Abstract
Background
Although neighborhoods with more collective efficacy have better health in general, recent work suggests that social norms and collective efficacy may in combination influence health behaviors such as smoking.
Methods
Using data from the New York Social Environment Study (conducted in 2005; n=4,000), we examined the separate and combined associations of neighborhood collective efficacy and anti-smoking norms with individual smoking. The outcome was current smoking, assessed using the WMH-CIDI tobacco module. Exposures of interest were neighborhood collective efficacy, measured as the average neighborhood response on a well-established scale, and neighborhood anti-smoking norms, measured as the proportion of residents who believed regular smoking was unacceptable. All analyses adjusted for demographic and socioeconomic characteristics, as well as history of smoking prior to residence in the current neighborhood, individual perception of smoking level in the neighborhood, individual perception of collective efficacy, and individual smoking norms.
Results
In separate generalized estimating equation logistic regression models, neighborhood collective efficacy was not associated with smoking (OR 1.06, 95% CI 0.84–1.34) but permissive neighborhood smoking norms were associated with more smoking (OR 1.34, 95% CI 1.03–1.74), particularly among residents with no prior history of smoking (OR 2.88, 95% CI 1.92–4.30). When considered in combination, where smoking norms were permissive, higher collective efficacy was associated with more smoking; in contrast, where norms were strongly anti-smoking, higher collective efficacy was associated with less smoking.
Conclusions
Features of the neighborhood social environment may need to be considered in combinations to understand their role in shaping health and health behavior.
Keywords: neighborhood, social cohesion, informal social control, norms, smoking, cigarettes
1. Introduction
Research in a variety of disciplines including sociology, criminology, and psychology has suggested that cohesiveness and collective power of societies and communities are associated with improved health and wellbeing (Bandura, 2001, Durkheim, 1897, Faris and Dunham, 1939, Shaw and McKay, 1942). In this vein, Bandura introduced the notion of collective agency which he termed “collective efficacy” (Bandura, 2001). This concept combines notions of the cohesiveness of a group (social cohesion), with ideas about their potential efficacy in achieving group goals (informal social control) (Sampson et al., 1997).
In epidemiologic analyses, the presence of collective efficacy in a neighborhood has typically been associated with better health and fewer social ills (Kawachi and Berkman, 2000, Lomas, 1998, Sampson et al., 1997). However, recent discussions suggest that social cohesion, informal social control, and other related constructs may not be unequivocally beneficial (Greiner et al., 2004, Kushner and Sterk, 2005, Portes, 1998). Portes suggests that where social cohesion is high, there is the potential for unhealthy behavioral norms to be enforced on residents (Portes, 1998). Thus, when examining neighborhood cohesion in relation to health behavior, it may be important to consider the norms of the neighborhood in conjunction with the cohesiveness of a neighborhood to understand how the social environment will shape behavior (Durkheim, 1897, Kushner and Sterk, 2005, Portes, 1998). Neighborhoods with high levels of informal social control may enforce local norms on residents, however these norms may or may not be health promoting. Moreover, residents of cohesive neighborhoods may have more contact with one another, providing more opportunities for transmission of norms that, again, may enhance health or may be detrimental.
One health behavior of considerable interest in the context of this discussion is smoking, considered the leading behavioral cause of mortality, contributing to approximately 20% of deaths in the United States by some calculations (Mokdad et al., 2004). In addition to being a strong contributor to morbidity and mortality, smoking is strongly socially shaped and thus may be influenced by both the cohesiveness and the norms of communities. While the prevalence of smoking has been declining overall in the United States, 22% of adults are still current smokers (Trosclair et al., 2005); young people continue to take up smoking and currently approximately 25% of young people are smokers by the time they finish high school (Johnston et al., 2006). Broad national and statewide norm-changing media campaigns, smoking restriction legislation, and tobacco taxation that have attempted to change the social and structural environment of smoking are credited with contributing to the smoking declines to date; however, these declines have not been experienced equally in all populations, and there remains substantial variation in smoking geographically (Gilpin et al., 2004, Jiles et al., 2005, Nelson et al., 2001, Pierce et al., 1998). The local variation in smoking raises the question of what neighborhood social and structural characteristics might shape smoking behavior.
There is substantial research suggesting that neighborhood socioeconomic indicators are associated with smoking, however there is little research on other social and structural aspects of neighborhoods that may be related to smoking (Chuang et al., 2005a, Cubbin et al., 2006, Datta et al., 2006, Davey Smith et al., 1998, Diez Roux et al., 1997, Duncan et al., 1999, Ecob and Macintyre, 2000, Kleinschmidt et al., 1995, Monden et al., 2006, Ohlander et al., 2006, Reijneveld, 1998, Reijneveld, 2002, Ross, 2000, Sundquist et al., 1999, Tseng et al., 2001, van Lenthe and Mackenback, 2006). There have been two studies examining neighborhood cohesion and control in association with smoking, and both found that more cohesive neighborhoods had lower prevalences of smoking (Miles, 2006, Patterson et al., 2004). There are no studies of neighborhood smoking norms and smoking among adults. Most of the research on substance use norms has been conducted in adolescents, and the norms have not typically been conceptualized as neighborhood level properties. The existing studies among adolescents show the importance of adult and peer norms for predicting smoking (Chuang et al., 2005b, Eisenberg and Forster, 2003, Ellickson et al., 2003, Ennett et al., 1997, Frohlich et al., 2002), suggesting these may be important social properties to consider in the context of adult smoking.
The potential importance of neighborhood collective efficacy and substance use norms in shaping smoking behavior is clear. However, the only extant research on the convergence of collective efficacy and smoking norms comes from one qualitative study conducted in Scotland (Stead et al., 2001). In the neighborhood studied, smoking was used to cope with a variety of stressors including unemployment and unsafe environments. In addition, there were strong pro-smoking norms in the neighborhood and a strong sense of local identification and cohesiveness. The cohesiveness and norms seemed to combine, such that “…these positive aspects of life [such as cohesiveness] may ironically be as bound up with and reinforcing of smoking as the more negative aspects of life [such as stressors]” (Stead et al., 2001).
