been collected for more than a century, revealing a spatial and temporal mosaic of bimodality (Lack 1947; Bowman 1961; Ford et al. 1973; Hendry et al. 2006; Grant & Grant 2008). At one site (Academy Bay), bimodality was historically strong but is now weak. At a second site (El Garrapatero), bimodality is currently strong. At a third site (Borrero Bay), bimodality has not been documented in the past nor the present. This spatial and temporal variation suggests a dynamic tug of war between forces promoting diversification and those constraining it. At certain times and places, promoting factors may gain the upper hand and make substantial progress towards speciation. At other times and places, constraints may gain the upper hand and reverse any such progress. We suggest that this sort of dynamic tug of war can recur during adaptive radiations. Speciation events might then represent those few cases where promoting factors, such as strong disruptive selection and assortative mating, have a sustained run of influence over constraining factors. Darwin's finches may be a promising group for observing this dynamic.
We close by noting the value of comparing morphological divergence and reproductive barriers within species versus between species, in an adaptive radiation. A selection of recent studies of vertebrates can be used to highlight a contrast emerging from such comparisons. On the one hand, adaptive divergence and reproductive barriers between species may involve dimensions (ecological, morphological and physiological) that differ from those seen between groups within species, as may be the case for Timema walking sticks (Nosil & Sandoval 2008). On the other hand, adaptive divergence and reproductive barriers between species may closely parallel those within species, as might be the case for Geospiza (see above), Anolis lizards (Calsbeek et al. 2007), threespine stickleback (Robinson 2000; Bolnick & Lau 2008) and mosquitofish (Langerhans et al. 2007). Or perhaps the apparent correspondence in some of these examples simply reflects the need to study more dimensions. Either way, these considerations highlight the importance of determining the conditions under which adaptive radiation can or cannot result by simply extrapolating processes seen within species.
Acknowledgments
This work was conducted in concordance with Animal Use Protocols approved by the University of Massachusetts Amherst.Fieldwork was coordinated through the Charles Darwin Research Station and the Galápagos National Park Service. The rainfall data were provided by the Charles Darwin Research Station. Data collection in the field was assisted by E. Hilton, M. Rossi-Santos, D. Ruiz, A. Gabela, P. Kelley, M. Hendry, K. Huyghe, B. Vanhooydonck and D. Delaney. The manuscript was improved through comments from R. Calsbeek, P. Nosil and an anonymous reviewer. Financial support was provided by the National Science Foundation (J.P.—IBN 0347291), with additional funding from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (A.P.H.).
Supplementary Material
Here we provide the results of logistic regressions based on all adult G. fortis banded El Garrapatero (i.e. not just those between the two beak size modes, which is the case in table 1)
References
- Abbott I., Abbott L.K., Grant P.R. Comparative ecology of Galápagos ground finches (Geospiza Gould): evaluation of the importance of floristic diversity and interspecific competition. Ecol. Monogr. 1977;47:151–184. doi:10.2307/1942615 [Google Scholar]
- Abrams P.A., Rueffler C., Kim G. Determinants of the strength of disruptive and/or divergent selection arising from resource competition. Evolution. 2008;62:1571–1586. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00385.x. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00385.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benkman C.W. Divergent selection drives the adaptive radiation of crossbills. Evolution. 2003;57:1176–1181. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00326.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolnick D.I. Can intraspecific competition drive disruptive selection? An experimental test in natural populations of sticklebacks. Evolution. 2004;58:608–618. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolnick D.I., Fitzpatrick B.M. Sympatric speciation: models and empirical evidence. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2007;38:459–487. doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.38.091206.095804 [Google Scholar]
- Bolnick D.I., Lau O.L. Predictable patterns of disruptive selection in stickleback in postglacial lakes. Am. Nat. 2008;172:1–11. doi: 10.1086/587805. doi:10.1086/587805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowman R.I. Morphological differentiation and adaptation in the Galápagos finches. Univ. Calif. Publ. Zool. 1961;58:1–302. [Google Scholar]
- Calsbeek R., Smith T.B. Experimentally replicated disruptive selection on performance traits in a Carribean lizard. Evolution. 2007;62:478–484. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00282.x. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00282.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calsbeek R., Smith T.B., Bardeleben C. Intraspecific variation in Anolis sagrei mirrors the adaptive radiation of Greater Antillean anoles. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2007;90:189–199. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2007.00700.x [Google Scholar]
