Abstract
Objectives
To estimate the prevalence and workplace consequences of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
Methods
Ann ADHD screen was administered to 18–44 year-old respondents in ten national surveys in the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative (n = 7075 in paid or self employment; response rate 45.9–87.7% across countries). Blinded clinical reappraisal interviews were administered in the US to calibrate the screen. Days out of role were measured in the WHO Disability Assessment Schedule (WHO-DAS). Questions were also asked about ADHD treatment.
Results
An average of 3.5% of workers in the ten countries was estimated to meet DSM-IV criteria for adult ADHD (inter-quartile range: 1.3–4.9%). ADHD was more common among males than females and less common among professionals than other workers. ADHD was associated with a statistically significant 22.1 annual days of excess lost role performance compared to otherwise similar respondents without ADHD. No difference in the magnitude of this effect was found by occupation, education, age, gender, or partner status. This effect was most pronounced in Colombia, Italy, Lebanon, and the US. Although only a small minority of workers with ADHD ever received treatment for this condition, higher proportions were treated for comorbid mental-substance disorders.
Conclusions
ADHD is a relatively common condition among working people in the countries studied and is associated with high work impairment in these countries. This impairment, in conjunction with the low treatment rate and the availability of cost-effective therapies, suggests that ADHD would be a good candidate for targeted workplace screening and treatment programs.
Main messages
A high proportion of childhood ADHD persists into adulthood.
An average of 3.5% of workers in nationally representative surveys carried out in 10 countries met criteria for current DSM-IV adult ADHD.
Workers with ADHD have an average 8.4 excess sickness absence days per year and even higher annualized average excess numbers of workdays associated with reduced work quantity (21.7 days) and quality (13.6 days).
Only a small majority of these workers are treated for ADHD despite evidence that such treatment can be quite effective in improving functioning.
Policy implications
ADHD is a good candidate for targeted workplace screening and treatment programs.
Evaluation is needed to determine the extent to which best-practices outreach and treatment interventions would result in improvements in work performance that have a positive return-on-investment from the employer perspective.
Keywords: ADHD, Work loss, Functioning
INTRODUCTION
Although it is now well known that Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) often continues into adulthood,1–3 especially the inattention symptoms,4, 5 adult ADHD has only recently become the focus of clinical attention.6, 7 The same is true of epidemiological research, which has ignored adult ADHD in all but the most recent studies carried out since the development of fully-structured research diagnostic interviews in the early 1980s. Prevalence estimates of adult ADHD were consequently, until recently, based largely on extrapolations from childhood prevalence estimates using information about the proportion of childhood cases that persists into adulthood4, 8–10 or on direct estimation of prevalence in small samples.11–13 These studies produced adult ADHD prevalence estimates in the range 1–6%, suggested that adult ADHD is often seriously impairing,14–16 and found that ADHD is more often seen among the unemployed than the employed.17, 18
An attempt was made to confirm these results with more representative data in the WHO World Mental Health Survey Initiative,19 a series of representative population surveys carried out in 26 countries using a common instrument to assess the prevalence and correlates of mental disorders. An earlier WMH report estimated that the prevalence of DSM-IV Adult ADHD is 3.4% (inter-quartile range: 1.2–7.3%) in the populations ages 18–44 of the ten WMH countries that assessed this disorder.20 That report also documented high comorbidity and substantial role impairment associated with adult ADHD in these countries.
The current report goes beyond that earlier report to estimate the prevalence of ADHD among working people (either employed or self-employed) and the effects of ADHD on role performance. Previous research on these topics was confined to patients in treatment for adult ADHD.21, 22 The current report, in comparison, considers nationally representative samples of people in the WMH countries in order to provide representative data on the burden of ADHD among working people.
METHODS
Sample
Adult ADHD was assessed in ten WMH countries. (Table 1) Three of these ten are classified less developed by the World Bank (Colombia, Lebanon, Mexico). The others are classified developed.23 The surveys were conducted face-to-face by trained lay interviewers in multi-stage household probability samples The weighted average response rate across all countries was 67.9% (range: 45.9–87.7%).
Table 1.
Sample Sizec |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Country | Survey namea |
Design overviewb | Field Dates |
(n1) | (n2) | Response Rated |
Belgium | ESEMeD | Stratified multistage clustered probability sample of individuals residing in households from the national register of Belgium residents. NR | 2001–2 | (15) | (486) | 50.6 |
Colombia | NSMH | Stratified multistage clustered area probability sample of household residents in all urban areas of the country (approximately 73% of the total national population) | 2003 | (22) | (1731) | 87.7 |
France | ESEMeD | Stratified multistage clustered sample of working telephone numbers merged with a reverse directory (for listed numbers). Initial recruitment was by telephone, with supplemental in-person recruitment in households with listed numbers. NR | 2001–2 | (39) | (727) | 45.9 |
Germany | ESEMeD | Stratified multistage clustered probability sample of individuals from community resident registries. NR | 2002–3 | (19) | (621) | 57.8 |
Italy | ESEMeD | Stratified multistage clustered probability sample of individuals from municipality resident registries. NR | 2001–2 | (32) | (853) | 71.3 |
Lebanon | LEBANON | Stratified multistage clustered area probability sample of household residents. NR | 2002–3 | (5) | (595) | 70.0 |
Mexico | M-NCS | Stratified multistage clustered area probability sample of household residents in all urban areas of the country (approximately 75% of the total national population). | 2001–2 | (27) | (1736 ) | 76.6 |
Netherlands | ESEMeD | Stratified multistage clustered probability sample of individuals residing in households that are listed in municipal postal registries. NR | 2002–3 | (22) | (516) | 56.4 |
Spain | ESEMeD | Stratified multistage clustered area probability sample of household residents. NR | 2001–2 | (16) | (960) | 78.6 |
United States | NCS-R | Stratified multistage clustered area probability sample of household residents. NR | 2002–3 | (139) | (3197) | 70.9 |
ESEMeD (The European Study Of The Epidemiology Of Mental Disorders); NSMH (The Colombian National Study of Mental Health); LEBANON (Lebanese Evaluation of the Burden of Ailments and Needs Of the Nation); M-NCS (The Mexico National Comorbidity Survey); NCS-R (The US National Comorbidity Survey Replication).