In this analysis, we examined the relations of neighborhood collective efficacy and neighborhood smoking norms with smoking behavior, both separately and in combination. Building on the extant literature, we hypothesized that the association between collective efficacy and smoking would to be modified by the norms of the community, and specifically that the association would be reversed; cohesive neighborhoods with permissive smoking norms would have the highest smoking, and cohesive neighborhoods with strong anti-smoking norms would have the lowest smoking. Moreover, in this analysis we applied methods that address the problems of stratification and social selection which are challenges to interpreting neighborhood research (Oakes, 2004). In this analysis, we examine large neighborhood areas in New York City (NYC). While these areas are larger than neighborhoods examined in many studies (Sampson et al., 1997), it is interesting to examine whether these processes operate at a larger scale.
2. Methods
The New York Social Environment Study (NYSES) is a multilevel study designed to examine neighborhood level exposures, including economic, social and structural characteristics, and substance use in NYC. The NYSES was conducted between June and December of 2005. We used random-digit-dial methods to contact and interview 4000 NYC residents. One adult 18 years or older was interviewed by telephone in each household; the respondent was the person who either most recently or would next celebrate their birthday (randomly selected). Interviews were conducted in English or Spanish. The cooperation percentage was 54%, representing the percentage of those contacted who agreed to participate in the study ((completed + screened out)/(completed + screened out + refused)). Respondents were offered $10 in compensation for their participation. The study protocol was approved by the institutional review boards of the New York Academy of Medicine, the University of Michigan, and the University of California, Berkeley.
2.1 Measures
Respondents were interviewed with a structured questionnaire that included questions on demographic and socioeconomic characteristics including age, race and ethnicity, gender, marital status, place of birth, education, income, employment, years lived in the current neighborhood, and interview language. Smoking behavior was assessed using the World Mental Health Comprehensive International Diagnostic Interview (WMH-CIDI) tobacco module (Kessler and Ustun, 2004). Measures from this module that were used in the present analysis include current smoking and a retrospectively recalled history of smoking including age first tried smoking, age first smoked regularly, and age last smoked. Current smoking was the outcome in this analysis. Research suggests that self-report of smoking is highly concordant with bio-markers of smoking (Vartiainen et al., 2002), and that self-report assessments are comparable by phone and in-person (Nelson et al., 2003). History of smoking, in conjunction with number of years lived in the current neighborhood was used to assess smoking prior to residence in the current neighborhood. Respondents were classified as those who never tried smoking, those who tried smoking but never smoked regularly, and those who smoked weekly or daily prior to residence in the current neighborhood. Smoking prior to residence in the current neighborhood was controlled as a confounder in all analyses to account for one contributor to social selection (Diez Roux, 2004); by controlling for history of smoking we assure that associations observed are not due to the fact that smokers are likely to move to certain types of neighborhoods (Ahern et al., 2008b). Individual perception of smoking level in the neighborhood was assessed as the proportion of adults in the neighborhood perceived to be smokers (none, few, some, most, all), and was controlled to distinguish the effect that neighborhood norms might have on smoking from the effect of that observing smoking behaviors of others in the neighborhood might have on smoking. Individual perception of collective efficacy and smoking norms were adjusted to distinguish the effect of each neighborhood characteristic, from that of individual perception of that characteristic.
Respondents provided information about their residential address or nearest cross-streets so that their locations could be geocoded and linked to their neighborhoods of residence.a The larger neighborhood areas for this analysis were the 59 community districts in NYC (median population 128,313, median square miles 3), well-defined units, each headed by an administrative community board that, as such, has political and social relevance for the residents. Politically, the board of each community district serves as an advisory body with a formal role designated by the City Charter in matters including land use, determining local budget priorities and monitoring City service delivery. Meetings are held monthly and are open to the public. Each board establishes committees to focus on specific issues of concern. Socially, community districts were initially defined by a resident consultative process organized by the Office of City Planning to reflect residents’ own descriptions of neighborhoods in the 1970s and as a consequence represent recognizable neighborhood areas with which residents identify, such as the Upper East Side, or the South Bronx. Many attributes of these neighborhood areas have been associated with resident health and health behaviors (Ahern and Galea, 2006, Ahern et al., 2008a, Ahern et al., 2008b, Bernstein et al., 2007, Galea et al., 2007, Galea et al., 2005, Galea et al., 2003, Hembree et al., 2005, Nandi et al., 2006).
Neighborhood collective efficacy was measured using the scale developed by Sampson and colleagues, which is comprised of the subscales of social cohesion and informal social control (Sampson et al., 1997). The social cohesion subscale includes five items with Likert responses and assesses residents’ perceptions of the extent to which their neighbors are close-knit, are helpful, get along, share values, and are trustworthy. The informal social control subscale also includes five items with Likert responses and measures perceptions of the likelihood that neighbors would intervene if children skipped school, sprayed graffiti, or disrespected an adult, if there were a fight, or if the city was closing a firehouse. The responses of all residents in each neighborhood are averaged to calculate the neighborhood level measure of collective efficacy. Cronbach’s alpha for the full collective efficacy scale was 0.77 (alpha for social cohesion was 0.64, alpha for informal social control was 0.72), consistent with previous reports (Echeverria et al., 2004, Sampson et al., 1997).
Neighborhood smoking norms were measured using a question modified from the National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH). Respondents were asked their opinion of adults smoking cigarettes regularly and were given the options of “acceptable”, “unacceptable” and “don’t care”. The neighborhood measure is the proportion of residents who believe it is “unacceptable” for adults to smoke cigarettes regularly in each neighborhood.
