Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Apr 8.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21(2):519–537. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000285

Maternal depressive symptoms in infancy: Unique contribution to children’s depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence?

Jean-François Bureau 1, M Ann Easterbrooks 2, Karlen Lyons-Ruth 3
PMCID: PMC2666930  NIHMSID: NIHMS77870  PMID: 19338696

Abstract

This prospective 20-year study assessed associations between maternal depressive symptoms in infancy, childhood, and adolescence and child and adolescent depressive symptoms in a sample of families at high psychosocial risk. Maternal symptomatology was assessed with the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) when children were infants (12 months), school-aged (age 8), and adolescents (age 19). Children’s depressive symptoms were measured at age 8 (Dimensions of Depression Profile for Children and Adolescents, Hatter & Nowakowski, 1987) and age 19 (CES-D). Maternal depressive symptoms during infancy contributed to the prediction of child depressive symptoms at age 8, after controlling for concurrent maternal depressive symptoms, clinical family risk in infancy, and gender. Clinical family risk in infancy marginally contributed to the prediction model. Disorganization of attachment in infancy and maternal hostility were independent predictors of depressive symptoms at age 8 and did not mediate the relation between maternal and child depressive symptoms. Depressive symptoms in adolescence were predicted by gender, children’s depressive symptoms at age 8, maternal depressive symptoms in adolescence, and maternal depressive symptoms in infancy. There was no moderating effect of gender. Adding to previous evidence on the importance of early maternal depression, maternal depressive symptoms during infancy were strongly related to the development of depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence.

Keywords: maternal depressive symptoms, child depressive symptoms, attachment, maternal sensitivity


There is now ample empirical evidence that depressive symptoms are not exclusive to adulthood. In fact, depression can be detected in children as young as 5 years of age, if not younger (see Luby, Belden, & Spitznagel, 2006; Luby, Heffelfinger, Mrakotsky, Brown, Hessler, Wallis, & Spitznagel, 2003; Rutter, Izard, & Read, 1986). Depression with childhood onset can have serious long-term developmental consequences, such as poorer school performance, interpersonal conflict, substance abuse, delinquency, and increased risk of suicide during the adolescent years (Elgar, McGrath, Waschbusch, Stewart, & Curtis, 2004). One important precursor of childhood and adolescent depression identified in the literature is the presence of maternal depressive symptoms (Cummings & Davies, 1994; Downey & Coyne, 1990; Goodman & Gotlib, 1999; 2002). Previous research has identified possible mediators (e.g. maternal sensitivity, quality of attachment) and moderators (e.g. gender) of this transmission of depressive symptoms. The purpose of this study was to explore the importance of timing of exposure to maternal depressive symptoms, as well as the relative influence of maternal sensitivity, security and disorganization of children’s attachments, and gender in the prediction of offspring depressive symptoms at 8 and 19 years of age in a low income sample. The use of an understudied risk group with low-income mothers showing a rather high rate of depressive symptoms should improve the likelihood of observing such transmission of symptoms to their offspring across childhood and adolescence.

Intergenerational transmission of depressive symptoms

Past research aimed at identifying the predictors of psycho-social functioning in infancy, childhood and adolescence demonstrates the importance of the occurrence of maternal depression. Research has focused both on heterotypic connections between parental depression and various developmental problems, as well as homotypic links between parental depression and child depression (Burt, Van Dulmen, Carlivati, Egeland, Sroufe, Forman, et al., 2005).

Previous studies have shown associations between maternal and offspring depressive or internalizing symptomatology. For example, a recent meta-analytic review revealed a significant effect size of .16 for the prediction of child internalizing problems from maternal depression (Connell & Goodman, 2002). Results from Lyons-Ruth, Easterbrooks, & Cibelli (1997) further suggest that maternal depressive symptoms are an important overall index linking a climate of family adversity to later internalizing symptoms, capturing variance shared with cumulative demographic risk scores and lowered infant cognitive development. It could be argued, however, that internalizing symptoms are not necessarily the equivalent of depressive symptoms and therefore these results must be interpreted cautiously. In line with this argument, Malcarne, Hamilton, Ingram, and Taylor (2000) demonstrated that, as compared to internalizing and externalizing problems, children’s depressive symptoms were differentially related to maternal variables, including maternal depression and stressful life events. The authors concluded that maternal depression may be specifically associated with affectively similar narrow-band depressive symptoms in children (Malcarne et al., 2000). Other studies found that children or adolescents of depressed mothers reported a higher rate of major depressive disorders (Silk, Shaw, Skuban, Oland, & Kovacs, 2006; Weissman, Wartner, Wickramaratne, Moreau, & Olfson, 1997), a more depressogenic attributional style (Jaenicke, Hammen, Zupan, Hiroto, Gordon, Adrian, & Burge, 1987; Garber & Flynn, 2001), lower self-worth (Garber & Flynn, 2001; Goodman, Adamson, Riniti, & Cole, 1994), and more hopelessness (Garber & Flynn, 2001) than offspring of well mothers.

However, as Jacobvitz, Hazen, Curran, and Hitchens (2004) pointed out, most investigations of the contributions of family interactions to depression in children are cross-sectional studies. More prospective longitudinal studies, such as Radke-Yarrow’s pioneering work comparing children of depressed, bipolar or healthy mothers (Radke-Yarrow, 1998), Duggal and colleagues’ prospective study of early family relationships factors associated to depressive symptoms expressed in childhood and adolescence (Duggal, Carlson, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2001), and Murray and colleagues prospective study of children of postnatally depressed mothers (Murray, Halligan, Adams, Patterson, and Goodyer, 2006), are needed to better understand the development of depressive symptoms over childhood and adolescence. Adolescence is particularly critical as a time of significant transitions in biopsychosocial development during which adult forms of psychopathology often develop. Thus, it is of significant interest to explore the extent to which the socio-emotional correlates of exposure to maternal depression observed earlier in childhood extend to adolescent functioning (Murray et al., 2006).

Timing of the first exposure to maternal depression

Few studies have addressed issues of timing and recurrence of maternal depressive symptoms. However, differences in children’s adaptation may be related to these factors. Alpern and Lyons-Ruth (1993) found that children exposed to maternal depressive symptoms only in infancy showed more anxiety symptoms as reported by both mothers and teachers, while children exposed to maternal depression in kindergarten but not infancy displayed hyperactive behavior, and children exposed to chronic maternal depressive symptoms exhibited hostile behavior problems toward peers, compared to children of non-depressed mothers. Essex and colleagues’ findings indicated that exposure to maternal depression in infancy increased the risk of externalizing behavior problems only when it occurred in the context of high concomitant internalizing symptoms. By contrast, exposure to maternal depression during the toddler/preschool period was not associated with risk for high internalizing symptoms at kindergarten age (Essex, Klein, Miech, & Smider, 2001). Both sets of authors concluded that their results supported the idea that infancy may be a sensitive period for the occurrence of internalizing symptoms later in childhood. In their longitudinal study, Murray et al. (2006) found that the presence or absence of maternal depression outside the postnatal period did not contribute to the prediction of adolescent emotional sensitivity or maturity (Murray et al., 2006).

Many reasons could be advanced to explain the significance of the infancy period for the transmission of depressive symptoms. First, timing of maternal depression may exert an effect on child development that is distinct from the effect of recurrence of maternal depression over time. Maternal depression during a limited period followed by remission may lead to increased child problems to the extent that depression interferes with the achievement of tasks specific to that developmental period (Dawson, Ashman, Panagiotides, Hessl, Self, Yamada, & Embry, 2003; Essex, Klein, Cho, & Kraemer, 2003). Infancy, in particular, is a developmental period of rapid growth in neurological, physical, and emotional systems. Infants develop their first attachment bonds, in turn providing a framework for the infant’s regulation of emotion (Bowlby, 1980; Davies & Cummings, 1994; Essex et al., 2001). Cummings and Cicchetti (1990) suggested that early relationship experiences contribute to a child’s internal representation of self, which is critical to the development of healthy self-esteem, Poor self-esteem, in turn, has been considered a core symptom of depressive disorders (Jacobvitz et al., 2004). According to this theoretical viewpoint, if early maternal depression contributes to an insecure attachment relationship (Lyons-Ruth, Connell, Grunebaum, & Botein, 1990; Murray, 1992) the detrimental effects of an early insecure attachment may continue after maternal depression has remitted.