Most WMH surveys are based on stratified multistage clustered area probability household samples in which samples of areas equivalent to shires in the UK were selected in the first stage followed by one or more subsequent stages of geographic sampling (e.g., towns within shires, blocks within towns, households within blocks) to arrive at a sample of households, in each of which a listing of household members was created and one or two people were selected from this listing to be interviewed. No substitution was allowed when the originally sampled household resident could not be interviewed. These household samples were selected from Census area data in all countries other than France (where telephone directories were used to select households) and the Netherlands (where postal registries were used to select households). Several WMH surveys (Belgium, Germany, Italy) used municipal resident registries to select respondents without listing households. Eight of the 10 WMH surveys considered here are based on nationally representative (NR) household samples, while two others are based on nationally representative household samples in urbanized areas (Colombia, Mexico).
ADHD was assessed only among respondents in the age range 18–44 in the Part II sample of each survey. Our focus is on the subsample of these respondents who were employed at the time of interview. The respondents within this sub-sample who were classified as meeting criteria for DSM-IV Adult ADHD are reported in the column labeled n1, while the total sub-sample of employed Part II respondents in the age range 18–44 are reported in the column labeled n2.
The response rate is calculated as the ratio of the number of households in which an interview was completed to the number of households originally sampled, excluding from the denominator households known not to be eligible either because of being vacant at the time of initial contact or because the residents were unable to speak the designated languages of the survey.
The WMH interview schedule consisted of two parts. All respondents completed Part I, which contained core diagnostic assessments. All Part I respondents who met criteria for a core disorder plus a probability sub-sample of others were administered Part II, which assessed disorders of secondary interest and a wide range of correlates. Adult ADHD was assessed in Part II. The Part II sample was weighted to adjust for the under-sampling of respondents who did not screen positive for any Part II disorders, making the weighted Part II sample representative of the full population.
As one requirement for a diagnosis of ADHD is symptom onset in childhood, it was necessary to ask respondents to provide retrospective reports about their childhood symptoms of inattention and impulsivity. Based on concerns that the accuracy of these reports might be especially low among elderly respondents, the assessment of ADHD was limited to respondents in the age range 18–44. A total of 11,422 respondents in this age range were screened across the ten surveys.
The WMH interview schedule and all other study materials were translated using standardized WHO translation and back-translation protocols and are posted at www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/wmh. Consistent interviewer training and quality control procedures were used in all surveys. Procedures for informed consent, which was obtained in all countries before beginning interviews, as well as for protecting human subjects, were approved and monitored for compliance by the Institutional Review Boards of the organizations coordinating the surveys in each country.
Adult ADHD
The retrospective assessment of childhood ADHD in the WMH surveys was carried out as part of a larger assessment of diverse mental disorders using Version 3.0 of the WHO Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI 3.0).24 The CIDI module that assessed ADHD was based on questions originally developed in the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for DSM-IV (DIS).25 Respondents classified retrospectively as having met full ADHD criteria in childhood were then asked a single question about whether they continued to have any current problems with attention or hyperactivity-impulsivity. As described in more detail elsewhere,20 a clinical reappraisal interview of these respondents was carried out in a probability sub-sample of respondents in the WMH sample in the US,26 using the Adult ADHD Clinical Diagnostic Scale (ACDS) V 1.2,27, 28 a semi-structured interview that includes the ADHD Rating Scale (ADHD-RS)29 for childhood ADHD and an adaptation of the ADHD-RS to assess current adult ADHD. The ADHD-RS has been used in clinical trials of adult ADHD.30, 31 As detailed elsewhere,26 a strong association (with an area under the receiver operating characteristic curve of .86) was found between the questions about ADHD in the main survey and the clinical diagnoses. Based on this result, a transformation rule was developed to convert responses to the CIDI ADHD symptom-recency questions into a predicted probability of adult ADHD for each respondent in the larger samples. This predicted probability was then transformed to a dichotomous case classification by drawing from the binomial distribution separately for each respondent based on their predicted probability. This dichotomous case measure is the outcome used in the current report. As noted below, appropriate statistical techniques were used to adjust estimates of prevalence and significance for the fact that the outcome measures were generated from predicted probability distributions rather than measured directly.
Socio-demographics, role performance and service use
The main focus of analysis was the prevalence and correlates of ADHD. We examined the associations of ADHD with socio-demographic variables, several measures of role performance, and responses to several questions about treatment. The socio-demographic variables included gender, age (18–29, 30–44), education (low, low-medium, high-medium, high), partner status (married or cohabitating versus never married or previously married, the latter including separated, divorced, and widowed), and broad occupational category (professional, white-collar technical, service, blue-collar). The education categories were different for each country and were designed to divide the population into rough quartiles. The occupation categories were based on the International Labor Organization International Standard Classification of Occupations (ISCO) (www.ilo.org/public/english/bureau/stat/isco/index).