2.2 Analysis
All analyses were weighted by the ratio of the persons in the household to phone lines in the household to account for the probability of selection for interview. In addition, all analyses were replicated with additional weighting to adjust the respondents to the joint age, race/ethnicity, and gender distribution within each neighborhood as determined from the 2000 Census data. This was done to assess the potential impact of non-response of particular population groups, and to examine whether non-response was a plausible explanation for the analysis findings. Because neither the magnitude nor the statistical significance of any of the parameter estimates changed appreciably after this additional weighting, analyses with only the selection probability weights are presented here.
Several descriptive analyses were conducted initially. Neighborhood collective efficacy and neighborhood smoking norms were assessed descriptively to examine the range of each exposure and to identify potential outliers, and the correlation between the neighborhood characteristics was assessed to assure that the two measures were not too collinear to consider in combination. To assess the extent of stratification, we examined the probabilities or propensities for living in neighborhoods with 1) high versus low collective efficacy (median split), 2) high versus low smoking norms (median split), and 3) four category combinations of high and low neighborhood collective efficacy and smoking norms, modeled as a function of individual characteristics (history of smoking, individual smoking norm, age, race/ethnicity, gender, marital status, place of birth, education, income, employment, years lived in the neighborhood, and survey language) (Rosenbaum and Rubin, 1983). Through this process we were able to examine whether there was overlap between the “types” of people, defined by covariate combinations, who lived in low collective efficacy neighborhoods and those who lived in high collective efficacy neighborhoods. Propensities are often used as part of matched analysis as a tool to control confounding (Rosenbaum and Rubin, 1983), however they can also serve a useful purpose in describing the distribution of covariates across different exposure groups. If the people who actually lived in high and low collective efficacy neighborhoods had similar and predominantly overlapping distributions of propensities then we knew that there was little stratification in terms of the variables in the propensity model. This implied that people of all “types” lived in both high and low collective efficacy neighborhoods and that the analysis did not rely on extrapolation (e.g., if all Latina women older than 65 years lived in high collective efficacy neighborhoods, extrapolation would be required to assess the effect of low collective efficacy on this “type” of person).
The two neighborhood measures and their combination were examined in association with current smoking in bivariable analysis. Individual demographic and socioeconomic characteristics that were conceptually considered confounders based on the literature were considered as confounders in the multivariable analysis. In addition, age, race and ethnicity and gender were considered potential effect modifiers based on previous research on neighborhood characteristics (Robert, 1999); years lived in the neighborhood and history of smoking were also considered as they were logical potential effect modifiers that had not been examined in previous analyses. Missingness indicator variables were included for all covariates where some respondents declined to answer. Generalized estimating equation (GEE) logistic regression models were used in all analyses to account for potential clustering by neighborhood, and to estimate population averaged parameter estimates with robust standard errors (Zeger et al., 1988); Each neighborhood exposure was examined separately in association with current smoking, then adjusted for demographic and socioeconomic confounders as well as history of smoking, and lastly adjusted for individual perception of smoking in the neighborhood and perception of each neighborhood characteristic (collective efficacy, smoking norms). All odds ratios presented are for a two standard deviation change in the neighborhood exposure. Finally, the combination of the neighborhood exposures was examined with an interaction term.
3. Results
The survey respondents were demographically similar to the overall population of NYC based on the most recent census, with 38.1% White, 27.0% African American, 5.0% Asian, 27.2% Hispanic, and 2.5% of other racial ethnic groups. Mean age was 45 years (range 18 – 94), 51.1% of respondents were female, and 39.2% were born outside the United States. Approximately 20% of respondents were current smokers. A full description of the sample is provided in Table 1.
Table 1.
NYSES population | 2000 Census – NYC | ||
---|---|---|---|
N | % | % | |
Total | 4000 | 100.00 | 100.00 |
Age | |||
18–24 | 350 | 11.78 | 13.23 |
25–34 | 685 | 18.05 | 22.54 |
35–44 | 815 | 19.50 | 20.82 |
45–54 | 808 | 21.42 | 16.68 |
55–64 | 612 | 14.85 | 11.26 |
≥ 65 | 690 | 14.39 | 15.46 |
Race/Ethnicity | |||
White | 1616 | 38.18 | 38.65 |
African American | 1055 | 27.03 | 22.97 |
Asian | 164 | 5.08 | 10.08 |
Hispanic | 958 | 27.19 | 24.71 |
Other | 95 | 2.52 | 3.59 |
Gender | |||
Male | 1880 | 48.89 | 46.20 |
Female | 2120 | 51.11 | 53.80 |
Marital Status | |||
Married | 1632 | 47.33 | |
Divorced | 479 | 9.56 | |
Separated | 208 | 4.70 | |
Widowed | 354 | 6.68 | |
Never Married | 1270 | 31.73 | |
Birth Place | |||
NYC | 1810 | 44.65 | |
Other US Location | 731 | 16.13 | |
Different Country | 1406 | 39.22 | |
Education | |||
Less than High School | 508 | 13.89 | |
High School/GED | 923 | 24.69 | |
Some College | 879 | 23.23 | |
College Graduate | 883 | 21.63 | |
Graduate Work | 730 | 16.57 | |
Income | |||
≤$40,000 | 1605 | 46.45 | |
$40,001 – $80,000 | 1093 | 31.97 | |
> $80,000 | 722 | 21.58 | |
Unemployed | |||
Yes | 321 | 8.56 | |
No | 3658 | 91.43 | |
Smoking before Moved to Neighborhood | |||
Ever Smoked/Tried Smoking | 686 | 16.4 | |
Weekly/Daily Smoker | 1341 | 31.08 | |
Never Smoked | 1973 | 52.52 | |
Current Smoker | |||
Yes | 835 | 20.40 | |
No | 3164 | 79.60 |
Examination of neighborhood collective efficacy suggested there were no outliers; the mean collective efficacy value was 3.5 with a range of 2.7 to 4.0. A value of 4 indicates that on average respondents somewhat agree that the neighborhood is cohesive, while a value of 3 indicates that on average respondents neither agree nor disagree that the neighborhood is cohesive. Two neighborhoods were outliers in terms of their anti-smoking norms, with values more than two standard deviations below the mean. Because we were not comfortable making inference about neighborhoods with such low levels of anti-smoking norms based on only two outlying neighborhoods, these neighborhoods were excluded from the analyses of neighborhood smoking norms. For the included neighborhoods, the average proportion of residents who believed it was unacceptable to smoke was 60% with a range of 43% to 76%. Neighborhood collective efficacy and smoking norms were weakly negatively correlated (pearson correlation coefficient −0.25, p=0.06).