Second, because neurophysiological systems develop postnatally, this place particular importance on mothers’ role as an external emotion regulator (see Ashman & Dawson, 2002 for a review). These authors suggested that a depressed mother’s inability to model effective self-regulatory strategies and to provide regulatory assistance may increase the stressful nature of mother-infant interaction and fail to provide the infant with effective coping strategies in distressing situations. Further, the negative effects of exposure to maternal depression on the infant’s frontal electrical brain activity (Dawson, Frey, Self, Panagiotides, Hessl, Yamada, & Rinaldi, 1999; Dawson et al., 2003), involved in emotion regulation and the inhibition of inappropriate responses, and on HPA axis activity (Francis, Diorio, LaPlante, Weaver, Seckl, & Meaney, 1996), involved in cortisol regulation and stress responses, are thought to have enduring impact beyond the infancy period.

Finally, the self-regulatory and social skills of school-age children are more robust than during the infancy and preschool periods. With the parent as the initial external regulator, emotional and behavioral regulation gradually are appropriated by the child (Cassidy, 1994). Similarly, their social worlds are more extensive. According to Denham (1998) mothers have a greater role in the socialization of emotion regulation and expression during the first two years of life as compared to later developmental periods. It is possible that children are less vulnerable to a first onset of maternal depression occurring later in development, as they have already developed effective emotion regulation and coping strategies as well as a larger social network of support figures (e.g., neighbors, friends, and teachers).

Possible mediators of transmission of depressive symptomatology

Models of the transmission of risk for depression posit several mechanisms, resulting in child vulnerabilities and mental health outcomes that are moderated by gender and the timing of maternal depression (Goodman & Gotlib, 1999; Downey & Coyne, 1990; Cummings & Davies, 1994). Goodman and Gotlib (1999), for example, suggested several possible mechanisms of transmission, such as: a) heritability of depression; b) other heritable child dysfunctional neurogulatory mechanisms; c) negative maternal cognitions, behaviors, and affect; and d) the stressful context of the children’s lives. Other mechanisms such as higher emotional sensitivity of children of depressed mothers (Murray et al., 2006) also have been highlighted as potential links between maternal and child depression.

Children who have a depressed mother are at increased genetic predisposition for depression. Research supporting this notion includes twin studies of depressive symptoms in children and adolescents (Eaves, Silberg, Meyer, Maes, Simonoff, Heath, et al., 1997; Eley & Stevenson, 1999; Rice, Harold, & Thapar, 2002; Thapar & McGuffin, 1994). Genetic factors, however, do not express themselves in isolation. Silberg and Rutter (2002) underlined that genetic effects interact with the environment, and suggested that a better understanding of environmental factors should provide the means for effective intervention by interrupting genetic expression. For that reason, attention has turned to exploring parenting and environmental mechanisms that may mediate the relation between maternal depression and child depressive symptoms (Burt et al., 2005).

Quality of parenting

Various explanations have been advanced to explain parental effects on child depressive symptomatology. For example, Murray, Cooper, and Hipwell (2003) suggested several possible pathways, including lack of contingency in the parent’s responses, parental insensitivity and unresponsiveness, parental hostility and intrusiveness, and the lack of joint attention. These behaviors, then, could affect a child’s sense of self, capacity to sustain attention, level of distress, and cognitive and memory function.

For example, maternal depressive symptoms have been consistently related to more negative parenting behaviors (Downey & Coyne, 1990; Nelson, Hammen, Brennan, & Ullman, 2003; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1999). Depressed mothers have been described as more likely to report difficulties managing their infants’ crying and demands (Seeley, Murray, & Cooper, 1996); more withdrawn, with less rapid, consistent, and positive responsiveness to their children’s actions (Burt et al., 2005; Dawson et al., 2003); and more intrusive, coercive, or hostile in their play or interaction with their child (Field, 1992).

Additional studies indicate that maternal behavior mediates the links between depressive symptoms and children’s adjustment. For example, Lyons-Ruth, Alpern, and Repacholi (1993) found that the relation between maternal depressive symptoms in infancy and child behavior problems at age 5 was mediated by the more irritable and intrusive interactive stance prospectively observed among depressed mothers in infancy. Belden and Luby (2006) also showed a mediating effect of maternal support on specific depressive behaviors based on observational data in a sample of preschoolers.

Similar results have also resulted from retrospective reports obtained in adolescence. Garber and Flynn (2001) found that early parenting, characterized by low levels of care and acceptance, predicted low levels of global self-worth in young adolescents. Johnson, Cohen, Kasen, Smailes, and Brook (2001) also found that associations between more general parental psychiatric disorders and increased offspring risk for disorder was fully mediated by maladaptive parental behaviors. Finally, Bifulco, Moran, Ball, Jacobs, Raines, Bunn, and Cavagin (2002) found that a composite index of childhood neglect/abuse mediated the association between mother’s history of depression and offspring psychiatric disorder. Rose and Abramson (1992) suggested that highly threatening and/or recurrent negative events associated with such adverse family environments leads to the development of a more general negative and hopeless style. According to this theory, abusive events are particularly likely to lead to pessimistic expectations among individuals who tend to explain such negative events in terms of internal, stable, and global causes, and who have more negative views of themselves (Garber & Flynn, 2001).

Security and disorganization of attachment

Another factor that could increase the likelihood of depressive symptoms in childhood is the development of insecure and/or disorganized attachment patterns. A basic tenet of attachment theory states that the experience of close relationships in adolescence and adulthood takes place within a framework that emerges from the child’s primary attachment relationships early in development (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Bowlby, 1980). In a recent meta-analysis of six independent studies, Martins & Gaffan (2000) found significantly lower rates of secure attachment among infants of depressed mothers (see also van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1999). However, they reported great variability among studies included in their meta-analysis. Murray et al. (2006) also demonstrated that insecure attachment in infancy was associated with higher depressed mood in adolescence. However, the prevalence of insecure attachment in the general population is 40% (see van IJzendoorn, et al., 1999), which is much higher than the prevalence of depressive symptoms. Finally, in one of the few studies of security of attachment in middle childhood, Easterbrooks and her colleagues (Graham & Easterbrooks, 2000) did not find a strong relation between maternal depressive symptoms and insecure attachment. In addition, the longitudinal study of Radke-Yarrow (1998) showed that children who were securely attached to depressed or bipolar mothers more often developed problems than did the children who were insecurely attached. Taken together, these results suggest a much more complex interrelation between security of attachment and transmission of depressive symptoms than was originally expected.

Results are also mixed concerning the non-specific association between attachment disorganization and maternal depression. van IJzendoorn and colleagues’ (van IJzendoorn, et al., 1999) meta-analysis reported that the association between maternal depression and attachment disorganization, while significant, was “disappointingly weak”. However, the van IJzendoorn et al. (1999) meta-analysis also found that the relation between maternal depression and attachment disorganization was stronger among clinical samples than among community samples. In the Martins and Gaffan (2000) meta-analysis more consistent effects were found for attachment disorganization than for the secure and avoidant attachment categories. Therefore, the question is still open regarding the association between insecurity and disorganization of attachment and the transmission of depressive symptoms.

Influence of children’s gender on development of depression

Women are more vulnerable to develop depressive symptoms, with an increased differential risk emerging during adolescence (American, Psychological Association, 2000). The research literature is less clear, however, concerning a possible moderating effect of child gender on the transmission of maternal depression to children and adolescents (see Sheeber, Davis, & Hops, 2002 for a review). Some studies suggest that girls are more vulnerable to maternal depression than are boys (Burl et al., 2005; Jacobvitz et al., 2004, Murray & Sines, 1996; Radke-Yarrow & Nottelmann, 1989). This may be due to differences in socialization of emotions, with mothers reinforcing sadness and relation-oriented behaviors in girls and anger and instrumental behaviors in boys (Sheeber et al., 2002; Silk et al., 2006). Mothers may also be more salient models for girls than for boys (see Elgar et al., 2004). Other studies, however, suggest that boys may be more vulnerable to maternal depressive symptoms, especially if exposed at a young age (Essex et al., 2003; Carter, Garrity-Rokous, Chazan-Cohen, Little, & Briggs-Gowan, 2001; Murray, 1992); this finding is consistent with boys’ documented vulnerability to adverse effects of maternal mental illness in general (Murray, Kempton, Woolgar, & Hooper, 1993; Zahn-Waxler, 1993; Hay, Pawlby, Sharp, Asten, Mills, & Kumar, 2001). Finally, some work suggests that gender differences in transmission might differ according to developmental periods, with a more pronounced effect of gender in adolescence as compared to childhood (Duggal, et al., 2001; Fergusson, Horwood, & Lynskey, 1995; Hops, Sherman, & Biglan, 1990). Therefore, the existence of a consistent moderator effect of gender on transmission of depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence remains unclear.