Role performance was assessed in the WHO Disability Assessment Schedule (WHO-DAS),32 a battery that includes questions about number of days out of role and quantity-quality of role performance in the 30 days before interview. Days out of role were assessed with the question: “Beginning yesterday and going back 30 days, how many days out of the past 30 were you totally unable to work or carry out your normal activities?” Decreased quantity of work was assessed with the question: “How many days out of the past 30 were you able to work and carry out your normal activities, but had to cut down on what you did or not get as much done as usual?” Decreased quality of work was assessed with the question: “How many days out of the past 30 did you cut back on the quality of your work or how carefully you worked?” Responses to the three questions were analyzed separately and together in a summary measure of overall role performance. The summary measure was created by counting each day out of role as one and each day of decreased quantity-quality of work as one-half day of lost performance. The summary measure was truncated at 30 in the uncommon case where the sum exceeded 30.
We asked about treatment of ADHD as well as more general questions about treatment of any emotional problem. Comparison of the two types of responses allowed us to pinpoint people with ADHD who had received treatment for co-occurring emotional problems but not for ADHD.
Analysis methods
As noted above, a prediction equation estimated in the clinical reappraisal sample was used to generate a probability of DSM-IV adult ADHD for each respondent who was administered the ADHD section in the CIDI. The method of multiple imputation (MI)33 was used to convert these predicted probabilities into dichotomous diagnostic classifications and to adjust significance tests for the fact that the predicted clinical diagnoses are imperfectly related to actual clinical diagnoses. This method is discussed in more detail elsewhere.26 Simple subgroup comparisons of prevalence were used to study socio-demographic correlates of ADHD in a MI logistic regression framework where a dichotomous measure of estimated ADHD was used as the dependent variable. Logits were exponentiated and are reported here as odds-ratios for ease of interpretation. MI linear regression analysis was used to estimate associations of ADHD with lost role performance. In this approach, the dichotomous measure of ADHD was used as an independent variable to predict each of the role performance measures with controls for age, gender, education, and occupation. The coefficients for the regression of work performance on ADHD in these models can be interpreted as the incrementally higher number of days of impaired role performance in the past 30 days associated with ADHD. These estimates were annualized by multiplying them by 12 (i.e., the number of months in a year). These individual-level annualized estimates were projected to the total civilian labor force of each country by multiplying the individual-level coefficients by the ADHD prevalence estimate and size of the labor force in that country.
All analyses were carried out on weighted data, so that all estimates presented here can be interpreted as the estimates for the general population of the different countries. Part I respondents in each survey were weighted to adjust for differential probabilities of selection within and between households and to match sample distributions to population distributions on socio-demographic and geographic variables. The Part II sample was additionally weighted for the under-sampling of Part I respondents without core disorders. Significance tests were estimated using the Taylor series linearization method,34 a design-based method implemented in the SUDAAN software system,35 in order to adjust for this weighting as well as to adjust for the fact that the vast majority of the WMH country-specific sampling designs used geographic clustering. All significance tests used two-sided Wald χ2 tests based on design-corrected MI variance-covariance matrices.
RESULTS
Prevalence
As previously reported,20 the MI prevalence estimate (standard error in parentheses) of current DSM-IV Adult ADHD pooled across all ten of the participating WMH surveys is 3.4% (0.4) (Table 2). The prevalence estimate among workers, in comparison, is 3.5% (0.4) compared to 3.3% (0.5) among other respondents. In the total sample, and in all countries except the USA, estimated prevalence does not differ significantly between workers and other respondents. In the USA, the estimated prevalence of ADHD in the US is significantly lower among workers than other respondents (4.5% vs. 7.2%, χ21 = 5.5, p = .021).
Table 2.
Overall | Employed or self- employed |
All other respondents |
|||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
% | (se) | (n) | % | (se) | (n) | % | (se) | (n) | χ23 | p | |
Belgium | 4.1 | (1.5) | (486) | 3.7 | (1.5) | (347) | 4.8 | (2.5) | (139) | 0.0 | .86 |
Colombia | 1.91 | (0.5) | (1731) | 1.91 | (0.6) | (790) | 1.91 | (0.6) | (941) | 0.0 | .92 |
France | 7.32 | (1.8) | (727) | 6.3 | (1.7) | (533) | 10.0 | (3.6) | (194) | 0.9 | .35 |
Germany | 3.1 | (0.8) | (621) | 3.5 | (1.1) | (432) | 2.1 | (0.8) | (189) | 1.2 | .28 |
Italy | 2.8 | (0.6) | (853) | 3.4 | (0.7) | (569) | 1.61 | (0.7) | (284) | 2.7 | .10 |
Lebanon | 1.81 | (0.7) | (595) | 0.91 | (0.5) | (305) | 2.8 | (1.4) | (290) | 2.9 | .09 |
Mexico | 1.91 | (0.4) | (1736) | 2.4 | (0.7) | (749) | 1.51 | (0.4) | (987) | 1.0 | .32 |
Netherlands | 5.0 | (1.6) | (516) | 4.9 | (1.9) | (389) | 5.3 | (2.8) | (127) | 0.1 | .79 |
Spain | 1.21 | (0.6) | (960) | 1.3 | (0.6) | (574) | 1.01 | (0.8) | (386) | 0.2 | .63 |
USA | 5.2 | (0.6) | (3197) | 4.5* | (0.7) | (2387) | 7.22 | (1.2) | (810) | 5.5 | .021 |
Total | 3.4 | (0.4) | (11,422) | 3.5 | (0.4) | (7075) | 3.3 | (0.5) | (4347) | 0.0 | .99 |
Significant difference in prevalence between employed and not employed at the .05 level, two-sided test.