Examining the propensities for living in neighborhoods with high versus low collective efficacy, high versus low anti-smoking norms, and four category combinations of collective efficacy and smoking norms, there was little suggestion of stratification. People of all “types”, based on individual covariates, lived in neighborhoods with different values of the neighborhood exposures. In all instances, less than 1% of respondents had propensity values that were more extreme (higher or lower) than the propensity values among respondents living in neighborhoods with a different exposure (tables and plots of the propensity values are available from the corresponding author).
In the first set of GEE logistic regression models, presented in Table 2, we examined the relation between neighborhood collective efficacy and smoking alone, adjusting for confounders including smoking history, and adjusting for individual perception of smoking in the neighborhood and individual perception of collective efficacy. In all models, lower neighborhood collective efficacy was associated with slightly higher odds of smoking, but none of the associations were significant (fully adjusted OR 1.06 for a 2 standard deviation (SD) decrease in collective efficacy, 95% CI 0.84–1.34). We found no interactions between collective efficacy and history of smoking, age, race/ethnicity, gender, or years lived in neighborhood.
Table 2.
Model 1 | Model 2b | Model 3b | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta | SE | OR | 95% CI | Beta | SE | OR | 95% CI | Beta | SE | OR | 95% CI | |
Neighborhood collective efficacy a | 0.13 | 0.13 | 1.13 | (0.88 – 1.46) | 0.13 | 0.12 | 1.14 | (0.9 – 1.43) | 0.06 | 0.12 | 1.06 | (0.84 – 1.34) |
Smoking before moved to neighborhood | ||||||||||||
Never smoked | 1.00 | 1.00 | ||||||||||
Ever smoked/tried smoking | 0.07 | 0.18 | 1.08 | (0.75 – 1.54) | 0.06 | 0.18 | 1.06 | (0.74 – 1.52) | ||||
Weekly/daily smoker | 2.83 | 0.15 | 16.97 | (12.73 – 22.62) | 2.81 | 0.15 | 16.57 | (12.43 – 22.09) | ||||
Individual perception of smoking in neighborhood | ||||||||||||
No adults smoke | 1.00 | |||||||||||
Few adults smoke | 1.29 | 0.65 | 3.64 | (1.03 – 12.88) | ||||||||
Some adults smoke | 1.55 | 0.65 | 4.73 | (1.32 – 16.99) | ||||||||
Most adults smoke | 1.85 | 0.68 | 6.34 | (1.68 – 23.87) | ||||||||
All adults smoke | 1.69 | 0.72 | 5.43 | (1.31 – 22.4) | ||||||||
Don’t know | 1.51 | 0.66 | 4.54 | (1.25 – 16.51) | ||||||||
Individual collective efficacy perception | 0.04 | 0.07 | 1.04 | (0.91 – 1.19) |
Betas and ORs for a 2 standard deviation decrease in neighborhood collective efficacy
Models additionally adjusted for age, race/ethnicity, sex, marital status, place of birth, education, income, employment, years lived in the neighborhood, survey language
In the second set of models, presented in Table 3, we examined the relation between neighborhood anti-smoking norms and smoking. In a bivariable GEE logistic regression model, more permissive neighborhood smoking norms were significantly associated with more current smoking (OR 1.82 for a 2 SD increase in permissiveness of smoking norms, 95% CI 1.48–2.24). After adjustment for demographic and socioeconomic confounders as well as smoking history, the association was somewhat attenuated (OR 1.67, 95% CI 1.32–2.13). After further adjustment for individual perceptions of smoking in the neighborhood and individual smoking norms, the association between anti-smoking norms and current smoking was more attenuated but remained significant (OR 1.34, 95% CI 1.03–1.74). When considering effect modification, we found interaction between history of smoking and anti-smoking norms (Table 3, model 4). Specifically, there was mainly an association between anti-smoking norms and current smoking among those with no previous history of smoking prior to residence in the current neighborhood (OR 2.88, 95% CI 1.92–4.30).
Table 3.