In conclusion, although transmission of depressive symptoms from mothers to offspring has been documented in previous studies, many salient questions remain. Exploration of issues such as a) timing of first exposure to maternal depression, b) differential salience of particular developmental periods, and c) effects of repeated exposure to maternal depression, require longitudinal designs. Moreover, the exploration of different variables as potential mediators or moderators has yielded mixed results across previous studies. Finally, the use of a high psychosocial risk sample should increase the likelihood of observing depressive symptoms, both in mothers and their offspring, and should also provide a wider range of individual differences in parental behavior and child attachment behavior.

Hypotheses

In the present study, we examined the relation of maternal depressive symptoms assessed in infancy, middle childhood, and adolescence to child depressive symptoms in middle childhood and adolescence among a sample of children at socioeconomic risk. In addition, clinical family risk, maternal sensitive behavior in mother-child interactions, and attachment security and disorganization were assessed in infancy and maternal sensitivity was also assessed in childhood.

Three general hypotheses were advanced:

  1. Maternal depressive symptoms in infancy would make a unique contribution to the prediction of child and adolescent symptoms, even after controlling for maternal concurrent or recurrent depressive symptoms.

  2. Attachment insecurity and disorganization in infancy, maternal lack of involvement and hostility in infancy, and maternal sensitivity in childhood would mediate the association between maternal and child/adolescent symptoms.

  3. Gender would moderate the association between maternal and child/adolescent depressive symptoms. No specific hypotheses were advanced concerning the direction of effect. However, because of the higher prevalence of depressive symptoms demonstrated by girls starting in adolescence, it may be expected that girls will show higher rates of depressive symptoms, at least in adolescence.

Method

Participants

The data were collected at mother-child laboratory visits in infancy (12 months and 18-months-old; n = 79), school-age (7- and 8-years-old; n = 45). and adolescence (19-years-old; n = 47); at 18 months, a home visit was conducted to assess maternal behavior.

In infancy, 41 families (18 girls) were referred to the study by health or social service agency staff because of concerns about the quality of the parent-infant relationship and 38 (15 girls) were families from the community, matched to the referred group on per-person family income; mother’s education, age, and race; and infant’s age, sex, and birth order. All families were below federal poverty level at study entry. The sample was 81 % Caucasian, 11% Latino, 4% African American, and 4% biracial children. Forty percent of mothers were not high school graduates and 49% were single parents. Mother’s mean age at child’s birth was 25 years (range = 16 to 42 years), 15 mothers (19%) were under 20 years of age.

Forty-five mother-child dyads (28 male, 17 female children) were seen in two lab visits when children were aged 7 and 8 years-old. Five families refused participation, and the remainder could not be relocated (Easterbrooks, Davidson, & Chazan, 1993). The families who participated at middle childhood did not differ significantly from those who did not participate on demographic measures in infancy such as family income. F(1,78) = .98, n.s., maternal education, χ2(79) = .32, n.s., and single mother family. χ2(79) = .55, n.s. Attrition in middle childhood also was not related to any of the infancy period observations: attachment, F(1,70) = 1.75, n.s., maternal depressive symptoms. F(1,55) = .75, n.s., maternal involvement, F(1,72) = 1.72, n.s., or maternal hostility, F(1,72) = 2.15, n.s.

Forty-seven dyads returned for a lab visit when adolescents (24 male, 23 female) were 19 years of age. Families who participated in the adolescent assessment did not differ significantly from those who did not participate on measures of maternal education in infancy, χ2(79) = 2.01, n.s., or measures of infant attachment, F(1,70) = .55, n.s., maternal depressive symptoms in infancy, F(1,55) = .68, n.s., maternal involvement in infancy, F(1,72) = .01, n.s., or maternal hostility in infancy, F(1,72) = .01, n.s. However, there was a modest, but statistically significant, relation with attrition for family income, F(1,78) = 5.36, p = .02, and single mother headed family, χ2(79) = 4.38, p = .04. Families who did not come back in adolescence had lower income per person per week in infancy (participants: m = 49.17, sd = 19.53; non-participant: m = 40.53, sd = 9.58), and a higher rate of single parenting (participants: 19%: non-participant: 41%). Overall, 36 dyads had complete data at all time periods.

Measures

Clinical Risk in Infancy

Participants in the clinical risk group were referred for clinical home visiting in infancy by pediatric nurses and other healthcare professionals because service providers observed difficulties in the mother’s provision of care for the infant. Ten of these families were being followed by state protective service workers due to documented maltreatment. A second group of participants with no history of referral to community services was recruited from the same neighborhoods; this group had never sought or received social services directed at parenting skills, and had never undergone extensive psychiatric treatment (see Lyons-Ruth et al., 1990). A score of 1 on clinical risk was assigned to the referred participants and a score of 0 was assigned to the non-referred group.

Center For Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D)

Mothers (Times 1, 2, and 3) and their adolescent offspring (Time 3) completed the CES-D, a 20-item. 60-point self-report scale used to measure current levels of depressive symptoms in adults and late adolescents. The CES-D is a widely used instrument for assessing general depressive symptoms in nonclinical samples, and is among the most frequently used and well-validated self-report measures of depressive symptoms. The reliability and validity of the CES-D has been well-established, with 100% sensitivity and 88% specificity in relation to clinical diagnosis using the cut-off scores (Radloff, 1977; Radloff & Locke, 1986). The mothers and adolescents rate items (e.g. “My sleep was restless”) according to how often they had felt that way over the course of the past week (rarely or never, little, occasionally, or much). Although a diagnosis of depression cannot be made based on CES-D results, scores of 16 or above indicate that an individual is exhibiting a clinically meaningful level of depressive symptoms (Radloff, 1977). In the present study, the percentage of mothers reporting severe symptoms was: 32% in infancy; 36% in middle childhood; and 32% in adolescence. The percentage of adolescents reporting severe symptoms was 43%.

Dimensions of Depression Profile for Children and Adolescents

The Dimensions of Depression Profile for Children and Adolescents (Harter & Nowakowski, 1987) was used to assess depressive symptoms at age eight. The self-report scale, appropriate for children aged six and older, consists of five, six-item subscales (self-blame, affect/mood, energy, self-worth, and suicidal ideation: upon recommendation from one of the authors of the scale, the suicidal ideation subscale was not utilized). Scores range from 1–4, with low scores indicating greater depressive symptoms. Examples of items include “Some kids feel kind of ‘down’ and depressed a lot of the time, but other kids feel ‘up’ and happy most of the time”. Factorial, convergent, discriminant, and construct validity have been demonstrated (Renouf & Harter, 1990). In the present sample, since the subscales were highly related to each other (alpha=.76), a composite score of depressive symptoms was used. There is no established cut-off score for this instrument.

Infant Attachment

At 18 months of age, mothers and infants were seen in the Ainsworth Strange Situation (Ainsworth, et al., 1978), a series of eight 3-minutes episodes involving two mother-infant separations and reunions. Videotapes were coded for the three infant attachment classifications as described by Ainsworth et al. (1978) and for disorganized/disoriented behaviors as described by Main and Solomon (1990). The three original attachment classifications (secure, avoidant, ambivalent) were assigned by both a computerized multivariate classification procedure developed on the original Ainsworth data (Connell, 1976; see also reference in Richters, Waters, & Vaughn, 1988) and a coder trained by M. Main. Agreement between the two sets of classifications on the full 18-months data set was 86%; kappa = .79. Indices of disorganization (see Main & Solomon, 1990 for a full description) were combined into a disorganization scale ranging from 1 (none) to 9 (strong, frequent, or extreme disorganization). M. Main trained the two coders. Agreement on the disorganized classifications between M. Main and the two coders for 32 randomly selected tapes was 83% and 82% respectively; kappas were .72 and .76 respectively. Agreement on the nine-point Level of Disorganized Behavior Scale between two coders for 32 randomly selected tapes was r = .84.

Given the large proportion of disorganization in the sample, the major attachment variable for this study was the extent of disorganization scale. The use of a continuous measure of extent of disorganization was used to maximize the power of the study. A second aspect of the attachment data, orthogonal to the disorganization construct, was whether the best fitting organized category was secure, ambivalent, or avoidant classifications. Given cell sizes (e.g. only 3 ambivalent (C)), the two insecure-organized categories were combined and a single secure/insecure construct that was also entered into the analyses1.

Maternal Behavior at Home

Naturalistic mother-infant interaction was videotaped at home for 40 minutes when children were aged 18 months. Maternal behavior was coded in ten 4-minute intervals on twelve 5-point rating scales and one limed variable (see Lyons-Ruth, et al., 1997, for a detailed description of the scales). Coders were naïve to all other data on the families. Interobserver reliabilities were computed on a randomly selected 20% of the videotapes. Intraclass reliability coefficients ranged from .76 to .99 for the 13 variables. The principal components analysis of the scales yielded two main factors: 1) maternal involvement (positive loadings for maternal sensitivity, warmth, verbal communication, and quantity of comforting touch and negative loadings for disengagement and flatness of affect); and 2) hostile-intrusiveness (negative loadings for quality of comforting touch and quality of caretaking touch and positive loadings for covert hostility, interfering manipulation, and anger).