- Clegg S.M., Frentiu F.D., Kikkawa J., Tavecchia G., Owens I.P.F. 4000 years of phenotypic change in an island bird: heterogeneity of selection over three microevolutionary timescales. Evolution. 2008;62:2393–2410. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00437.x. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00437.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coyne J.A., Orr H.A. Sinauer Associates; Sunderland, MA: 2004. Speciation. [Google Scholar]
- Dieckmann U., Doebeli M. On the origin of species by sympatric speciation. Nature. 1999;400:354–357. doi: 10.1038/22521. doi:10.1038/22521 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doebeli M., Blok H.J., Leimar O., Dieckmann U. Multimodal pattern formation in phenotype distributions of sexual populations. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2007;274:347–357. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.3725. doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3725 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edelaar P., Siepielski A.M., Clobert J. Matching habitat choice causes directional gene flow: a neglected dimension in ecology and evolution. Evolution. 2008;62:2462–2472. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00459.x. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00459.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ford H.A., Parkin D.T., Ewing A.W. Divergence and evolution in Darwin's finches. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 1973;5:289–295. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1973.tb00707.x [Google Scholar]
- Foster D., Podos J., Hendry A.P. A geometric morphometric appraisal of beak shape in Darwin's finches. J. Evol. Biol. 2008;21:263–275. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2007.01449.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gavrilets S. Princeton University Press; Princeton, NJ: 2004. Fitness landscapes and the origin of species. [Google Scholar]
- Grant P.R. Princeton University Press; Princeton, NJ: 1986. Ecology and evolution of Darwin's finches. [Google Scholar]
- Grant B.R., Grant P.R. Evolution of Darwin's finches caused by a rare climatic event. Proc. R. Soc. B. 1993;251:111–117. doi:10.1098/rspb.1993.0016 [Google Scholar]
- Grant B.R., Grant P.R. High survival of Darwin's finch hybrids: effects of beak morphology and diet. Ecology. 1996;77:500–509. doi:10.2307/2265625 [Google Scholar]
- Grant P.R., Grant B.R. Non-random fitness variation in two populations of Darwin's finches. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2000;267:131–138. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.0977. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.0977 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant P.R., Grant B.R. Unpredictable evolution in a 30-year study of Darwin's finches. Science. 2002;296:707–711. doi: 10.1126/science.1070315. doi:10.1126/science.1070315 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant P.R., Grant B.R. Evolution of character displacement in Darwin's finches. Science. 2006;313:224–226. doi: 10.1126/science.1128374. doi:10.1126/science.1128374 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant P.R., Grant B.R. Princeton University Press; Princeton, NJ: 2008. How and why species multiply. [Google Scholar]
- Grant P.R., Grant B.R., Markert J.A., Keller L.F., Petren K. Convergent evolution of Darwin's finches caused by introgressive hybridization and selection. Evolution. 2004;58:1588–1599. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2004.tb01738.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hendry A.P., Grant P.R., Grant B.R., Ford H.A., Brewer M.J., Podos J. Possible human impacts on adaptive radiation: beak size bimodality in Darwin's finches. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2006;273:1887–1894. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.3534. doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3534 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrel A., Podos J., Huber S.K., Hendry A.P. Bite performance and morphology in a population of Darwin's finches: implications for the evolution of beak shape. Funct. Ecol. 2005;19:43–48. doi:10.1111/j.0269-8463.2005.00923.x [Google Scholar]
- Huber S.K. Effects of the introduced parasite Philornis downsi on nestling growth and mortality in the medium ground finch (Geospiza fortis) Biol. Conserv. 2008;141:601–609. doi:10.1016/j.biocon.2007.11.012 [Google Scholar]
- Huber S.K., Podos J. Beak morphology and song production covary in a population of Darwin's finches (Geospiza fortis) Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2006;88:489–498. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2006.00638.x [Google Scholar]