The upper end of the 95% confidence interval of this estimate is below the prevalence estimate for the total sample.
The lower end of the 95% confidence interval of this estimate is above the prevalence estimate for the total sample.
The prevalence estimate for the US here is somewhat different from the estimate reported in a previous report17 because a somewhat different imputation equation was used in the analyses reported here in order to be consistent with the equation used in the other WMH countries.
Socio-demographic correlates
For all the countries combined, prevalence of ADHD among workers differs significantly by gender and occupation, but not for age, education, or partner status (Table 3). ADHD is more common among males than females, with an odds-ratio (OR) of 1.7. ADHD is less common among professionals than other workers, with the elevated ORs of the other occupational groups relative to professionals in the range between 1.7 (service workers) and 3.0 (white collar technical workers).
Table 3.
Prevalence1 | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
% | (se) | OR* | (95% CI) | χ2 | df | p | |
Gender | |||||||
Male | 4.2 | (0.6) | 1.7* | (1.1–2.0) | 8.6 | 1 | .003 |
Female | 2.5 | (0.4) | 1.0 | ||||
Age | |||||||
18–29 | 3.8 | (0.5) | 1.0 | 0.1 | 1 | .73 | |
30–44 | 3.2 | (0.5) | 0.9 | (0.7–1.3) | |||
Education2 | |||||||
Low | 4.7 | (1.0) | 1.3 | (0.8–2.3) | 4.0 | 3 | .26 |
Low-medium | 4.5 | (0.7) | 1.3 | (0.8–2.1) | |||
High-medium | 3.2 | (0.5) | 1.0 | (0.6–1.5) | |||
High | 1.8 | (0.4) | 1.0 | ||||
Partner status | |||||||
Married/cohabitating | 3.1 | (0.5) | 1.0 | 2.9 | 2 | .23 | |
Separated/widowed/divorced | 4.1 | (0.9) | 1.4 | (0.9–2.1) | |||
Never married | 4.0 | (0.5) | 1.3 | (0.9–1.8) | |||
Occupation | |||||||
Professional | 1.7 | (0.4) | 1.0 | 13.9 | 3 | .003 | |
White collar technical | 5.8 | (1.3) | 3.0* | (1.7–5.5) | |||
Service | 2.9 | (0.4) | 1.7* | (1.0–3.0) | |||
Blue collar | 4.0 | (0.6) | 2.0* | (1.1–2.0) |
Significantly difference from the contrast category at the .05 level, two-sided test
The prevalence of ADHD among respondents in each of the socio-demographic categories
See the text for a definition of the education categories.
Interaction analyses found that no significant between-country differences in the associations of ADHD with either age or gender, significant differences in the associations of ADHD with education (p=0.008), occupation (p = .009), and partner status (p=0.030). Inspection showed that the interaction involving education is due exclusively to respondents in the lowest two education categories having a very low estimated prevalence of ADHD in Colombia. The interaction involving occupation is due to white collar technical workers having an exceptionally high estimated prevalence in the US and blue collar workers having a high estimated prevalence in France and Spain. Occupation is unrelated to ADHD in the other countries. The interaction involving partner status, finally, is due to previously married people having a high estimated prevalence in the Netherlands and never married people having a high estimated prevalence in the US.
ADHD and role performance
ADHD is significantly related to overall role performance in the total sample, with an annualized individual-level regression coefficient of 22.1 days out of role (reference: subjects without ADHD). (Table 4) Days out of role and reduced quantity-quality of role performance are all statistically significant in the combined data. Workers with ADHD have an average excess 8.4 days out of role, 21.7 days of decreased work quantity, and 13.6 days of decreased work quality. Projections of individual-level effects to the civilian labor force yields an estimate that 143.8 million lost days of productivity occur each year in these countries associated with ADHD.
Table 4.