Model 1 | Model 2b | Model 3b | Model 4b,c | |||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta | SE | OR | 95% CI | Beta | SE | OR | 95% CI | Beta | SE | OR | 95% CI | Beta | SE | OR | 95% CI | |
Neighborhood smoking norms a | 0.60 | 0.11 | 1.82 | (1.48 – 2.24) | 0.51 | 0.12 | 1.67 | (1.32 – 2.12) | 0.29 | 0.13 | 1.34 | (1.03 – 1.74) | 1.06 | 0.21 | 2.88 | (1.92 – 4.3) |
Smoking before moved to neighborhood | ||||||||||||||||
Never smoked | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | |||||||||||||
Ever smoked/tried smoking | −0.04 | 0.17 | 0.96 | (0.69 –1.34) | −0.14 | 0.19 | 0.87 | (0.6 – 1.25) | −0.11 | 0.18 | 0.90 | (0.63 – 1.28) | ||||
Weekly/daily smoker | 2.82 | 0.15 | 16.82 | (12.51 – 22.62) | 2.70 | 0.16 | 14.90 | (10.99 – 20.2) | 2.72 | 0.15 | 15.21 | (11.33 – 20.41) | ||||
Individual perception of smoking in neighborhood | ||||||||||||||||
No adults smoke | 1.00 | 1.00 | ||||||||||||||
Few adults smoke | 1.43 | 0.65 | 4.17 | (1.17 – 14.87) | 1.39 | 0.66 | 4.00 | (1.09 – 14.65) | ||||||||
Some adults smoke | 1.60 | 0.66 | 4.96 | (1.36 – 18.02) | 1.57 | 0.67 | 4.82 | (1.29 – 18.08) | ||||||||
Most adults smoke | 1.91 | 0.7 | 6.73 | (1.72 – 26.35) | 1.88 | 0.72 | 6.52 | (1.6 – 26.52) | ||||||||
All adults smoke | 1.92 | 0.75 | 6.80 | (1.57 – 29.49) | 1.89 | 0.77 | 6.65 | (1.48 – 29.8) | ||||||||
Don’t know | 1.28 | 0.66 | 3.59 | (0.98 – 13.15) | 1.24 | 0.67 | 3.46 | (0.92 – 12.97) | ||||||||
Individual smoking norm | ||||||||||||||||
No opinion | 1.00 | 1.00 | ||||||||||||||
Acceptable | 0.48 | 0.16 | 1.62 | (1.19 – 2.19) | 0.46 | 0.16 | 1.59 | (1.16 – 2.18) | ||||||||
Unacceptable | −1.47 | 0.12 | 0.23 | (0.18 – 0.29) | −1.47 | 0.12 | 0.23 | (0.18 – 0.29) | ||||||||
Missing | 0.01 | 0.3 | 1.01 | (0.56 – 1.81) | 0.04 | 0.29 | 1.04 | (0.59 – 1.84) | ||||||||
Smoking history * smoking norms | ||||||||||||||||
Never smoked * smoking norms | ||||||||||||||||
Ever smoked/tried smoking * smoking norms | −1.23 | 0.42 | - | |||||||||||||
Weekly/daily smoker * smoking norms | −1.18 | 0.27 | - |
Betas and ORs for a 2 standard deviation decrease in neighborhood anti-smoking norms (increase in permissiveness of smoking norms)
Models additionally adjusted for age, race/ethnicity, sex, marital status, place of birth, education, income, employment, years lived in the neighborhood, survey language
ORs are not presented for some betas in model 4 because they must be interpreted with the interaction. The OR for the association between neighborhood smoking norms and smoking among those who never smoked before moving to the neighborhood is 2.88, as presented in the table, because never smoked is the reference group. The OR among those who tried smoking is e(1.06–1.23) = 0.84 (95% CI 0.44–1.60). The OR among those who were weekly or daily smokers is e(1.06–1.18) = 0.89 (95% CI 0.62–1.26).
In the final part of the analysis, we examined the interaction of neighborhood collective efficacy and neighborhood anti-smoking norms in association with smoking. As neighborhood anti-smoking norms were mainly associated with smoking among those with no history of smoking prior to residence in their current neighborhood, this analysis was initially conducted with the full population, and later considered in subgroups depending on smoking history. In the full population, there was some suggestion of the pattern hypothesized, however it was not statistically significant after adjustment for confounders. However in the subgroup with no history of smoking, the pattern was strong and consistent with the hypothesis. Among those with no prior history of smoking, the association between collective efficacy and smoking depended on the levels of anti-smoking norms in the neighborhood in fully adjusted models. As shown in Figure 1, in neighborhoods with permissive smoking norms, higher collective efficacy was associated with more smoking. In contrast, in neighborhoods with strong anti-smoking norms, higher collective efficacy was associated with less smoking. The association between collective efficacy and smoking was modified by the smoking norms in the neighborhood.
4. Discussion
Using data from a representative survey of New York City residents, we found a combined association of neighborhood collective efficacy and smoking norms with smoking – where norms were permissive about smoking, higher collective efficacy strengthened this association and individual odds of smoking were the highest; where norms were strongly anti-smoking, higher collective efficacy was protective and individual odds of smoking were the lowest. This relation was strongest among those with no prior history of smoking. These findings corroborate qualitative research on this topic (Stead et al., 2001), and support discussions of the importance of considering norms around behavior in conjunction with community cohesion and social control (Portes, 1998).
In this analysis we adjusted for individual demographic and socioeconomic characteristics, as well as individual perceptions of neighborhood collective efficacy and individual smoking norms. Considering smoking norms, this means that the norms of the community are associated with smoking, beyond an individual’s norm or opinion about smoking. For example, someone might believe it is acceptable to smoke, but because there are strong attitudes against smoking in the community, they may be less likely to smoke themselves. It is also important to note that individual norms are naturally a pathway through which community norms might influence individual smoking behaviors, so the estimates of associations between neighborhood norms and smoking adjusting for individual norms are likely to be conservative.
These findings suggest that efforts aimed at building collective efficacy as a way to improve health may also need to consider and target other characteristics of communities to accomplish the intended goal. In this instance, building collective efficacy without considering or addressing the norms about smoking might have the opposite of the intended effect if norms about smoking are permissive (Stead et al., 2001). A more complete assessment of the social environment may give us greater understanding of the range of issues that need to be considered in understanding the influences on behavior and in planning any sort of intervention. Of course, an association observed between existing community exposures and outcomes does not necessarily represent how a community might respond to an intervention on an exposure – this is one of the greatest challenges in the interpretation of community level research (Oakes, 2004, Sampson, 2003). However, this research has the potential to give insight into the types of community level factors that would be important to consider in intervention planning. In addition, aspects of the structural environment such as availability of cigarettes, pricing and taxation of cigarettes, and advertising would likely be important to consider in conjunction with aspects of the social environment examined here.