Emotional Availability Scales

Mothers and children were observed in a laboratory playroom when the children were 7 years of age. Assessments of emotional availability in the dyad were conducted based on a child-mother reunion (5–10 minutes) following an hour-long separation. Emotional availability was evaluated using the Emotional Availability Scales. Middle Childhood version 2nd edition (Biringen, Robinson, & Emde, 1993), and the maternal sensitivity scale was used for this analysis. Maternal sensitivity is a 9-point scale that assesses the overall interactive quality and affective tone of the mother’s behavior. Sensitive behavior is indicated by genuine positive affect, responsiveness to the child, balancing autonomy and child initiative, and flexibility in interaction. Because the maternal sensitivity scale (in this scale edition) was curvilinear, scores were transformed for later analyses. Transformed variables represented the distance of the score from the optimal scale point. For maternal sensitivity, the optimal scale point was 9; based on the distance from optimal, transformed scores could range from 0–8. For the transformed variables, lower scores indicated better functioning, since they were closer to the optimal score. All tapes were examined by 2 coders; kappas ranged from .95–1.00 (M=.98). Coders were naive to all other data.

MI Strategy

Missing data were imputed using NORM software (Schafer, 1999), which is designed to follow the MI guidelines outlined by Rubin (1987; Schafer, 1997). Procedures outlined by Schafer and Olsen (1998) were followed. Twenty data sets were generated, using Norm and data were analyzed using standard statistical software. We then concatenated the estimates and their standard errors from each data set and submitted them to NORM to assess statistical significance, taking into account the overall rate of missingness. In the present sample, the rate of missing information ranged from 0 to 44% across the study variables (M =21%, SD = 17.9%)2. Rubin has published guidelines for the number of data sets that should be imputed based on the rate of missing data. The efficiency of an estimate based on 20 imputations for a rate of missing information of 40% is 99%, which is excellent (see Rubin, 1987).

Results

Preliminary analyses

Due to the uniformly low income nature of the sample in infancy, analyses did not reveal any significant relations of family income or single mother family to maternal depressive symptoms in infancy. r= −.13, n.s. and F(1,55) = .03, n.s., respectively; infant disorganization, r = .06, n.s. and F(1,70) = .15, n.s., respectively; or maternal hostility in infancy, r = −.12, n.s. and F(1,72) = .06, n.s., respectively. Maternal involvement measured in infancy was not associated with single mother status, F(1,72) = .12, n.s. but showed a small but significant association with family income, r = .24, p < .05.

No childhood variables related to single mother family or family income, respectively: maternal sensitivity at age 7, F(1,42) = 1.29, n.s.; F(1,42) = 1.43, n.s.; maternal depressive symptoms at age 8, F(1,42) = .01, n.s.; F(1,42) = .97, n.s.; child depressive symptoms at age 8, F(1,42) = .23, n.s.; F(1,42) = 1.61, n.s. Adolescent variables also were unrelated to single mother family or family income, respectively: maternal depressive symptoms, F(1,28) = .05, n.s.; r = .11, n.s.; or adolescent depressive symptoms. F(1,27) = 3.07, n.s.; r = .01, n.s.

Mothers in the clinical risk group reported higher levels of depressive symptoms than did mothers in the non-clinical risk group, both in infancy and adolescence (see Table 1). Because of the overlap of depressive symptoms and clinical risk, regression models were tested with clinical risk status entered first in the regression models in order to control for the interdependence between clinical risk status and maternal depressive symptoms. Table 1 presents the correlation among all study variables.

Table 1.

Correlation among variables

Variablesa 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
1. Gender ---
2. Clinical risk status −0.05 ---
3. Secure/insecure (18 Months)b 0.00 −0.07 ---
4. Disorganization (18 Months) 0.30** 0.25* 0.08 ---
5. Maternal Involvement (18 Months) −0.04 0.01 0.23* −0.04 ---
6. Maternal Hostility (18 Months) 0.05 0.39** −0.07 0.26* 0.05 ---
7. Maternal Sensitivity (7 Years) −0.11 −0.31** −0.06 −0.16 −0.03 −0.48** ---
8. Mat. Depressive symptoms (12 Months) −0.12 0.46** 0.04 0.13 −0.06 0.25* −0.47** ---
9. Mat. Depressive symptoms (8 Years) 0.14 0.10 −0.15 −0.12 −0.01 0.00 −0.35** 0.29* ---
10. Mat. Depressive symptoms (adolescence) −0.12 0.26* 0.16 −0.15 −0.27* 0.00 0.01 0.15 0.37** ---
11. Mat. Depressive symptoms (Recurrence) −0.11 0.39** 0.02 −0.14 −0.12 0.15 −0.38** 0.62** 0.64** 0.65** ---
12. Child Depressive symptoms (8 Years) c 0.07 −0.40** −0.01 −0.23 −0.01 −0.33* 0.41** −0.44** −0.09 −0.14 −0.28* ---
13. Child Depressive symptoms (adolescence) 0.20 0.21 −0.14 0.05 −0.31* −0.13 0.10 0.22 0.10 0.33* 0.31* 0.15
a

Correlations run on 20 imputed data sets. Average values are presented.

b

Secure = 1 / Insecure = 2 based on three-category classification, disregarding extent of disorganization.

c

A lower score on this scale is indicative of higher depressive symptoms.

*

p<.05

**

p<.01

Maternal depressive symptoms and child and adolescent depressive symptoms

Hierarchical multiple regression analyses examined the influence of gender, clinical status, and maternal depressive symptoms on child depressive symptoms at 8 and 19 years of age. Variables were entered in decreasing proximal relation to children’s symptoms.

For child depressive symptoms at age 8, the regression model indicated that neither child gender, clinical risk status, nor concurrent maternal depressive symptoms contributed to the prediction of child depressive symptoms (see Table 2). However, maternal depressive symptoms during the first year of life did uniquely contribute to the prediction of child depressive symptoms at age 8, even with gender, clinical risk status, and concurrent maternal depressive symptoms in middle childhood controlled.

Table 2.

Parameter estimates for the hierarchical multiple regression model predicting children’s depressive symptoms at age 8

Predictor Average estimate Standard-error Estimated t-value Estimated DF Probability
Step 1: Gender .01 .13 .10 156 .92
Step 2: Clinical risk status −.25 .15 −1.68 128 .09t
Step 3: Maternal symptoms (Age 8) .01 .01 .18 99 .86
Step 4: Maternal symptoms (Infancy) −.01 .001 −2.47 359 .01 **

Note. These statistics are based on Rubin’s (1987) guidelines for assessing and combining parameter estimates.

t

p<.10

**

p<.01

A unique contribution of mothers’ depressive symptoms in infancy could occur due to particular aspects of the infancy period or because early maternal symptoms are a marker for more chronic or recurrent maternal symptoms over time. To explore the later possibility, an index of severe recurrent depressive symptoms was created, with one point added for each assessment period where a mother’s CES-D symptoms score was above the clinical cut-off level, resulting in a scale which ranged from 0 to 2 for the childhood period and from 0 to 3 for the adolescent period. An additional hierarchical regression analysis, with this recurrence index controlled, t(157) = .24, n.s., revealed that maternal depressive symptoms in infancy still significantly predicted depressive symptoms in childhood, t(223) = −2.54, p < .01, with gender also controlled, t(120) = .11, n.s. Therefore, the predictive importance of maternal symptoms in infancy was not due to an effect of repeated exposure over time.

In relation to the prediction of depressive symptoms in adolescence, hierarchical regression results revealed a significant effect of gender on depressive symptoms in adolescence (see Table 3). At age 19, adolescent boys in this sample reported more depressive symptoms (M = 18.82, SD =14.17) than did adolescent girls (M = 13.50, SD = 9.70). Results also indicated that both the child’s own depressive symptoms at age 8 and concurrent maternal depressive symptoms in adolescence contributed to the prediction of adolescent depressive symptoms after controlling for gender and early clinical status. Again, maternal depressive symptoms in infancy uniquely contributed to the prediction of adolescent depressive symptoms after controlling for all the preceding factors.

Table 3.