Individual Level |
National level (in million of days) |
||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Absenteeism | Quantity | Quality | Total | Absenteeism | Quantity | Quality | Total | ||||||||||
Days | (se) | Days | (se) | Days | (se) | Days | (se) | Days | (se) | Days | (se) | Days | (se) | Days | (se) | (n) | |
Belgium | 10.8 | (15.1) | 18.3 | (28.9) | 9.4 | (9.4) | 16.5 | (23.8) | 1.1 | (1.6) | 1.9 | (3.0) | 1.0 | (1.0) | 1.7 | (2.5) | (347) |
Colombia | 21.9* | (9.4) | 14.3 | (9.9) | 13.0 | (9.6) | 29.4* | (11.9) | 6.2* | (2.7) | 4.1 | (2.8) | 3.7 | (2.7) | 8.3* | (3.4) | (790) |
France | −1.0 | (3.9) | 24.9 | (27.7) | 20.1 | (12.9) | 20.1 | (17.5) | −1.0 | (4.0) | 25.5 | (28.3) | 20.6 | (13.2) | 20.6 | (17.9) | (533) |
Germany | 13.3 | (14.4) | −3.6 | (4.9) | 3.9 | (4.6) | 13.6 | (14.6) | 10.6 | (11.5) | −2.8 | (3.9) | 3.1 | (3.7) | 10.8 | (11.6) | (432) |
Italy | 7.7 | (9.5) | 25.6* | (12.6) | 6.4 | (6.8) | 22.2 | (13.9) | 4.0 | (5.0) | 13.4* | (6.6) | 3.3 | (3.6) | 11.6 | (7.2) | (569) |
Lebanon | 5.8 | (9.8) | 30.7 | (26.8) | −4.1 | (4.0) | 19.4 | (13.3) | 0.1 | (0.1) | 0.4 | (0.4) | −0.1 | (0.1) | 0.3 | (0.2) | (305) |
Mexico | 5.0 | (4.8) | 5.5 | (6.9) | −0.9 | (1.0) | 6.1 | (6.7) | 2.4 | (2.3) | 2.6 | (3.3) | −0.4 | (0.5) | 2.9 | (3.2) | (749) |
Netherlands | −8.8 | (14.2) | −37.3 | (16.8) | −8.2 | (4.7) | −28.4 | (15.8) | −2.2 | (3.5) | −9.3* | (4.2) | −2.0 | (1.2) | −7.0 | (3.9) | (389) |
Spain | −2.8 | (2.5) | 3.5 | (8.8) | 7.5 | (7.8) | 1.1 | (8.1) | −0.4 | (0.3) | 0.5 | (1.2) | 1.0 | (1.0) | 0.1 | (1.1) | (574) |
U.S | 10.0* | (4.6) | 29.1* | (9.0) | 20.6* | (5.9) | 28.3* | (8.4) | 37.0* | (16.9) | 107.5* | (33.4) | 76.1* | (21.9) | 104.7* | (31.2) | (2387) |
All countries | 8.4* | (2.7) | 21.7* | (5.6) | 13.6* | (3.2) | 22.1* | (4.8) | 54.8* | (17.7) | 141.3* | (36.7) | 88.6* | (20.9) | 143.8* | (31.5) | (7075) |
Significant at the .05 level, two-sided test
Estimates are based on linear regression equations in which the role performance outcomes are regressed on a dummy predictor variable that distinguishes workers estimated to have ADHD from all other workers, controlling for age, gender, education and occupation.
No significant interactions were found in the total sample between ADHD and any socio-demographics in predicting total role performance. However, statistically significant differences were found across country (p=0.044), with the strongest associations in Colombia, Italy, Lebanon, and the US.
It is noteworthy that no controls for comorbidity were introduced into these analyses on days out of role, despite our previous analyses documenting high comorbidity between ADHD and other DSM-IV disorders.20 The reasoning was that ADHD is temporally primary to the vast majority of comorbid disorders, meaning that any attenuation of the associations between ADHD and role performance from controlling comorbid disorders would indicate mediation (i.e., ADHD leading to secondary disorders that, in turn, cause decrements in role performance) rather than spuriousness (i.e., control variables causing both ADHD and decrements in role performance, with ADHD playing no causal role). Nevertheless, it is instructive to investigate the extent to which such controls attenuate the ADHD-impairment associations. The analyses carried out to produce the results in Table 4 were consequently repeated with controls for the other DSM-IV mental-substance disorders in the WMH surveys. The significant individual-level association (standard error in parentheses) between ADHD and overall role performance in the total sample remained significant, but decreased from 22.1 (4.8) days per year to 15.8 (4.7) days per year with the introduction of these controls; meaning that most of the days out of role could be attributed to ADHD and not to the disorders co-occurring with ADHD.
Role performance versus work performance
As noted in the section on measures, the WHO-DAS measures decrements in role performance rather than work performance. It is also possible that respondents counted some regularly scheduled days off work as having role impairment if they had difficulty with household chores or other normal activities because of problems with their physical or mental health. We investigated this issue by reanalyzing the US data, where workers were administered both the WHO-DAS and the HPQ assessment of work performance. In the original analysis (Table 4), the overall annualized association between ADHD and days out of role in the US was 28.3 (8.4) days. In the analysis with the HPQ substituted for the WHO-DAS as the outcome, the annualized association between ADHD and days out of work was 33.5 (10.1) days. The fact that the latter is higher than the former was unexpected. To the extent that the same pattern would hold in other WMH countries, the WHO-DAS analyses reported above yielded estimates of the associations between ADHD and role performance that were conservative relative to work performance.
Treatment
Respondents who screened positive for current ADHD were asked whether they received any professional treatment for their problems with concentration, inattention, or impulsivity at any time in the 12 months before the interview. Very few respondents reported receiving such treatment. (Table 5) Indeed, it was only in the Netherlands, where 2.7% of estimated cases reported receiving such treatment, and in the US, where 12.6% did so, that any respondents estimated to have ADHD reported any treatment for the symptoms of ADHD. However, with the exceptions of Lebanon and Mexico, considerably more respondents of the respondents with ADHD reported receiving treatment for some other emotional problems in the same time period. The proportion of these cases in treatment other than in Lebanon and Mexico, where none did so, is in the range between 3.5% (Belgium) and 9.6% (Colombia) in six of the other countries and much higher in the Netherlands (21.3%) and the US (43.4%).
Table 5.