It is also notable that the associations between neighborhood norms, collective efficacy and smoking were strongest among those who did not have any history of smoking prior to living in the current neighborhood. Those with no prior history of smoking comprise more than half of respondents, so at the population level this represents a substantial group affected. Within this group, ten percent started smoking since living in their current neighborhood. The observation that neighborhood norms and collective efficacy were strongly related to smoking for those with no prior history of smoking suggests that these may be important factors in smoking initiation. However, the absence of associations shown between neighborhood smoking norms and smoking does not mean that norms do not matter at all for those with a history of smoking. It may be particularly interesting to consider norms in relation to quitting for those with a smoking history.
This analysis raises the issue of the scale(s) at which the process of collective efficacy operates, as earlier analyses have typically considered neighborhoods smaller than those in this analysis (Sampson et al., 1997). The large neighborhood areas used in our analysis are consistent with what most of our respondents reported thinking of as their neighborhoods, residential density is high in NYC, and New Yorkers walk long distances as part of daily life, all suggesting our large neighborhood areas are relevant neighborhoods for residents. In addition, seminal work on collective efficacy and related constructs did not conceptualize them as relevant to one specific size of place, and the construct has been considered in units widely varied in size including countries, counties and neighborhoods (Bandura, 2001, Durkheim, 1897, Faris and Dunham, 1939, Sampson et al., 1997, Shaw and McKay, 1942). However, measuring the constructs of collective efficacy and smoking norms at this larger neighborhood level certainly may smooth over heterogeneity in these measures that could have been observed at a smaller level of resolution. Overall, examining these and other constructs at different scales is informative as to the scales and ways in which they may operate on health and health behavior, given that the processes operating in a small neighborhood may be different from those in a large neighborhood area.
It will be interesting for future work to examine whether these results are replicated in other cultural settings. This analysis was inspired by qualitative work in Scotland (Stead et al., 2001), and our results were consistent with those findings suggesting the phenomenon is not unique to the United States context. However, it will also be interesting to consider the combined effects of norms and collective efficacy on substance use in settings in which the range of norms and collective efficacy might be quite different; for example, in places with very low collective efficacy the effect of group norms might be attenuated as limited interactions lead to limited exposure to the norms of others. Certainly research in a variety of countries demonstrating heterogeneous effects of measures of social capital and cohesion among cultural population subgroups suggests the potential for combined effects of measures of capital and cohesion with other cultural aspects in shaping health and health behavior (Baron-Epel et al., 2008, Kim and Kawachi, 2006, Pronyk et al., 2008).
There are several limitations to this study. The cooperation percentage was 54%, which is consistent with many other recent telephone based studies (Galea and Tracy, 2007). However, this cooperation percentage does raise concern about how well the study sample represents the city of New York. Participants were informed that they would be participating in a “survey about the neighborhoods where New Yorkers live and what people think about their neighborhoods”, and thus they were not likely to refuse based on their use or non-use of cigarettes. The distribution of demographic characteristics such as age, race, gender and birth place is very similar to the 2000 Census data for NYC. Moreover the findings were robust to additional weighting to adjust the respondents to the age, race/ethnicity, and gender distribution within each neighborhood as determined from the 2000 Census data, suggesting non-response did not distort the study findings. However the participants may still differ from those in the City overall in ways that we were unable to capture. The data on smoking behaviors in this study were self-reported, and while self-report is standard practice in substance use research, there may be differences between actual behaviors and reported behaviors. The neighborhood smoking norm variable was based on a question originally designed to assess individual norms, and thus represents only one of many ways these norms could have been assessed. The issue of contagion has been raised in recent discussions of analyses that examine community characteristics and individual outcomes (Oakes, 2004). Essentially, this problem arises when the prevalence of the outcome (in this case smoking) affects the probability of the outcome for any individual (i.e., prevalence affects incidence). This dependence of the outcomes between individuals means that parameter estimates from a traditional analysis do not accurately reflect how much of a change in outcome would be expected from a change in exposure (Koopman and Longini, 1994). A complementary analytic approach that merits consideration for future analyses of neighborhood characteristics and health would be one that can account for these dynamic processes, such as the systems modeling approaches that have been used to model infectious diseases (Koopman and Longini, 1994, Ness et al., 2007).
Among several strengths, this study includes a large population based sample. We assessed the extent of stratification using propensities and found that there was virtually no stratification in this analysis. We also accounted for one contributor to social selection by adjusting for history of smoking prior to each person’s residence in their current neighborhood. Social selection has been considered one of the major barriers to determining whether the environment has an influence on people, or whether people who have worse health “drift” or select into worse types of environments (Oakes, 2004, Sampson, 2003). Because we adjusted for history of smoking, associations observed are not due to an effect of smokers moving into certain types of neighborhoods (e.g., neighborhoods with permissive smoking norms). In addition, we adjusted for individual perception of smoking in the neighborhood, assuring that any association with neighborhood smoking norms is not explained by residents observing more smoking behavior in certain neighborhoods.
Overall, we found that two aspects of the social environment, collective efficacy and smoking norms, were associated with current smoking, and that these two elements of the environment had to be considered together to fully understand their relation with smoking behavior. This and other analyses that consider the complexities of neighborhoods, such as the combinations of characteristics that may shape their effects, may ultimately give us a richer understanding of how the social and structural aspects of the environment shape health and health behavior.