Parameter estimates for the hierarchical multiple regression model predicting depressive symptoms in adolescence (age 19)

Predictor Average estimate Standard-error Estimated t-value Estimated d.f. Probability
Step 1: Gender 7.67 3.21 2.39 133 .02*
Step 2: Clinical risk status 3.21 3.86 .83 96 .41
Step 3: Child’s symptoms (Age 8) 9.28 3.35 2.77 158 .01 **
Step 4: Maternal symptoms (Ado.) .41 .18 2.31 55 .02*
Step 5: Maternal symptoms (Age 8) −.19 .20 −.95 56 .34
Step 6: Maternal symptoms (Infancy) .34 .14 2.50 283 .01 **

Note. These statistics are based on Rubin’s (1987) guidelines for assessing and combining parameter estimates.

*

p<.05

**

p<. 01

A follow-up regression analysis was again conducted, controlling for recurrence of symptoms. Both child depressive symptoms at age 8. t(134) = 2.42. p < .05, and maternal depressive symptoms in infancy. t(134) = 2.42, p < .05, still significantly predicted depressive symptoms in adolescence when recurrence of symptoms, t(162) = −.02, n.s., was included in the model. However, concurrent maternal depressive symptoms in adolescence were only marginally associated with adolescent symptoms, t(97) = 1.89, p < .10. after the recurrence variable was included in the model.

Contribution of infant attachment and maternal sensitivity to the prediction of depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence

The third research question concerned variables suggested by previous literature to mediate the obtained relations between maternal and child depressive symptoms. Variables evaluated included secure/insecure attachment status, extent of disorganization of attachment, maternal involvement and hostility at 18 months and maternal sensitivity at 7 years. Note that these variables were measured between the two assessments of maternal and child depressive symptoms, making a mediation effect logically plausible.

Attachment at 18 months

Secure/insecure attachment classification did not meet prerequisites for mediation as classification was not predicted by maternal depressive symptoms at 12 months, t(469) = .32, n.s., nor was it a predictor of child depressive symptoms either at age 8, t(230) = −.07, n.s., or in adolescence, t(95) = −.92, n.s. Extent of attachment disorganization also did not meet conditions for testing mediation in that there was no relation between attachment disorganization and maternal depressive symptoms in infancy: t(586) = .81, n.s. However, importantly, disorganization of attachment was an independent predictor of children’s depressive symptoms at age 8, t(225) = − 2.01, p < .05.

Maternal involvement and hostility at 18 months

Of the two maternal interaction factors measured at 18 months only maternal hostility met the criteria for a mediation analysis. Maternal hostility was significantly associated both with early maternal depressive symptoms. t(289) = 2.00, p < .05. and with children’s symptoms at age 8, t(108) = −2.39, p < .05. Contrary to hypotheses, results of the mediational analyses showed that maternal hostility was an independent predictor but was not a significant mediator of the relation between maternal and child symptoms. Introduction of maternal hostility to the regression models was not associated with a significant decrease in the variance explained by early maternal depressive symptoms as indicated by a Sobel test of −.11, n.s. When included in the same model, maternal hostility in infancy, t(115) = −1.80, p < .10 marginally predicted child depressive symptoms while maternal depressive symptoms in infancy, t(326) = −3.07, p < .01, remained a significant predictor.

In relation to adolescent depressive symptoms, the criteria for testing a mediation model of maternal hostility were not met as the two variable were not related. The maternal involvement factor measured in infancy also did not met the criteria for mediation analysis due to the lack of association with maternal symptoms. However, early maternal involvement was an independent predictor of offspring depressive symptoms in adolescence, t(141) = −2.28, p < .02, though not at age 8.

Maternal sensitivity at age 7

Maternal sensitivity measured at 7 years of age met the criteria for a mediation analysis in relation to child symptoms at age 8. This variable was significantly associated with early maternal depressive symptoms. t(243) = −4.12, p < .01. and with children’s symptoms at age 8, t(141) = 3.28, p < .01. However, maternal sensitivity was not a significant mediator, as its introduction into the regression models was not associated with a significant decrease in the variance explained by early maternal depressive symptoms. Sobel test: −.06, n.s. The criteria for testing a mediation model in adolescence were not met.

Gender as moderator of the association between maternal and child and adolescent’s depressive symptoms

In order to test for moderating effects (Baron & Kenny, 1986), the interactions of gender and each of the predictors presented in Tables 1 and 2 were included in the regressions. There was no evidence of moderating influences of gender on the prediction of children’s depressive symptomatology, at either age 8 (p between .62 and .95) or in adolescence (p between .46 and .94). Finally, there was no evidence of a moderating effect of gender on the relation between child depressive symptoms at age 8 and adolescence.

Final regression models predicting child depressive symptoms at age 8 and 19

Based on results from previous analyses, two final regression models, including all significant predictors of child depressive symptoms in both middle childhood and adolescence, are presented in Table 4. For child symptoms at age 8, an additive model that included extent of disorganization in infancy, maternal hostility in infancy, maternal sensitivity at age 7, and maternal depressive symptoms in infancy accounted for 30% of variance in 8 years depressive symptoms. For adolescent symptoms, an additive model that included child gender, child depressive symptoms at age 8, maternal concurrent symptoms, and maternal symptoms in infancy accounted for 32% of variance. Note that maternal involvement in infancy is no longer a significant predictors when included in a model with all other predictors, therefore this variable was not included in the final regression model.

Table 4.

Parameter estimates for the additive regression models predicting depressive symptoms in both middle childhood and adolescence

Predictor Average estimate Standard-error Estimated t-value Estimated d.f. Probability
Middle childhood (age 8)

Extent of disorganization (Infancy) −.10 .06 −1.78 282 .08t
Maternal hostility (Infancy) −.15 .08 −1.97 96 .06t
Maternal sensitivity (age 7) .23 .10 2.23 129 .03*
Maternal symptoms (Infancy) −.01 .01 −2.33 313 .02*

Adolescence (age 19)

Gender .33 .13 2.67 259 .01**
Child’s symptoms (Age 8) 8.27 3.41 2.42 127 .02*
Maternal symptoms (Ado.) 6.89 3.21 2.15 128 .03*
Maternal symptoms (Infancy) .38 .16 2.36 59 .02*

Note. These statistics are based on Rubin’s (1987) guidelines for assessing and combining parameter estimates.

t

p<.10

*

p<.05

**

p<.01

Discussion

The first objective of the present study was to explore the influence of timing of exposure to maternal depressive symptoms on the development of depressive symptoms in children aged 8 and 19 years in a sample of families at high socio-economic risk. It was proposed that maternal depressive symptoms in infancy would be critical to the prediction of the child’s symptoms in middle childhood and adolescence, even after controlling for concurrent and recurrent maternal depressive symptoms. Results supported this hypothesis, which is consistent with previous studies that have shown a significant association between maternal and offspring depressive symptoms (reviewed in Goodman & Gotlib, 2002).

Our results also support the idea that infancy may be a “sensitive period” for the development of depressive symptoms later in childhood and adolescence. There are several reasons why this developmental period may be particularly crucial. First, infancy marks a time of great dependence on caregivers to provide support for both physiological and emotional regulation. At the same time, infants are developing attachment bonds, which form the basis for their “felt security” (Davies & Cummings, 1994). The quality of the child’s early internal representation of self in the context of the primary relationship may be critical to the development of healthy self-esteem (Cummings & Cicchetti, 1990), a component of depressive symptoms. Moreover, the infant’s social world may be more limited than that of a preschooler, or school-aged child, whose multiple social contexts may provide more opportunities for buffering the impact of depressive symptoms of the primary caregiver. It is possible, also, that children who experience negative effects of exposure to early maternal depressive symptoms may show “cascade” effects, with impacts continuing through later periods of development (Dawson et al., 2003; Essex et al., 2003).

Although the influence of early maternal depressive symptoms was expected, it was surprising that concurrent maternal symptoms did not play a more important role in the development of depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence. The only significant relation between maternal and child concurrent symptoms was found in adolescence, but the association became non-significant when recurrence of maternal symptoms over time was controlled for. This suggests that it is more the enduring quality of these symptoms rather then their contingency that is influential in adolescents’ symptoms. However, it is noteworthy that influence of early maternal symptoms remains after controlling for recurrence of symptoms over time. This is consistent with results by Dawson and her colleagues (2003) suggesting an enduring effect on child internalizing problems of remitted, nonchronic exposure to early maternal depression. Another study (Hammen & Brennan, 2003), however, found that when unconfounded with severity and duration, postpartum depression was not more risky (for youth depression) than was maternal depression at other periods. However, as the authors noted, the study relied on women’s retrospective reports of their past depressions, and the reliability of such reports is known to be modest.

It is possible that the stage-salient needs of children at later developmental periods are less impacted by maternal depression than stage salient needs in infancy. Goodman and Gotlib (1999) suggested that early needs for security and emotion regulation might shift towards a need for social and emotional understanding in preschool, and towards a need for general social support and consistent discipline in middle childhood and adolescence. With development, children’s social worlds increasingly include relationships with others (e.g., family members, peers, child care providers and teachers) who may serve important compensatory or buffering roles in the development of depressive symptoms in the child of a depressed mother.