Treatment for any emotional problem |
|||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
General Medical |
Any Mental Health |
Human Services |
CAM1 | Any treatment for any emotional problems |
Treatment for ADHD |
||||||||
% | (se) | % | (se) | % | (se) | % | (se) | % | (se) | % | (se) | (n)2 | |
Belgium | 2.5 | (5.7) | 3.1 | (7.0) | 0.0 | 0.0 | 3.5 | (7.2) | 0.0 | (15) | |||
Colombia | 1.0 | (1.0) | 8.6 | (7.3) | 0.0 | 2.3 | (2.3) | 9.6 | (7.3) | 0.0 | (22) | ||
France | 2.4 | (1.6) | 4.7 | (2.9) | 0.0 | 0.0 | 5.8 | (3.2) | 0.0 | (39) | |||
Germany | 0.0 | 3.8 | (6.5) | 0.0 | 0.0 | 3.8 | (6.5) | 0.0 | (19) | ||||
Italy | 6.2 | (3.6) | 3.2 | (2.3) | 0.0 | 1.6 | (1.6) | 7.8 | (4.0) | 0.0 | (32) | ||
Lebanon | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 | (5) | ||||||
Mexico | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 | (27) | ||||||
Netherlands | 20.2 | (11.8) | 16.9 | (11.8) | 0.0 | 14.0 | (11.5) | 21.3 | (11.9) | 2.7 | (2.4) | (22) | |
Spain | 4.3 | (3.5) | 4.3 | (3.5) | 0.0 | 0.0 | 4.3 | (3.5) | 0.0 | (16) | |||
USA | 20.0 | (3.5) | 23.7 | (5.1) | 9.1 | (1.8) | 10.6 | (2.9) | 43.4 | (5.1) | 12.6 | (3.9) | (139) |
CAM: complementary and alternative medical treatment (e.g., self help group, chiropractor, faith healer, etc.)
The numbers of respondents with ADHD reported here, when divided by the total numbers of employed respondents in the age range 18–44, which were reported in Table 1, do not reproduce the prevalence estimates reported in Table 1. This is because the sample sizes reported here are unweighted (i.e., they represent the actual numbers of respondents in the various samples), while the prevalence estimates are based on weighted data.
DISCUSSION
Several limitations are noteworthy. First, DSM-IV criteria for ADHD were developed with children in mind and offer only limited guidance regarding adult diagnosis. Clinical studies make it clear that symptoms of ADHD are more heterogeneous and subtle in adults,36, 37 leading some researchers to suggest that assessment of adult ADHD might require an increase in the variety of symptoms assessed,38 reduction in the severity threshold,39 or reduction in the DSM-IV six-of-nine symptom requirement.40 To the extent that such changes would lead to a more valid assessment, our estimates of prevalence and related impairment will be conservative.
Second, adult ADHD was diagnosed based entirely on adult respondent self-report. Childhood ADHD is diagnosed largely from parent and teacher reports because children with ADHD have notoriously little insight into their symptoms.41 Use of informants is much more difficult for adults, though, making it necessary to rely on self-report.37 Methodological studies comparing adult self-reports versus informant reports of adult ADHD symptoms document the same general pattern of under-estimation as in child self-reports,42, 43 suggesting that prevalence is probably under-estimated here.
Third, the MI imputation model used to estimate ADHD in this study was based on a clinical assessment carried out only in the US. We have no way to confirm whether the calibration is as accurate in other countries. This is especially problematic given that little research on adult ADHD has been conducted outside of the US, making it unclear if the same markers apply in other countries. Given the centrality of this issue, it is important for structured assessment of adult ADHD to be expanded for use in future surveys and for the validity of these assessments to be evaluated in clinical reappraisal studies outside the US.
Within the context of these limitations, our results show adult ADHD to be a fairly common disorder in the labor force associated with substantial lost role performance. Our finding that the prevalence of ADHD is generally as high among workers as others was unexpected based on previous clinical research that has generally found patients with ADHD to have a high unemployment rate.18 However, disaggregation found that unemployed respondents have a higher prevalence of ADHD (5.5%) than working people (3.5%), while homemakers (1.9%) and students (2.2%) have the lowest rates.20
The finding that adult ADHD is significantly more prevalent among male than female workers is consistent with much previous general population research.18 The finding that ADHD is less prevalent among professionals than other workers is not surprising given that ADHD interferes with cognitive performance and might create a selection bias against success in professional work. The finding that ADHD is not related to age in the range considered here (i.e., 18–29 vs. 30–44) extends the broader finding that ADHD does not spontaneously remit in early adulthood.17
The finding that adult ADHD appears to be somewhat more prevalent in developed than developing countries could reflect the fact that the notion of a “deficit” existing in attentiveness has to be defined in relation to the level of environment demands on attention. A deficit exists only when demands exceed the person’s abilities. It might be that high environmental demands for attentiveness are more common in the workplaces of developed countries, leading to the higher recognition of adult ADHD in those countries. However, this possibility is only a speculation that should be confirmed with objective cognitive tests before it is accepted as true.
The key finding of the paper is that adult ADHD is associated with significant decrements in role performance. This finding is broadly consistent with much clinical evidence28 and with evidence from neuropsychological studies.44 The magnitude of the associations found here, though, are quite large in relation to comparable estimates reported in the literature for other chronic physical and mental disorders.45–47
It is noteworthy that we found more than half the days out of role associated with ADHD to be due to reduced quantity-quality of role performance rather than to days out of role. This is important from an employer perspective because many employers consider some number of days out of work (typically one per month) part of the cost of doing business and have mechanisms to reduce financial losses due to larger numbers of absence days (e.g., caps on paid sick days, disability insurance). However, employers typically expect their workers to be working when they are on the job. To find that most ADHD-related lost role performance occurs on days in role, then, is both striking and disturbing from an employer perspective.