Footnotes
Of the 4000 respondents, 93.1% (3725) were geocoded using address (2859) or nearest cross-streets (866). For the remaining 275 we had insufficient address or cross-street information, or only had zip code information. These participants were linked to the neighborhood that had the largest percentage of overlap with their zip code (98.5% had more than 50% overlap between the zip code and the neighborhood, 68.7% had more than 75% overlap). An indicator for linkage to the neighborhood by zip code instead of by geocoding was considered in all analyses as a potential confounder.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Ahern J, Galea S. Social context and depression after a disaster: the role of income inequality. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60:766–770. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.042069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahern J, Galea S, Hubbard A, Karpati A. Population vulnerabilities and capacities related to health: a test of a model. Soc Sci Med. 2008a;66:691–703. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahern J, Galea S, Hubbard A, Midanik L, Syme SL. “Culture of drinking” and individual problems with alcohol use. Am J Epidemiol. 2008b;167:1041–1049. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective. Annual Reviews of Psychology. 2001;52:1–26. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Epel O, Weinstein R, Haviv-Mesika A, Garty-Sandalon N, Green MS. Individual-level analysis of social capital and health: a comparison of Arab and Jewish Israelis. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66:900–910. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.10.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein KT, Galea S, Ahern J, Tracy M, Vlahov D. The built environment and alcohol consumption in urban neighborhoods. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;91:244–252. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chuang Y-C, Cubbin C, Ahn D, Winkleby MA. Effects of neighbourhood socioeconomic status and convenience store concentration on individual level smoking. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2005a;59 doi: 10.1136/jech.2004.029041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chuang Y-C, Ennett ST, Bauman KE, Foshee VA. Neighborhood influences on adolescent cigarette and alcohol use: mediating effects through parent and peer behaviors. J Health Soc Behav. 2005b;46:187–204. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cubbin C, Sundquist K, Ahlen H, Johansson S-E, Winkleby MA, Sundquist J. Neighborhood deprivation and cardiovascular disease risk factors: protective and harmful effects. Scand J Public Health. 2006;34:228–237. doi: 10.1080/14034940500327935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Datta GD, Subramanian S, Colditz GA, Kawachi I, Palmer JR, Rosenberg L. Individual, neighborhood, and state-level predictors of smoking among US Black women: a multilevel analysis. Soc Sci Med. 2006;63:1034–1044. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davey Smith G, Hart C, Watt G, Hole D, Hawthorne V. Individual social class, area-based deprivation, cardiovascular disease risk factors, and mortality: the Renfrew and Paisley study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1998;52:399–405. doi: 10.1136/jech.52.6.399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diez Roux AV. Estimating neighborhood health effects: the challenges of causal inference in a complex world. Soc Sci Med. 2004;58:1953–1960. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00414-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diez Roux AV, Nieto FJ, Mutaner C, Tyroler HA, Comstock GW, Shahar E, Cooper LS, Watson RL, Szklo M. Neighborhood environments and coronary heart disease: A multilevel analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 1997;146:48–63. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan C, Jones K, Moon G. Smoking and deprivation: Are there neighbourhood effects? Soc Sci Med. 1999;48:497–505. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(98)00360-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durkheim E. Suicide: a study in sociology. Free Press; New York: 1897. [Google Scholar]
- Echeverria SE, Diez-Roux AV, Link BG. Reliability of self-reported neighborhood characteristics. J Urban Health. 2004;81:682–701. doi: 10.1093/jurban/jth151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ecob R, Macintyre S. Small area variations in health related behaviours; do these depend on the behavior itself, its measurement, or on personal characteristics. Health Place. 2000;6:261–274. doi: 10.1016/s1353-8292(00)00008-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg ME, Forster JL. Adolescent smoking behavior. Measures of social norms. Am J Prev Med. 2003;25:122–128. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(03)00116-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellickson PL, Bird CE, Orlando M, Klein DJ, McCaffrey DF. Social context and adolescent health behavior: does school-level smoking prevalence affect students’ subsequent smoking behavior? J Health Soc Behav. 2003;44:525–535. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ennett ST, Flewelling RL, Lindrooth RC, Norton EC. School and neighborhood characteristics associated with school rates of alcohol, cigarette, and marijuana use. J Health Soc Behav. 1997;38:55–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faris RE, Dunham W. Mental disorders in urban areas: an ecological study of schizophrenia and other psychoses. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 1939. [Google Scholar]
- Frohlich KL, Potvin L, Gauvin L, Chabot P. Youth smoking initiation: Disentangling context from composition. Health Place. 2002;8:155–166. doi: 10.1016/s1353-8292(02)00003-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galea S, Ahern J, Nandi A, Tracy M, Beard J, Vlahov D. Urban neighborhood poverty and the incidence of depression in a population-based cohort study. Ann Epidemiol. 2007;17:171–179. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2006.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galea S, Ahern J, Rudenstine S, Wallace Z, Vlahov D. Urban built environment and depression: a multilevel analysis. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2005;59:822–827. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.033084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galea S, Ahern J, Vlahov D, Coffin PO, Fuller C, Leon AC, Tardiff K. Income distribution and risk of fatal drug overdose in New York City neighborhoods. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2003;70:139–148. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(02)00342-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galea S, Tracy M. Participation rates in epidemiologic studies. Ann Epidemiol. 2007;17:643–653. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2007.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilpin EA, Lee L, Pierce JP. Changes in population attitudes about where smoking should not be allowed: California versus the rest of the USA. Tob Control. 2004;13:38–44. doi: 10.1136/tc.2003.004739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greiner KA, Li C, Kawachi I, Hunt DC, Ahluwalia JS. The relationships of social participation and community ratings to health and health behaviors in areas with high and low population density. Soc Sci Med. 2004;59 doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hembree C, Galea S, Ahern J, Tracy M, Markham Piper T, Miller J, Vlahov D, Tardiff KJ. The urban built environment and overdose mortality in New York City neighborhoods. Health Place. 2005;11:147–156. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2004.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiles R, Hughes E, Murphy W, Flowers N, McCracken M, Roberts H, Ochner M, Balluz L, Mokdad A, Elam-Evans L, Giles W. Surveillance for certain health behaviors among states and selected local areas--Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System, United States, 2003. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2005;54:1–116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. National Institute on Drug Abuse. II 2006. Monitoring the Future, national survey results on drug use 1975–2005. [Google Scholar]
- Kawachi I, Berkman L. Social cohesion, social capital, and health. In: Berkman L, Kawachi I, editors. Social Epidemiology. Oxford University Press; New York: 2000. pp. 174–190. [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Ustun TB. The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative Version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2004;13:93–121. doi: 10.1002/mpr.168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim D, Kawachi I. A multilevel analysis of key forms of community- and individual-level social capital as predictors of self-rated health in the United States. J Urban Health. 2006;83:813–826. doi: 10.1007/s11524-006-9082-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleinschmidt I, Hills M, Elliott P. Smoking behaviour can be predicted by neighbourhood deprivation measures. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1995;49:S72–77. doi: 10.1136/jech.49.suppl_2.s72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koopman J, Longini I. The ecological effect of individual exposures and nonlinear disease dynamics in populations. Am J Public Health. 1994;84:836–842. doi: 10.2105/ajph.84.5.836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kushner HI, Sterk CE. The limits of social capital: Durkheim, suicide, and social cohesion. Am J Public Health. 2005;95:1139–1143. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.053314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lomas J. Social capital and health: implications for public health and epidemiology. Soc Sci Med. 1998;47:1181–1188. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(98)00190-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miles R. Neighborhood disorder and smoking: findings of a European urban survey. Soc Sci Med. 2006;63:2464–2475. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mokdad AH, Marks J, Stroup D, Gerberding J. Actual causes of death in the United States, 2000. JAMA. 2004;291:1238–1245. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.10.1238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monden CW, van Lenthe FJ, Mackenback JP. A simultaneous analysis of neighbourhood and childhood socio-economic environment with self-assessed health and health-related behaviours. Health Place. 2006;12:394–403. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2005.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nandi A, Galea S, Ahern J, Bucciarelli A, Vlahov D, Tardiff K. What explains the association between neighborhood-level income inequality and the risk of fatal overdose in New York City? Soc Sci Med. 2006;63:662–674. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson D, Marcus S, Wells H, Laird G, Dever J. Cigarette smoking in 99 metropolitan areas - United States 2000. MMWR. 2001;50:1107–1113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson DE, Powell-Griner E, Town M, Kovar MG. A comparison of national estimates from the National Health Interview Survey and the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System. Am J Public Health. 2003;93:1335–1341. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.8.1335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ness RB, Koopman JS, Roberts MS. Causal system modeling in chronic disease epidemiology: a proposal. Ann Epidemiol. 2007;17:564–568. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2006.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- NSDUH, National Survey on Drug Use and Health. [accessed on February 4, 2008]; doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2008.09.005. http://www.oas.samhsa.gov/nhsda.htm. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Oakes JM. The (mis)estimation of neighborhood effects: Causal inference for a practicable social epidemiology. Soc Sci Med. 2004;58:1929–1952. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohlander E, Vikstrom M, Lindstrom M, Sundquist K. Neighbourhood non-employment and daily smoking: a population-based study of women and men in Sweden. Eur J Public Health. 2006;16:78–84. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cki047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patterson JM, Eberly LE, Ding Y, Hargreaves M. Associations of smoking prevalence with individual and area level social cohesion. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004;58:692–697. doi: 10.1136/jech.2003.009167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierce JP, Gilpin EA, Emery SL, White MM, Rosbrook B, Berry CC, Farkas AJ. Has the California tobacco control program reduced smoking? JAMA. 1998;280:893–899. doi: 10.1001/jama.280.10.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Portes A. Social capital: its origins and applications in modern sociology. Annu Rev Sociol. 1998;24:1–24. [Google Scholar]
- Pronyk PM, Harpham T, Morison LA, Hargreaves JR, Kim JC, Phetla G, Watts CH, Porter JD. Is social capital associated with HIV risk in rural South Africa? Soc Sci Med. 2008;66:1999–2010. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.01.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reijneveld S. The impact of individual and area characteristics on urban socioeconomic differences in health and smoking. Int J Epidemiol. 1998;27:33–40. doi: 10.1093/ije/27.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reijneveld S. Neighbourhood socioeconomic context and self reported health and smoking: a secondary analysis of data on seven cities. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2002;56:935–942. doi: 10.1136/jech.56.12.935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robert SA. Socioeconomic position and health: the independent contribution of community to socioeconomic context. Annu Rev Sociol. 1999;25:489–516. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenbaum PR, Rubin DB. The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika. 1983;70:41–55. [Google Scholar]
- Ross CE. Walking, exercising, and smoking: does neighborhood matter? Soc Sci Med. 2000;51:265–274. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(99)00451-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ. Neighborhood-level context and health: lessons from sociology. In: Kawachi I, Berkman LF, editors. Neighborhoods and Health. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2003. pp. 132–146. [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, Earls F. Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science. 1997;277:918–924. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5328.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw CR, McKay HD. Juvenile delinquency and urban areas. The University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 1942. [Google Scholar]
- Stead M, MacAskill S, MacKintosh A-M, Reece J, Eadie D. “It’s as if you’re locked in”: qualitative explanations for area effects on smoking in disadvantaged communities. Health Place. 2001;7:333–343. doi: 10.1016/s1353-8292(01)00025-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sundquist J, Malmstrom M, Johansson S-E. Cardiovascular risk factors and the neighbourhood environment: A multilevel analysis. Int J Epidemiol. 1999;28:841–845. doi: 10.1093/ije/28.5.841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trosclair A, Caraballo R, Marlarcher A, Husten C, Pechacek T. Cigarette smoking among adults - United States, 2003. MMWR. 2005;54:509–513. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tseng M, Yeatts K, Millikan R, Newman B. Area-level characteristics and smoking in women. Am J Public Health. 2001;91:1847–1850. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.11.1847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Lenthe FJ, Mackenback JP. Neighbourhood and individual socioeconomic inequalities in smoking: the role of physical neighbourhood stressors. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60:699–705. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.043851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vartiainen E, Seppala T, Lillsunde P, Puska P. Validation of self reported smoking by serum cotinine measurement in a community-based study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2002;56:167–170. doi: 10.1136/jech.56.3.167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeger SL, Liang KY, Albert PS. Models for longitudinal data: a generalized estimating equation approach. Biometrics. 1988;44:1049–1060. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]