The association between children’s depressive symptoms at ages 8 and 19 also is interesting in itself. Although the zero-order correlation between the two assessments is modest and non-significant, depressive symptoms at age 8 became a significant predictor of symptoms at age 19 when co-variance of different predictors (such as gender and clinical risk status) was taken into account in a regression model. This is consistent with results obtained by Duggal et al. (2001) indicating a modest but significant correlation between measures of child depression and adolescent depression in a high-risk sample. Also in line with Duggal et al. (2001), our results suggest that depressive symptoms of girls and boys are equally stable between middle childhood and adolescence. Although these results suggest some continuity between childhood and adolescence in the development of depressive symptoms, this question has been understudied in the past and could have important implications both for treatment and prevention of depression. On one hand, the fact that, in the present study, both scores are strongly predicted by early maternal depressive symptoms may partly explain the continuity observed between them. It is generally accepted that depressive symptoms, along with psychopathology in general, becomes more apparent and crystallized in adolescence. However, as there are fewer studies including reports of depressive symptoms in middle childhood, it is possible that precursors of adolescent depressive symptoms are already developed and apparent at this earlier period. On the other hand, there is recent evidence that different features characterize depressive symptoms shown at different developmental stages (see meta-analytic study by Weiss & Garber, 2003), which, along with the use of different assessment instruments, may explain the modest association between childhood and adolescent symptoms obtained in this study.

The second objective of this study was to explore the influence of possible mediators of the relation between maternal depressive symptoms early in infancy and the child’s later depressive symptoms. It was hypothesized that infant attachment insecurity and disorganization as well as maternal hostility or sensitivity might play this role. Results showed that neither infant insecurity and disorganization measured at 18 months, maternal hostility and involvement measured at 18 months, nor maternal sensitivity measured at 7 years met criteria required for mediation. However, results showed that infant disorganization, maternal hostility in infancy, and maternal sensitivity in middle childhood were independent predictors of the child’s depressive symptoms at age 8, while maternal lack of involvement in infancy independently predicted symptoms in adolescence.

The absence of association between attachment insecurity and disorganization and maternal depressive symptoms is interesting. Although the meta-analysis by van IJzendoorn et al. (1999) suggested a similar lack of association, a more recent meta-analysis by Martins and Gaffan (2000) suggested associations between maternal depression and both insecurity and disorganization. Jacobvitz et al. (2004) suggested that only severe and/or chronic maternal depression is associated with infant attachment disorganization and that less severe depression is not. It is possible that our results would have been different had we used clinical evaluation of maternal depression. However, even though disorganization of attachment and maternal depressive symptoms have non-shared features, these non-shared features each contribute to the development of later depressive symptoms in children. It is possible that each explains different dimensions or facets of depressive symptoms in childhood. For example, the sadness and resignation aspect of depression might be explained by exposure to maternal depressive mood, while the lack of emotion regulation and deficient problem solving skills may be more strongly associated with disorganization of attachment. Concerning the lack of predictive power of attachment insecurity. Green and Goldwyn (2002) discussed the fact that its high base rate in the normal population has reduced its predictive value for psychopathology.

The results obtained with regard to mother-infant interaction are also interesting. Both maternal hostility measured in infancy and maternal sensitivity measured at age 7 were associated with children’s depressive symptoms, as well as with maternal depressive symptoms in infancy but were not mediators of the transmission of depressive symptoms. These results are consistent with recent studies suggesting that quality of parenting is not a mediator of transmission of depressive symptoms, rather making independent contributions to the prediction of offspring symptoms (Burt et al., 2005; Garber & Flynn, 2001; Nelson et al., 2003). As in the case of infant disorganization, these results indicate that maternal hostility, maternal involvement, maternal sensitivity, and maternal depressive symptoms have non-shared features that each contribute to dimensions of depressive symptoms in children at age 8 and/or adolescence. Hart, Jones, and Field (2003) previously described two interactive styles identified in depressed mothers; maternal withdrawal and intrusiveness. It is expected that different child outcomes would be associated with those two types of maternal depression (see Hart et al., 2003). Nonetheless, it is hard to explain why hostility would be associated with childhood depressive symptoms, while lack of involvement would be associated with symptoms in adolescence. At least one previous study showed a similar pattern of results suggesting that maternal early care lacking in emotional supportiveness measured in infancy was a unique predictor of depressive symptoms in adolescence (Duggal et al., 2001). Future research is necessary to explore whether different configurations of depressive symptoms in children and adolescents are associated with these different maternal patterns.

A last objective was to explore whether gender would be a moderator of the association between early maternal depressive symptoms and child and adolescent symptoms. Our results revealed no gender effect on transmission of depressive symptoms in childhood or adolescence or on continuity of depressive symptoms in the child. It is important to note, however that the relatively small sample size of the present study may have limited the power to detect such moderation effects and these results should be interpreted cautiously. Although some hypotheses have been advanced in favor of possible moderation effects of gender differences, previous empirical results have been inconsistent in that regard (see Sheeber, et al., 2002). In their review of this literature, Sheeber et al. (2002) concluded that more studies are necessary to reach firm conclusions. The fact that adolescent boys showed higher rates of depressive symptoms than girls in this sample was unanticipated, considering the higher prevalence of depressive symptoms found in adolescent girls in previous literature (see APA, 2000; Watts & Markham, 2005). A second significant gender effect was shown for the attachment disorganization, with boys showing higher rates of disorganization than girls. This gender difference was already discussed in previous studies conducted with this sample (David & Lyons-Ruth, 2005; Lyons-Ruth, Bronfman, & Parsons, 1999). While noting that similar results have been obtained in other high-risk samples. David and Lyons-Ruth (2005) demonstrated that female infants are more likely to show an affiliative behavioral strategy in the face of maternal disrupted behaviors, whereas male infants are more likely to exhibit conflicted, disorganized behaviors. It may be that boys in at-risk settings from infancy are at higher risk for depressive symptoms than more normative epidemiologic data would indicate.

It is important to note some limitations of the present study. First, since our measures were not clinical diagnostic instruments, care must be used in moving from the level of depressive symptoms to a clinical diagnosis. This is also true for the composite variable evaluating recurrence of symptoms. Although we used symptoms scores in the clinical range, it should not be considered equivalent to a diagnostic instrument. It is possible, but not certain, that results may have been different (e.g. stronger relation with disorganization or influence of clinical risk status) if clinical diagnoses of depression were used. However, the child correlates of maternal depressive symptoms are similar whether depression has been defined by psychiatric diagnostic criteria or by depressive symptom scales, and both sets of findings have been well-replicated (see Lyons-Ruth, Wolfe, & Lyubchik, 2000).

Second, it is difficult to determine whether the use of slightly different instruments to assess similar concepts had a significant effect on the pattern of results obtained in this study. Although the maternal depressive symptoms instrument was consistent over time, the longitudinal design necessitated different assessments of child depressive symptoms at the two ages. Nevertheless, Rice, Harold, and Thapar (2005) recently suggested that the structural characteristics and constellations of depressive symptoms are similar in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood, so we have reason to believe that our measures are sufficiently similar. Moreover, compared to existing literature, the utilization of different informants to assess depressive symptoms in children and mothers is a strength of the present study. As Burt et al. (2005) pointed out, if mothers rate both their own symptoms and the child’s outcomes, any association that is found may be due to effects of the informant rather than true environmental effects. The fact that we found a significant association between maternal depressive symptoms and the child’s reports of their own symptoms brings some robustness to the concept of transmission of depressive symptoms.

Finally, results from the present study must be interpreted cautiously due to the relatively small sample size and the significant attrition. These are usual features of longitudinal studies conducted in high risk samples with comprehensive assessment including interviews and laboratory visits. Multiple imputation data analytic methods were employed to minimize the impact of attrition, but as these statistical methods are relatively recent, caution must be made in the interpretation of these results.