Although we found statistically significant differences in ADHD prevalence across occupation, no between-occupation difference were found in the association between ADHD and role performance. Specifications involving other demographic variables were also generally not significant. These results suggest that the adverse effects of ADHD are widespread rather than concentrated among workers in jobs where high concentration is critical for success. The unusual finding that the association between ADHD and role performance is positive in the Netherlands is consequently difficult to explain and might be due to the low number of respondents or low estimated prevalence of ADHD in the Netherlands.
Our results regarding treatment of ADHD show clearly that adult ADHD is not recognized as a disorder that requires treatment in most of the countries studied. A much higher proportion of cases in the US and the Netherlands could be detected if professionals treating patients with other emotional problems screened for comorbid ADHD, as sizable minorities of ADHD cases in both countries receive treatment for other emotional problems. In the other countries, though, only small proportions of ADHD cases receive treatment for any emotional problem.
The above results raise the question whether adult ADHD is a candidate for targeted workplace screening and treatment programs. Short screening scales that are both sensitive and specific for adult ADHD exist.48, 49 It might be cost-effective from the employer perspective to implement workplace screening programs with such a scale to detect and provide treatment for workers with ADHD. The thinking here is that ADHD among workers has nontrivial prevalence, high impairment, and a low rate of treatment, whereas cost-effective therapies exist that are related to improvements in some objective aspects of role performance.50–52 The obvious next step from a public health perspective, given these findings is to evaluate the extent to which best-practices outreach and treatment would result in improvement in functioning that might have a positive return-on-investment for employers.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The surveys discussed in this article were carried out in conjunction with the World Health Organization World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative. We thank the WMH staff for assistance with instrumentation, fieldwork, and data analysis. These activities were supported by the United States National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH070884), the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, the Pfizer Foundation, the US Public Health Service (R13-MH066849, R01-MH069864, and R01 DA016558), the Fogarty International Center (FIRCA R01-TW006481), the Pan American Health Organization, Eli Lilly and Company, Ortho-McNeil Pharmaceutical, Inc., GlaxoSmithKline, and Bristol-Myers Squibb Company. A complete list of WMH publications can be found at http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/wmh/. The Colombian National Study of Mental Health (NSMH) is supported by the Ministry of Social Protection. The ESEMeD project is funded by the European Commission (Contracts QLG5-1999-01042; SANCO 2004123), the Piedmont Region (Italy), Fondo de Investigación Sanitaria, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Spain (FIS 00/0028), Ministerio de Ciencia y Tecnología, Spain (SAF 2000-158-CE), Departament de Salut, Generalitat de Catalunya, Spain, Instituto de Salud Carlos III (CIBER CB06/02/0046, RETICS RD06/0011 REM-TAP), and other local agencies and by an unrestricted educational grant from GlaxoSmithKline. The Lebanese National Mental Health Survey (LEBANON) is supported by the Lebanese Ministry of Public Health, the WHO (Lebanon), anonymous private donations to IDRAAC, Lebanon, and unrestricted grants from Janssen Cilag, Eli Lilly, GlaxoSmithKline, Roche, and Novartis. The Mexican National Comorbidity Survey (MNCS) is supported by The National Institute of Psychiatry Ramon de la Fuente (INPRFMDIES 4280) and by the National Council on Science and Technology (CONACyT-G30544- H), with supplemental support from the PanAmerican Health Organization (PAHO). The US National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) is supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH; U01-MH60220) with supplemental support from the National Institute of Drug Abuse (NIDA), the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA), the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (RWJF; Grant 044708), and the John W. Alden Trust.
DECLARATION OF INTEREST
Preparation of this report was supported, in part, by an unrestricted educational grant from Eli Lilly and Company in addition to the core WMH funders. Eli Lilly staff were sent an information copy of the paper at the time of submission, but were not involved either in designing the study, carrying out analyses, interpreting results, preparing the report, or deciding on whether or not the paper should be published.
REFERENCES
- 1.Barkley RA. Major life activity and health outcomes associated with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2002;63 Suppl 12:10–15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, et al. Adult outcome of hyperactive boys. Educational achievement, occupational rank, and psychiatric status. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:565–576. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820190067007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Weiss G, Hechtman L. Hyperactive Children Grown Up: ADHD in Children, Adolescents, and Adults. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Biederman J, Mick E, Faraone SV. Age-dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: impact of remission definition and symptom type. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:816–818. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Swanson JM, Sergeant JA, Taylor E, et al. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic disorder. Lancet. 1998;351:429–433. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Pary R, Lewis S, Matuschka PR, et al. Attention deficit disorder in adults. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2002;14:105–111. doi: 10.1023/a:1016807021779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wilens TE, Biederman J, Spencer TJ. Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Ann Rev Med. 2002;53:113–131. doi: 10.1146/annurev.med.53.082901.103945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, et al. The persistence of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder into young adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition of disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111:279–289. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, et al. Adult psychiatric status of hyperactive boys grown up. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:493–498. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.4.493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Weiss G, Hechtman L, Milroy T, et al. Psychiatric status of hyperactives as adults: a controlled prospective 15-year follow-up of 63 hyperactive children. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1985;24:211–220. doi: 10.1016/s0002-7138(09)60450-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Heiligenstein E, Conyers LM, Berns AR, et al. Preliminary normative data on DSM-IV attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in college students. J Am Coll Health. 1998;46:185–188. doi: 10.1080/07448489809595609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Murphy K, Barkley RA. Prevalence of DSM-IV symptoms of ADHD in adult licensed drivers: Implications for clinical diagnosis. J Atten Disord. 1996;3:147–161. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Weiss M, Murray C. Assessment and management of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. Can Med Assoc J. 2003;168:715–722. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, et al. Educational and occupational outcome of hyperactive boys grown up. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:1222–1227. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199709000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.McGough JJ, Barkley RA. Diagnostic controversies in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1948–1956. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.11.1948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Murphy K, Barkley RA. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder adults: comorbidities and adaptive impairments. Compr Psychiatry. 1996;37:393–401. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(96)90022-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, et al. The prevalence and effects of adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder on work performance in a nationally representative sample of workers. J Occup Environ Med. 2005;47:565–572. doi: 10.1097/01.jom.0000166863.33541.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Scahill L, Schwab-Stone M. Epidemiology of ADHD in school-age children. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2000;9:545–555. vii. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kessler RC, Haro JM, Heeringa SG, et al. The World Health Organization World Mental Health Survey Initiative. Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. 2006;15:161–166. doi: 10.1017/s1121189x00004395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Fayyad J, De Graaf R, Kessler R, et al. Cross-national prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;190:402–409. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.034389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Swensen A, Birnbaum HG, Ben Hamadi R, et al. Incidence and costs of accidents among attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder patients. J Adolesc Health. 2004;35(346):e1–e9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Swensen AR, Birnbaum HG, Secnik K, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: increased costs for patients and their families. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42:1415–1423. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200312000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.World Bank. Washington, DC: The International Bank for Reconstruction and Development/The World Bank; World Development Report 2004: Making Services Work for Poor People. 2003
- 24.Kessler RC, Üstün TB. The World Mental Health (WMH) survey initiative version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2004;13:93–121. doi: 10.1002/mpr.168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Robins LN, Cottler L, Bucholz K, et al. Diagnostic Interview Schedule for DSM-IV. St. Louis, MO: Washington University; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, et al. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;163:716–723. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.4.716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Adler L, Cohen J. Diagnosis and evaluation of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2004;27:187–201. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2003.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Adler L, Spencer T. The Adult ADHD Clinical Diagnostic Scale (ACDS) V 1.2. New York, NY: New York University School of Medicine; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 29.DuPaul GJ, Power TJ, Anastopoulos AD, et al. ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Checklists, Norms, and Clinical Interpretation. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Michelson D, Adler L, Spencer T, et al. Atomoxetine in adults with ADHD: two randomized, placebo-controlled studies. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53:112–120. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01671-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Spencer T, Biederman J, Wilens T, et al. Efficacy of a mixed amphetamine salts compound in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:775–782. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.8.775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Chwastiak LA, Von Korff M. Disability in depression and back pain: evaluation of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule (WHO DAS II) in a primary care setting. J Clin Epidemiol. 2003;56:507–514. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(03)00051-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rubin DB. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Wolter KM. Introduction to Variance Estimation. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag; 1985. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Research Triangle Institute. SUDAAN: Professional Software for Survey Data Analysis [computer program]. Version 8.01. Research Triangle Park, NC: Research Triangle Institute; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 36.De Quiros GB, Kinsbourne M. Adult ADHD. Analysis of self-ratings on a behavior questionnaire. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2001;931:140–147. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wender PH, Wolf LE, Wasserstein J. Adults with ADHD. An overview. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2001;931:1–16. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Barkley RA. ADHD behavior checklist for adults. The ADHD Report. 1995;3:16. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ratey J, Greenberg S, Bemporad JR, et al. Unrecognized attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults presenting for outpatient psychotherapy. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 1992;4:267–275. doi: 10.1089/cap.1992.2.267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Kooij JJ, Buitelaar JK, van den Oord EJ, et al. Internal and external validity of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in a population-based sample of adults. Psychol Med. 2005;35:817–827. doi: 10.1017/s003329170400337x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Jensen PS, Rubio-Stipec M, Canino G, et al. Parent and child contributions to diagnosis of mental disorder: are both informants always necessary? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38:1569–1579. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199912000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Gittelman R, Mannuzza S. Diagnosing ADD-H in adolescents. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1985;21:237–242. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Zucker M, Morris MK, Ingram SM, et al. Concordance of self- and informant ratings of adults' current and childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Psychol Assess. 2002;14:379–389. doi: 10.1037//1040-3590.14.4.379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hervey AS, Epstein JN, Curry JF. Neuropsychology of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. 2004;18:485–503. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Druss BG, Marcus SC, Olfson M, et al. Comparing the national economic burden of five chronic conditions. Health Aff (Millwood) 2001;20:233–241. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.20.6.233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kessler RC, Greenberg PE, Mickelson KD, et al. The effects of chronic medical conditions on work loss and work cutback. J Occup Environ Med. 2001;43:218–225. doi: 10.1097/00043764-200103000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Wang PS, Beck A, Berglund P, et al. Chronic medical conditions and work performance in the health and work performance questionnaire calibration surveys. J Occup Environ Med. 2003;45:1303–1311. doi: 10.1097/01.jom.0000100200.90573.df. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, et al. The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med. 2005;35:245–256. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704002892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kessler RC, Adler LA, Gruber MJ, et al. Validity of the World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS) Screener in a representative sample of health plan members. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2007;16:52–65. doi: 10.1002/mpr.208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Schweitzer JB, Lee DO, Hanford RB, et al. Effect of methylphenidate on executive functioning in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: normalization of behavior but not related brain activity. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56:597–606. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Simpson D, Plosker GL. Spotlight on atomoxetine in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. CNS Drugs. 2004;18:397–401. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200418060-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Turner DC, Clark L, Dowson J, et al. Modafinil improves cognition and response inhibition in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:1031–1040. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]