- Huber S.K., De León L.F., Hendry A.P., Bermingham E., Podos J. Reproductive isolation of sympatric morphs in a population of Darwin's finches. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2007;274:1709–1714. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.0224. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kingsolver J.G., Hoekstra H.E., Hoekstra J.M., Berrigan D., Vignieri S.N., Hill C.E., Hoang A., Gibert P., Beerli P. The strength of phenotypic selection in natural populations. Am. Nat. 2001;157:245–261. doi: 10.1086/319193. doi:10.1086/319193 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lack D. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 1947. Darwin's finches. [Google Scholar]
- Langerhans R.B., Gifford M.E., Joseph E.O. Ecological speciation in Gambusia fishes. Evolution. 2007;61:2056–2074. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00171.x. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00171.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nosil P., Sandoval C.P. Ecological niche dimensionality and the evolutionary diversification of stick insects. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e1907. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001907. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001907 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petren K., Grant P.R., Grant B.R., Keller L.F. Comparative landscape genetics and the adaptive radiation of Darwin's finches: the role of peripheral isolation. Mol. Ecol. 2005;14:2943–2957. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2005.02632.x. doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2005.02632.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfennig D.W., Rice A.M. An experimental test of character displacement's role in promoting postmating isolation between conspecific populations in contrasting competitive environments. Evolution. 2007;61:2433–2443. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00190.x. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00190.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfennig D.W., Rice A.M., Martin R.A. Field and experimental evidence for competition's role in phenotypic divergence. Evolution. 2007;61:257–271. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00034.x. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00034.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Podos J. Correlated evolution of morphology and vocal signal structure in Darwin's finches. Nature. 2001;409:185–188. doi: 10.1038/35051570. doi:10.1038/35051570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ratcliffe L.M., Grant P.R. Species recognition in Darwin's finches (Geospiza, Gould). I. Discrimination by morphological cues. Anim. Behav. 1983;31:1139–1153. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80021-9 [Google Scholar]
- Robinson B.W. Trade offs in habitat-specific foraging efficiency and the nascent adaptive divergence of sticklebacks in lakes. Behaviour. 2000;137:865–888. doi:10.1163/156853900502501 [Google Scholar]
- Rueffler C., Van Dooren T.J.M., Leimar O., Abrams P.A. Disruptive selection and then what? Trends Ecol. Evol. 2006;21:238–245. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2006.03.003. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2006.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rundle H.D., Nosil P. Ecological speciation. Ecol. Lett. 2005;8:336–352. doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00715.x [Google Scholar]
- Schluter D. Estimating the form of natural selection on a quantitative trait. Evolution. 1988;42:849–861. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1988.tb02507.x. doi:10.2307/2408904 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schluter D. Experimental evidence that competition promotes divergence in adaptive radiation. Science. 1994;266:798–801. doi: 10.1126/science.266.5186.798. doi:10.1126/science.266.5186.798 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schluter D. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 2000. The ecology of adaptive radiation. [Google Scholar]
- Schluter D. Frequency dependent natural selection during character displacement in sticklebacks. Evolution. 2003;57:1142–1150. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00323.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schluter D., Grant P.R. Determinants of morphological patterns in communities of Darwin's finches. Am. Nat. 1984;123:175–196. doi:10.1086/284196 [Google Scholar]
- Schluter D., Price T.D., Grant P.R. Ecological character displacement in Darwin's finches. Science. 1985;227:1056–1059. doi: 10.1126/science.227.4690.1056. doi:10.1126/science.227.4690.1056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skúlason S., Smith T.B. Resource polymorphisms in vertebrates. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1995;10:366–370. doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(00)89135-1. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(00)89135-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith T.B. Disruptive selection and the genetic basis of bill size polymorphism in the African finch Pyrenestes. Nature. 1993;363:618–620. doi:10.1038/363618a0 [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Here we provide the results of logistic regressions based on all adult G. fortis banded El Garrapatero (i.e. not just those between the two beak size modes, which is the case in table 1)