In conclusion, our results provide support for the importance of early exposure to maternal depressive symptoms for the child’s later depressive symptoms in both middle childhood and adolescence. Although we did not measure the genetic effects or influence of maternal depressive symptoms prior to birth, none of our results contradict hypotheses advanced in these areas. Our results, however, do not support hypotheses of an influence of gender or of early clinical risk status on the transmission of depressive symptoms from mother to child. Finally, it appears that infant attachment disorganization, maternal hostility measured in infancy, and maternal sensitivity measured in middle childhood influence children’s depressive symptoms at age 8, while maternal lack of involvement during infancy influences symptoms in adolescence. None of these factors, however, serve as a mediator of the transmission of depressive symptoms from mothers to children. These results have important implications for intervention and prevention. The most important implication is the robust and long term negative effect of early maternal depressive symptoms on child and adolescent dysphoric mood. This speaks to the need for prevention programs during pregnancy and the early years of life. Another meaningful finding was the independent effect of maternal depressive symptoms (relative to other aspects of mother-child interaction and attachment) as a predictor of the development of depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence. This suggests that interventions that focus on aspects of maternal depressive symptoms, rather than general maternal sensitivity, may be more effective in preventing transmission of depressive symptoms from mothers to their offspring. Previous investigations, however, have documented the effectiveness of relationship-based interventions in promoting secure attachments and better social adaptation among infants of depressed mothers (see Cicchetti, Toth, & Rogosch, 1999; Cohen, Lojkasek, Muir, Muir, & Parker, 2002; Gelfand Teti, Seiner, & Jameson, 1996; Toth, Rogosh, Todd Manly, & Cicchetti, 2006).

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a Canadian post-doctoral fellowship to the first author and by NIMH grants 35122 and 62030 to K. Lyons-Ruth.

This research was supported by grants to K. Lyons-Ruth from the National Institutes of Health (MH062030 and MH5122) and to A. Easterbrooks from a Tufts University Faculty Research Award.

Footnotes

1

Secure: n = 33 (23 secure & 10 disorganized (D)-secure); Insecure: n = 38 (15 A, 0 C, 16 D/A, and 3 D/C)

2

Imputation could he conducted with data missing at a rate higher than 50% (Widaman, 2006).

Contributor Information

Jean-François Bureau, Harvard Medical School.

M. Ann Easterbrooks, Tufts University.

Karlen Lyons-Ruth, Harvard Medical School.

References

  1. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  2. Alpern L, Lyons-Ruth K. Preschool children at social risk: Chronicity and timing of maternal depressive symptoms and child behavior problems at school and at home. Development and Psychopathology. 1993;5:371–387. [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. Washington. DC: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  4. Ashman SB, Dawson G. Maternal depression, infant psychobiological development, and risk for depression. In: Goodman SH, Gotlib IH, editors. Children of depressed parents: Mechanisms of risk and implications for treatment. American Psychological Association; 2002. pp. 37–58. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baron R, Kenny D. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51:1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Belden A, Luby J. Preschoolers’ depression severity and behaviors during dyadic interactions: The mediating role of parental support. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45:213–222. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000189133.59318.5e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bifulco A, Moran PM, Ball C, Jacobs C, Raines R, Bunn A, Cavagin J. Childhood adversity, parental vulnerability and disorder: Examining inter-generational transmission of risk. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43:1075–1086. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Biringen Z, Robinson JL, Emde RN. Emotional availability scales Unpublished coding manual. University of Colorado Health Sciences Center; Denver, Colorado: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss, Vol. 3: Loss, sadness and depression. New York: Basic Books; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  10. Burt KB, Van Dulmen MHM, Carlivati J, Egeland B, Sroufe LA, Forman DR, Appleyard K, Carlson EA. Mediating links between maternal depression and offspring psychopathology: The importance of independent data. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46:490–499. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00367.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carter AS, Garrity-Rokous E, Chazan-Cohen R, Little C, Briggs-Gowan MJ. Maternal depression and comorbidity: Predicting early parenting, attachment security, and toddler social-emotional problems and competencies. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40:18–26. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200101000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cassidy J. Emotion regulation: Influence of attachment relationships. The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioural considerations, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child. In: Fox NA, Campos JJ, editors. Development. Vol. 59. 1994. pp. 228–249. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cicchetti D, Toth SL, Rogosch FA. The efficacy of toddler-parent psychotherapy to increase attachment security in offspring of depressed mothers. Attachment and Human Development. 1999;1:34–66. doi: 10.1080/14616739900134021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cohen NJ, Lojkasek M, Muir E, Muir R, Parker CJ. Six-month follow-up of two mother-infant psychotherapies: Convergence of therapeutic outcomes. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2002;23:361–380. [Google Scholar]
  15. Connell AM, Goodman SH. The association between psychopathology in fathers versus mothers and children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior problems: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulleting. 2002;128:746–773. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Connell DB. Individual differences in attachment: An investigation into stability, implications, and relationships to structure of early language development Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Syracuse University; Syracuse. NY: 1976. [Google Scholar]
  17. Cummings EM, Cicchetti D. Toward a transactional model of relations between attachment and depression. In: Greenberg MT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, editors. Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1990. pp. 339–372. [Google Scholar]
  18. Cummings EM, Davies PT. Maternal depression and child development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1994;35:73–112. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01133.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Davies PT, Cummings EM. Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;166:387–411. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. David DH, Lyons-Ruth K. Differential attachment responses of male and female infants to frightening maternal behavior: tend or befriend versus fight or flight? Infant Mental Health Journal. 2005;26:1–18. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dawson G, Ashman SB, Panagiotides H, Hessl D, Self J, Yamada E, Embry L. Preschool outcomes of children of depressed mothers: Role of maternal behavior. contextual risk, and children’s brain activity. Child Development. 2003;74:1158–1175. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dawson G, Frey K, Self J, Panagiotides H, Hessl D, Yamada E, Rinaldi J. Frontal electrical brain activity in infants of depressed mothers: Relation to variations in infant behavior. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:589–605. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Denham SA. Emotional development in young children. New York: Guilford press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  24. Downey G, Coyne JC. Children of depressed parents: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;108:50–76. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.108.1.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Duggal S, Carlson EA, Sroufe A, Egeland B. Depressive symptomatology in childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:143–164. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401001109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Easterbrooks MA, Davidson CE, Chazan R. Psychosocial risk, attachment, and behavior problems among school-aged children. Development and Psychopathology. 1993;5:389–402. [Google Scholar]
  27. Eaves L, Silberg J, Meyer JM, Maes HH, Simonoff E, Pickles A, Rutter M, Neale M, Reynolds CA, Erikson MT, Heath AC, Loeber R, Tuett KR, Hewitt JK. Genetics and developmental psychopathology: 2. The main effects of genetics and environment of behavioral problems in the Virginia twin study of adolescent behavioral development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38:965–980. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Eley TC, Stevenson J. Exploring the covariation between anxiety and depression symptoms: A genetic analysis of the effect of age and sex. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1999;40:1273–1284. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Elgar FJ, McGrath PJ, Waschbusch DA, Stewart SH, Curtis LJ. Mutual influences on maternal depression and child adjustment problems. Clinical Psychology Review. 2004;24:441–459. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Essex MJ, Klein MH, Cho E, Kraemer HC. Exposure to maternal depression and marital conflict: Gender differences in children’s later mental health symptoms. Journal od American Academy for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:728–737. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046849.56865.1D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Essex MJ, Klein MH, Miech R, Smider NA. Timing of initial exposure to maternal major depression and children’s mental health symptoms in kindergarten. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;179:151–156. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.2.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Lynskey MT. Maternal depressive symptoms and depressive symptoms in adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1995;36:1161–1178. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01363.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Field T. Infant of depressed mothers. Development and Psychopathology. 1992;4:49–66. doi: 10.1017/s0954579497001260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Francis D, Diorio J, LaPlante P, Weaver S, Seckl JR, Meaney MJ. The role of early environmental events in regulating neuroendocrine development: Moms, pups, stress, and glucocorticoid receptors. In: Ferris CF, Grisso T, editors. Annals of the New York Academy of Scoences: Vol. 794. Understanding aggressive behavior in children. New York: New York Academy of Sciences; 1996. pp. 136–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Garber J, Flynn C. Predictors of depressive cognitions in young adolescents. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2001;25:353–376. [Google Scholar]
  36. Gelfand DM, Teti DM, Seiner SA, Jameson PB. Helping mothers fight depression: Evaluation of a home-based intervention program for depressed mothers and their infants. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1996;25:406–422. [Google Scholar]
  37. Goodman SH, Adamson LB, Riniti J, Cole S. Mothers’ expressed attitudes: Associations with maternal depression and children’s self-esteem and psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1994;33:1265–1274. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199411000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Goodman SH, Gotlib IH. Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: A developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychological Review. 1999;3:458–490. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.106.3.458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Goodman SH, Gotlib IH. Children of depressed parents: Mechanisms of risk and implications for treatment. Washington. DC: American Psychological Association; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  40. Graham CA, Easterbrooks MA. School-aged children’s vulnerability to depressive symptomatology: The role of attachment security, maternal depressive symptomatology, and economic risk. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:201–213. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400002054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Green J, Goldwyn R. Annotation: Attachment disorganisation and psychopathology: new findings in attachment research and their potential implications for developmental psychopathology in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43:835–846. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hammen C, Brennan PA. Severity, chronicity, and timing of maternal depression and risk for adolescent offspring diagnoses in a community sample. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:253–258. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.3.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hart SH, Jones NA, Field T. Atypical expressions of jealousy in infants of intrusive- and withdrawn-depressed mothers. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2003;33:193–207. doi: 10.1023/a:1021452529762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Harter S, Nowakowski M. The dimensions of Depression Profile for Children and Adolescents. University of Denver; Denver. Colorado: 1987. [Google Scholar]
  45. Hay DF, Pawlby S, Sharp D, Asten P, Mills A, Kumar R. Intellectual problems shown by 11 year old children whose mothers had postnatal depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2001;42:871–889. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hops H, Sherman L, Biglan A. Maternal depression, marital discord, and children’s behavior: A developmental perspective. In: Patterson GR, editor. Depression and aggression in family interactions. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum; 1990. pp. 185–208. [Google Scholar]
  47. Jacobvitz D, Hazen N, Curran M, Hitchens K. Observations of early triadic family interactions: Boundary disturbances in the family predict symptoms of depression, anxiety, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:577–592. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404004675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Jaenicke C, Hammen C, Zupan B, Hiroto D, Gordon D, Adrian C, Burge D. Cognitive vulnerability in children at risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1987;15:559–572. doi: 10.1007/BF00917241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Johnson JG, Cohen P, Kasen S, Smailes E, Brook JS. Association of maladaptive parental behavior with psychiatric disorder among parents and their offspring. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58:453–460. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.5.453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Luby JL, Belden AC, Spitznagel E. Risk factors for preschool depression: The mediating role of early stressful life events. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:1292–1298. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01672.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Luby JL, Heffelfinger AK, Mrakowsky C, Brown KM, Hessler MJ, Wallis JM, Spitznagel E. The clinical picture of depression in preschool children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:340–348. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200303000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lyons-Ruth K, Alpern L, Repacholi B. Disorganized infant attachment classification and maternal psychosocial problems as predictors of hostile-aggressive behavior in the preschool classroom. Child Development. 1993;64:572–585. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02929.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lyons-Ruth K, Bronfman E, Parsons E. Maternal disrupted affective communication, maternal frightened or frightening behavior, and disorganized infant attachment strategies. In: Vondra J, Barnett D, editors. Atypical patterns of infant attachment: Theory, research and current directions, monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Lyons-Ruth K, Connell DB, Grunebaum HU, Botein S. Infants at social risk: Maternal depression and family support services as mediators of infant development and security of attachment. Child Development. 1990;61:85–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02762.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Lyons-Ruth K, Easterbrooks MA, Cibelli CD. Infant attachment strategies, infant mental lag, and maternal depressive symptoms: Predictors of internalizing and externalizing problems at age 7. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:681–692. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.4.681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Lyons-Ruth K, Wolfe R, Lyubchik A. Depression and the parenting of young children: Making the case for early preventive mental health services. Harvard Review of Psychiatry. 2000;8:148–153. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Main M, Solomon J. Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In: Greenberg M, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, editors. Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1990. pp. 121–160. [Google Scholar]
  58. Malcarne VL, Hamilton NA, Ingram RE, Taylor L. Correlates of distress in children at risk for affective disorder: Exploring predictors in the offspring of depressed and nondepressed mothers. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2000;59:243–251. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(99)00155-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Martins C, Gaffan EA. Effects of early maternal depression on pattern of infant-mother attachment: A meta-analytic Investigation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2000;41:737–746. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Murray KT, Sines JO. Parsing the genetic and nongenetic variance in children’s depressive behavior. Journal of Affective Disorders. 1996;36:23–34. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(95)00089-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Murray L. The impact of postnatal depression on infant development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1992;33:543–561. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00890.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Murray L, Cooper P, Hipwell A. Mental health of parents caring for infants. Archives of Women’s Mental Health. 2003;6:71–77. doi: 10.1007/s00737-003-0007-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Murray L, Halligan SL, Adams G, Patterson P, Goodyer IM. Socioemotional development in adolescents at risk for depression: The role of maternal depression and attachment style. Development and Psychopathology. 2006;18:489–516. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Murray L, Kempton C, Woolgar M, Hooper R. Depressed mothers’ speech to their infants and its relation to infant gender and cognitive development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1993;34:1083–1102. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb01775.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Nelson DR, Hammen C, Brennan PA, Ullman JB. The impact of maternal depression on adolescent adjustment: The role of expressed emotion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2003:935–944. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.71.5.935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Chronicity of maternal depressive symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and child functioning at 36 months. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:1297–1310. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.5.1297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Radke-Yarrow M. Children of depressed mothers: From early childhood to maturity. Cambridge University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  68. Radke-Yarrow M, Nottelmann E. Affective development in children of well and depressed mothers; Paper presented at Society for Research in Child Development; Kansas City, MO. 1989. Apr, [Google Scholar]
  69. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  70. Radloff LS, Locke BZ. The community mental health assessment survey and the CES-D scale. In: Weissman MM, Myers JK, Ross CE, editors. Community surveys of psychiatric disorders, Surveys in Psycholosocial Epidemiology. Vol. 4. 1986. pp. 177–189. [Google Scholar]
  71. Renouf AG, Harter S. Low self-worth and anger as components of the depressive experience in young adolescents. Development and Psychopathology. 1990;2:293–310. [Google Scholar]
  72. Rice F, Harold GT, Thapar A. Assessing the effects of age, sex, and shared environment on the aetiology of depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43:1039–1051. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Rice F, Harold GT, Thapar A. The link between depression in mothers and offspring: An extended twin analysis. Behavior Genetics. 2005;35:565–577. doi: 10.1007/s10519-005-5432-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Richters JE, Waters E, Vaughn BE. Empirical classification of infant-mother relationships from interactive behavior and crying during reunion. Child Development. 1988;59:512–522. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rose DT, Abramson LY. Developmental predictors of depressive cognitive style: Research and theory. In: Cicchetti D, Toth SL, editors. Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology. Vol. 4. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1992. pp. 323–349. [Google Scholar]
  76. Rubin DB. Multiple imputation for nonresponse surveys. New York: Wiley; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  77. Rutter M, Izard CE, Read PB. Depression in young people. New York: Guilford Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  78. Schafer JL. Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. London: Chapman & Hall; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  79. Schafer JL. NORM: Multiple imputation of incomplete multivariate data under a nonnal model (version 2) 1999 [Computer software] Retrieved from http://www.stat.psu.edu/ils/misoftwa.html.
  80. Schafer JL, Olsen MK. Multiple imputation for multivariate-missing data problems: A data analyst’s perspective. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 1998;33:545–571. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3304_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Sheeber L, Davis B, Hops H. Gender-specific vulnerability to depression in children of depressed mothers. In: Goodman SH, Gotlib IH, editors. Children of depressed parents: Mechanisms of risk and implications for treatment. American Psychological Association; 2002. pp. 253–276. [Google Scholar]
  82. Seeley S, Murray L, Cooper PJ. The detection and treatment of postnatal depression by health visitors. Health Visit. 1996;64:135–138. [Google Scholar]
  83. Silberg J, Rutter M. Nature-nurture interplay in the risks associated with parental depression. In: Goodman SH, Gotlib IH, editors. Children of depressed parents: Mechanisms of risk and implications for treatment. American Psychological Association; 2002. pp. 13–36. [Google Scholar]
  84. Silk JS, Shaw DS, Skuban EM, Oland AA, Kovacs M. Emotion regulation strategies in offspring of childhood-onset depressed mothers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:69–78. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01440.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Thapar A, McGuffin P. A twin study of depressive symptoms in childhood. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;165:259–265. doi: 10.1192/bjp.165.2.259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Toth SL, Rogosch FA, Todd Manly J, Cicchetti D. The efficacy of toddler-parent psychotherapy to reorganize attachment in the young offspring of mothers with major depressive disorder: A randomized preventive trial. 2006 doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.1006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. van I, Jzendoorn MH, Schuengel C, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Disorganized attachment in early childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:225–249. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Watts SJ, Markham RA. Etiology of depression in children. Journal of Instructional Psychology. 2005;32:266–270. [Google Scholar]
  89. Weiss B, Garber J. Developmental differences in the phenomenology of depression. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:403–430. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Weissman MM, Warner V, Wickramaratne P, Moreau D, Olfson M. Offspring of depressed parents 10 years later. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1997;54:932–940. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830220054009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Widaman KF. Missing data: What to do with or without them. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2006;71:42–64. [Google Scholar]
  92. Zahn-Waxler C. Warriors and worriers: gender and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1993;5:79–89. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES