Abstract
Prepulse inhibition (PPI) of startle is an operational measure of the pre-attentive filtering process known as sensorimotor gating. Originally identified in patients with schizophrenia, PPI deficits have been observed in multiple but not all psychiatric disorders. Thus, as with most signs and symptoms of psychiatric disorders, deficits in PPI cut across diagnostic categories. It remains unclear whether the diversity of disorders exhibiting deficient PPI bespeaks diagnostic overlaps or comorbidities. Given the recent focus on treatments for cognitive deficits of schizophrenia independently of treating psychosis, the relationship of PPI deficits to cognitive deficits becomes of interest. Although PPI cannot be considered to be a cognitive process per se, abnormalities in pre-attentive information processing may be predictive of or lead to complex cognitive deficits. Animal models of PPI deficits produced by dopamine agonists reliably predict existing antipsychotics. Nevertheless, since neither PPI nor cognitive deficits in schizophrenia are ameliorated by standard antipsychotics, current research is exploring the predictive value of non-dopaminergic PPI models in identifying treatments for gating disturbances independently of their relevance to specific disorders. Both PPI and cognitive deficits in schizophrenia patients are not reversed by first generation antipsychotics but may be attenuated by clozapine. Similarly, effects of glutamate antagonists on symptoms in patients and PPI in animals appear to be reduced by clozapine. Hence, treatment-induced reversals of deficits in PPI produced by glutamate antagonists may provide animal, and human, models to aid in the discovery of treatments of cognitive deficits in patients already treated with existing antipsychotics.
Keywords: Prepulse inhibition, Schizophrenia, NMDA, Antipsychotics, MATRICS
PREPULSE INHIBITION IN PATIENTS
Gating Deficits in Schizophrenia
Measures of sensory or sensorimotor gating are among the most widely studied physiological markers used in laboratory studies of schizophrenia. For example, the auditory “sensory gating” paradigm pioneered by Freedman’s group involves a condition-test paired-stimulus paradigm in which the P50 event-related potential (ERP) elicited by the second of two audible clicks is normally reduced relative to the ERP elicited by the first click (Freedman et al., 1999). In schizophrenia patients, however, this suppression of the P50 is diminished, apparently due to a reduction in short-term habituation. An analogous paradigm has been developed for use in rodents (Freedman et al., 1999). This cross-species ERP paradigm has been critical in the identification of the alpha-7 nicotinic receptor as a potential target for pro-cognitive co-treatments in schizophrenia (Martin et al., 2004). Another cross-species gating paradigm, prepulse inhibition of startle (PPI) is the focus of this review and differs qualitatively from the P50 ERP paradigm. Because it involves both sensory stimuli and motor responses (Graham, 1975), PPI is referred to as a measure of “sensorimotor gating” rather than sensory gating (Geyer and Braff, 1987; Braff and Geyer, 1990). In PPI, the acoustic startle response elicited by a sudden loud noise is measured in the presence or absence of a weak prepulse stimulus, which may be in the same or a different modality. The weak prepulse robustly inhibits the response to the subsequent startling stimulus. In contrast to P50 suppression, PPI is clearly not a form of habituation. In humans, startle is usually assessed via the eye-blink component of startle, using electromyography. In animals, the whole-body flinch aspect of the startle response is quantified using an accelerometer that is sensitive to dynamic movements. As first reported by Braff and colleagues in 1978 (Braff et al., 1978) and confirmed in many subsequent reports (Braff et al., 2001), PPI is reduced in schizophrenia patients. The early demonstrations of PPI deficits in schizophrenia were based on groups of patients who were, for the most part, treated with first generation or so-called typical antipsychotic drugs. More recent studies have demonstrated similar deficits even in first-break patients who had never been treated with any antipsychotics (Ludewig et al., 2003). Thus, deficient PPI in schizophrenia is not attributable to medications or the course of illness, but it is also not reversed by first-generation antipsychotic treatments.
Sensorimotor Gating Deficits in Psychiatric disorders
Studies of PPI as an operational measure of sensorimotor gating were originally intended to test the general theory that failures of inhibitory filtering mechanisms can lead to sensory overload and consequent cognitive fragmentation in schizophrenia (Geyer and Braff, 1987; Braff and Geyer, 1990). Nevertheless, subsequent research has demonstrated that PPI is reduced not only in schizophrenia but also in patients suffering from a variety of neuropsychiatric disorders characterized by deficits in the gating of motor, sensory, and/or cognitive information (Braff et al., 2001). Thus, as reviewed in some detail in Braff et al. (2001), studies prior to 2001 demonstrated that PPI deficits are also evident in patients with schizotypal personality disorder, Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD), Tourette’s Syndrome, and Huntington’s Disorder, and under some experimental conditions PTSD. This group of disorders has been suggested to reflect a family of disorders which can be characterized as having deficits in the gating of motor (Huntington’s, Tourette’s), sensory (schizophrenia), and/or cognitive information (OCD) (Braff et al., 2001; Hoenig et al., 2005). In addition, more recent studies have revealed similar deficits in PPI in another psychotic disorder, namely bipolar disorder patients, at least in the manic phase of the illness (Perry et al., 2001). Bipolar disorder has long been conceptualized as including failure of motor inhibition. Adequate studies have not yet been reported to determine the extent to which these PPI deficits in bipolar disorder are state-related, and specific to the manic phase, or trait markers. One indication supporting the former possibility is the recent report that PPI is normal in effectively treated, non-manic patients with childhood bipolar disorder (Rich et al., 2005). In additional studies prompted in part by suggested genetic contributions to schizophrenia or related disorders, deficits in PPI have also been identified in children with 22q deletion syndrome (Sobin et al., 2005), young persons with Fragile X syndrome (Frankland et al., 2004), and adults with Asperger’s syndrome (McAlonan et al., 2002), seizure disorder (Pouretemad et al., 1998), or Lewy body dementia (Perriol et al., 2005).
As the range of psychiatric disorders in which PPI has been examined has increased, further evidence has accrued to demonstrate that abnormalities in sensorimotor gating are largely independent of classical diagnostic categorizations. Within the family of anxiety disorders, one finds PPI deficits in panic disorder (Ludewig et al., 2002). These deficits in panic disorder patients appear to be reduced though perhaps not abolished by standard clinical treatments (Ludewig et al., 2005). Panic Disorder can also be seen as one which patients suffer from impairments in inhibiting the processing of sensory and cognitive information related to anxiogenic stimuli. Similarly, boys diagnosed with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) exhibit deficits in the attentional modulation of PPI, although they appear to be normal in the typical passive PPI paradigm used in most other studies (Hawk et al., 2003). Furthermore, as appears to be the case in panic disorder, effective treatment of ADHD with methylphenidate reverses the attentional deficit seen in the PPI paradigm (Hawk et al., 2003). It should also be noted, however, that deficient PPI is not found in several other psychiatric disorders, including Parkinson’s disease (Perriol et al., 2005), Alzheimer’s disease (Hejl et al., 2004; Perriol et al., 2005), or unipolar depression (Ludewig and Ludewig, 2003; Perry et al., 2004; Quednow et al., 2004). Thus, deficits in PPI span a wide range of diagnostic categories, although some specificity is evident. The so-called family of psychiatric gating disorders can be seen to be consonant with the broad importance of behavioral inhibition, attention, and information processing to normal behavioral function. PPI appears to tap into fundamental aspects of inhibitory processing that are impacted in a variety of disease states. Such measures are in fact the most tractable to study in cross-species models relevant to psychiatric treatments (Geyer and Markou, 2002). One implication of this observation is that it may be fruitful to consider developing treatments that use PPI paradigms to develop treatments that would improve gating functions independently of broad diagnostic syndromes. Considering such a possibility is timely insofar as recent efforts have begun to establish opportunities for registration of compounds as treatments for specific aspects of complex psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia, without requiring that these treatments affect the entire clinical syndrome.
THE MATRICS PROGRAM
The MATRICS Program to Develop Procognitive Treatments in Schizophrenia
For decades, the United States Food and Drug Administration (FDA) only licensed drugs for use in schizophrenia if they impacted positive psychotic symptoms. This constraint prevented the identification and development of novel treatments for the problems experienced by schizophrenia patients (Fenton et al., 2003; Hyman and Fenton, 2003). Despite the many observations that specific signs and symptoms appear to transcend classical diagnostic categories, the FDA approach to psychiatric treatment has been to treat the entire disorder with one compound, rather than treat specific clinical problems with specific compounds. Clearly, cognitive deficits - which have been long recognized as being important aspects of schizophrenia - are not treated adequately, if at all, by current antipsychotic treatments (Green, 1996; Fenton et al., 2003). Indeed, these cognitive deficits appear to contribute significantly to the poor functional outcome exhibited by schizophrenia patients treated with antipsychotics (Green et al., 2004a). Thus, despite the plethora of existing antipsychotic treatments, the cognitive deficits remain as clinical problems in schizophrenia, and most patients cannot work effectively.
In response to the identification of the bottleneck limiting the development of treatments specifically directed at the cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, the U.S. National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) developed the MATRICS Program. MATRICS - Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia - developed a broad consensus regarding the nature of the cognitive impairments in schizophrenia and how they might best be assessed and treated (Marder and Fenton, 2004). The MATRICS approach to the problem is to treat cognitive deficits and psychotic symptoms separately. Over a period of two years, MATRICS gathered the relevant stakeholders in both industry and academia to achieve a consensus and establish a clear path that would enable the FDA to consider registering compounds intended to treat cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, independently of treating psychosis per se. The paradigmatic use of such compounds would be as co-treatments in schizophrenia patients maintained on stable antipsychotic medications. Hence, these patients would presumably exhibit few of the positive symptoms of schizophrenia that could complicate the assessment of cognitive deficits and impede the ability of patients to benefit from cognitive enhancers.
The MATRICS Consensus Process
MATRICS consisted of a series of consensus conferences, each organized by an expert committee and listed on the MATRICS website (www.matrics.ucla.edu). First, the Neurocognition Committee identified the primary domains of cognitive deficits that characterize patients with schizophrenia: working memory; attention/vigilance; verbal learning and memory; visual learning and memory; speed of processing; reasoning and problem-solving; and social cognition (Nuechterlein et al., 2004). Second, the Neuropharmacology Committee identified the most promising molecular targets, existing compounds, human test measures, and potentially useful animal models for use in the discovery of treatments that target basic mechanisms related to complex cognitive operations (Geyer and Tamminga, 2004). Third, a consensus process was used to develop recommendations for the appropriate cognitive tests to be used in clinical assessments of potential cognitive enhancers (Green et al., 2004b). Fourth, a MATRICS conference examined ways to increase collaborations between NIMH and industry in order to support drug discovery and registration. Fifth, a joint meeting MATRICS meeting brought the FDA and NIMH together to address the processes needed for assessment of cognition as an endpoint in clinical trials (Buchanan et al., 2005). Sixth and last, a concluding MATRICS meeting looked ahead toward a research agenda that would foster improved methods for the discovery, validation, and assessment of pro-cognitive co-treatments for schizophrenia (Geyer and Heinssen, 2005). Throughout the two-year process, the MATRICS conferences included participants from NIMH, NIH, FDA, as well as academic and industry representatives, and were very effective in establishing a broad consensus regarding the appropriate criteria and opportunities for discovering and evaluating co-treatments for the cognitive impairments in schizophrenia.
PREPULSE INHIBITION MODELS IN ANIMALS
Using Prepulse Inhibition for Drug Discovery
As a result of the MATRICS program, drug discovery in schizophrenia is now focused on the identification of potential pro-cognitive co-treatments. In the post-MATRICS era, the question arises as to the possible utility of PPI models in the discovery process for pro-cognitive co-treatments. Since the anticipated application is for co-treatments to be used in patients already stably treated with antipsychotic drugs, any animal model that is responsive to first-generation antipsychotics is likely to be uninformative. The fundamental difficulty in evaluating the potential applicability of animal models for the prediction of pro-cognitive agents in schizophrenia is the absence of any established positive control compound. That is, in the absence of any path to registration of pro-cognitive treatments that do not also treat positive symptoms of schizophrenia, virtually no studies have been done in this specific area (Floresco et al., 2005). What we have some information about, however, are comparisons of different classes of antipsychotic drugs on both cognition and PPI in patients with schizophrenia. It is clear that first-generation antipsychotics, which are principally dopamine D2 antagonists, have no beneficial effects on cognition (Hagan and Jones, 2005). Similarly, as evident from the many early demonstrations of deficient PPI in antipsychotic-treated patients, first-generation compounds do not normalize PPI in schizophrenia (Braff et al., 2001; Kumari and Sharma, 2002). With respect to second-generation antipsychotics, and in particular clozapine, the evidence is less clear but indicates that clozapine and some other multi-receptor antagonist antipsychotics may help to improve cognition to some degree (Meltzer and McGurk, 1999) and appear to be associated with relatively normal PPI (Kumari and Sharma, 2002). Of particular interest in this regard is a cross-sectional study indicating that clozapine treatment, relative to typical antipsychotic treatments, is associated with reduced PPI deficits in patients with schizophrenia (Kumari et al., 1999). While the evidence indicating that second-generation antipsychotics, especially clozapine, may ameliorate PPI deficits in schizophrenia is based largely on cross-sectional studies (Kumari et al., 1999; Hagan and Jones, 2005), some longitudinal studies are now suggesting that PPI deficits may be reversed in groups of schizophrenia patients treated primarily with second-generation compounds (Meincke et al., 2004). Although much more work is needed to clarify the effects of newer antipsychotics on both cognitive and PPI deficits in schizophrenia, it is clear that first-generation anti-psychotics fail to normalize either class of deficits.
NMDA Antagonist Effects in Humans
The original suggestion that glutamatergic systems may contribute to symptoms of schizophrenia, derived from the observation that N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonists such as phencyclidine or ketamine produce psychotic symptoms that resemble those seen in schizophrenia (Krystal et al., 1994; Malhotra et al., 1996; Vollenweider and Geyer, 2001). In contrast to effects produced by dopamine agonists such as amphetamine, which primarily resemble only the positive symptoms of schizophrenia, the effects of NMDA antagonists have been suggested to mimic the positive, negative, and cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia (Krystal et al., 1994; Malhotra et al., 1996; Vollenweider and Geyer, 2001; Geyer and Moghaddam, 2002). Further, administration of the NMDA receptor antagonist ketamine to schizophrenia patients exacerbates both psychotic symptoms and cognitive impairments (Malhotra et al., 1997b). With respect to the cognitive deficits, it appears that, within groups of schizophrenia patients, the most robust correlates of the deficits in PPI are abnormalities in distractibility (Karper et al., 1996) and thought disorder (Perry et al., 1999). Importantly, there is strong evidence that the psychotomimetic effects of ketamine in humans are insensitive to first-generation antipsychotics such as haloperidol, but are reduced in patients treated with clozapine (Malhotra et al., 1997a,b).
Prepulse Inhibition Models in Rats
The cross-species nature of startle and PPI has aided the development of animal models of deficits that are extremely similar to the gating deficits seen in psychiatric disorders. By virtue of the historical focus on treating positive symptoms of psychosis in schizophrenia, most of the existing literature on uses of PPI in animal models has addressed the possible discovery of novel antipsychotic treatments. Given the presumed relevance of dopaminergic systems to schizophrenia, mania, and other psychiatric disorders, the earliest rodent models began with demonstrations of the ability of dopamine agonists to disrupt PPI in rats. Subsequently, the rat PPI models have evolved into at least four different models (Geyer et al., 2001). These distinct models have PPI measures in common but are differentiated by the manipulations used to disrupt PPI: 1) psychostimulant dopamine agonists; 2) hallucinogenic serotonin agonists; 3) psychotomimetic NMDA receptor antagonists; and 4) developmental manipulations such as isolation rearing or neonatal lesions of the ventral hippocampus. Three of the models are based on changes induced by acutely administered psychotomimetic drugs. While pharmacological approaches that alter PPI help to identify relevant neural substrates, they do not assess environmental or developmental contributions to PPI deficits. In contrast, the fourth PPI model is based on the loss of PPI in adult rats subsequent to social isolation during development (Powell and Geyer, 2002). Although this isolation rearing model and other developmentally based models has proven to be of value in testing antipsychotic treatments (Pen and Moreau, 2002; Powell and Geyer, 2002; Cilia et al., 2005), only the dopamine and NMDA models are particularly relevant for the present discussion.
The Dopamine Prepulse Inhibition Model in Rats
As detailed in other reviews (Geyer et al., 2001; Swerdlow et al., 2001), PPI deficits that mimic those seen in schizophrenia were first produced in animals by the administration of direct or indirect dopamine agonists, such as apomorphine or d-amphetamine (Geyer et al., 1990). The original dopamine model focused primarily on testing the ability of antipsychotic drugs to block the PPI-disruptive effects of apomorphine in rats (Swerdlow et al., 1994). In sum, these effects of apomorphine in rats are reliably prevented by virtually all antipsychotics that have appreciable affinity for dopamine D2 receptors. Swerdlow et al. (1994) have shown an excellent correlation between the clinical potency of an antipsychotic and its ability to block the PPI-disruptive effects of the dopamine agonist apomorphine in rats. Although this finding provides important validation of the predictive validity of the dopamine PPI model for antipsychotic drugs, it primarily reflects the importance of dopamine D2 antagonism in antipsychotic drug action and therefore only recapitulates in a behavioral paradigm what was already known from simple ligand-binding assays. Thus, the dopamine agonist PPI model is an example of what we have called “receptor tautology”, given that the receptor mechanism of the agonist used to induce the schizophrenia-like PPI deficit predicts the antagonists that the behavioral test will identify. Furthermore, in the context of searching for pro-cognitive co-treatments to be added to stable regimens of current antipsychotics that have dopamine D2 antagonist actions, any animal model based on D2 agonist effects is clearly irrelevant.
In contrast to D2 antagonist effects, manipulations of dopamine D1 receptors was considered one of the most promising possible targets for pro-cognitive agents in schizophrenia by the MATRICS Neuropharmacology group (Goldman-Rakic et al., 2004). The effects of dopamine agonists on PPI in rats are clearly due largely to actions at the dopamine D2-family of receptors (Geyer et al., 2001), which is quite consistent with the actions of existing antipsychotic drugs. Overall, D2 agonists rather than D1 agonists reduce PPI in rats, with the corresponding antagonists have the expected opposing effects. In addition, the effects of indirect releasers of dopamine, such as amphetamine or cocaine, also appear to disrupt PPI in rats via actions mediated at D2-family receptors. Nevertheless, several reports have demonstrated important differences between rat strains in their sensitivity - or insensitivity - to the PPI-disruptive effects of dopamine agonists (Swerdlow et al., 2000; Geyer et al., 2001). Furthermore, recent studies by Swerdlow’s group have shown some important differences in the influences of direct D1 and D2 agonists in various rat strains, and have even demonstrated the heritability of some of these differences (Swerdlow et al., 2006). In mice, the effects of dopaminergic manipulations on PPI are even more complex. In particular, the influences of dopamine D1 receptors on PPI appear to be much more important in mice than in rats. Extensive studies with both selective antagonists and receptor subtype-specific knockout mice have demonstrated that the effects of amphetamine on PPI are attributable to actions at the D2 subtype of the D2-family of dopamine receptors, and not to D1, D3, or D4 receptors (Ralph et al., 1999; Ralph-Williams et al., 2002). Similarly, the disruptions of PPI observed in dopamine transporter knockout mice, likely mediated indirectly via the increased synaptic levels of dopamine, are reversed by D2 and not D1 receptor antagonists (Ralph et al., 2001). Nevertheless, in contrast to rats, D1 agonists are much more effective than D2 agonists in disrupting PPI in mice (Ralph-Williams et al., 2002; 2003; Ralph and Caine, 2005). These effects of the direct D1 agonists are prevented by D1 and not D2 antagonists (Ralph-Williams et al., 2003) and are absent in D1 but not in D2 knockout mice (Ralph- Williams et al., 2002). Thus, while indirect agonist effects on PPI in mice appear to be mediated similarly to those observed in mice, the actions of direct dopamine agonists in mice appear to differ from those in rats or, for that matter, from the effects of indirect dopamine agonists in mice. Clearly, more research is needed to clarify the sources of these surprising disparities.
Such disparities prompt considerations of which rodent literature is more relevant to the influences of dopamine receptors on PPI in humans, though adequate discussion of this issue is beyond the scope of this review. In brief, as summarized elsewhere (Swerdlow et al., 2001; 2002), neither indirect dopamine agonists, such as amphetamine, nor direct dopamine D2 agonists, such as bromocriptine, pergolide, or ropinirole, reliably disrupt PPI in humans. While the doses of dopamine agonists used in some studies may have been inadequate, even the effects of dopaminergic antagonists that have been observed in humans have not been readily replicated (e.g., Abduljawad et al., 1999). Furthermore, as noted above, PPI deficits in schizophrenia patients are clearly not reversed by even prolonged treatment with first-generation antipsychotic treatments having clear antagonist actions at D2 dopamine receptors (Braff et al., 2001). Since there is almost no evidence regarding the possible influences of dopamine D1 receptor manipulations on PPI in humans, the potential relevance of the effectiveness of D1 agonists and antagonists on measures of PPI in mice cannot yet be evaluated.
The NMDA Antagonist Prepulse Inhibition Model
The rodent PPI model that shows the greatest potential to provide insight into the unique effects of second- rather than first-generation antipsychotics is the NMDA antagonist model. As reviewed elsewhere (Geyer et al., 2001), both competitive and non-competitive NMDA antagonists (e.g., phencyclidine, dizocilpine, and ketamine) produce robust deficits in PPI in rats, mice, or infra-human primates. In addition, the NMDA antagonist model appears to be the least sensitive of the existing rodent models to strain differences in the observed effects (Geyer et al., 2001). Many studies of the effects of antipsychotics on the PPI-disruptive effects of NMDA antagonists have confirmed that first-generation antipsychotics such as haloperidol do not attenuate the PPI-disruptive effects of NMDA antagonists in rats (Geyer et al., 1990; 2001). Similarly, the effects of NMDA antagonists on PPI are maintained in mice treated with dopamine antagonists or in mutant mice lacking specific subtypes of dopamine receptors (Geyer et al., 2002; Ralph-Williams et al., 2002). In contrast, clozapine and some other second-generation antipsychotics have been demonstrated to reduce the disruption in PPI produced by NMDA antagonists in both rats (Geyer et al., 2001) and mice (Geyer et al., 2002; Brody et al., 2004). This interaction between clozapine and NMDA antagonists is seen only with a limited range of doses and has been confirmed in many but not all studies in rats (Geyer et al., 2001). Complementing the studies in rodents, clozapine has been demonstrated to attenuate the effects of phencyclidine on PPI in monkeys, while haloperidol was ineffective (Linn et al., 2003). These results in experimental animals are consistent with the human studies discussed above, indicating that the psychotic symptoms produced by NMDA antagonists are not reduced by typical antipsychotics but are blocked by clozapine (Malhotra et al., 1997a,b). Such findings led to the suggestion that the phencyclidine-PPI model might enable the specific identification of atypical rather than typical antipsychotic treatments (Geyer and Ellenbroek, 2003).
There is some limited evidence that chronic treatments with relatively high doses of typical antipsychotics in rats can attenuate the effects of NMDA antagonists on PPI (Geyer et al., 2001), in contrast to the ineffectiveness of acute administrations of these drugs. While further studies along these lines are clearly warranted, it should be noted that the antipsychotic effects of dopamine antagonists in patients appear to be evident even acutely and attributable to the same pharmacological actions that are responsible for the longer term further improvement in psychosis ratings seen with chronic antipsychotic administration. Thus, the use of acute treatments in predictive animal models is consistent with the clinical reality. Furthermore, the apparent effectiveness of chronic administrations of typical antipsychotics in the NMDA model is not corroborated by the studies of dopamine receptor knockout mice, which clearly demonstrate that neither D1 nor D2 dopamine receptors are required for NMDA antagonists to disrupt PPI (Ralph-Williams et al., 2002).
The interactions between second-generation antipsychotics and NMDA antagonists in PPI paradigms are unlikely to be mediated by competition for a common receptor, because antipsychotics do not have appreciable affinity for NMDA receptors. Rather, the reductions in NMDA antagonist-induced PPI deficits following clozapine-like antipsychotics likely reflect interactions within the complex forebrain circuitry that modulates PPI (Bakshi and Geyer, 1998; Swerdlow et al., 2001). Thus, the NMDA antagonist PPI model does not appear to be another instance of receptor tautology and may, therefore, provide a pathway to identification of novel molecular targets for treatments of schizophrenia.
CONCLUSIONS
Deficits in PPI were originally examined in patients with schizophrenia, testing the theory that gating deficits contribute to cognitive disorganization. Nevertheless, PPI deficits have been observed subsequently in multiple psychiatric disorders. Thus, as with most signs and symptoms of psychiatric disorders, deficits in PPI cut across diagnostic categories. It remains unclear whether the diversity of disorders exhibiting deficient PPI bespeaks diagnostic overlaps or comorbidities. Rather than focus on this question, many have argued that psychiatric treatments need to target specific components of illness rather than entire syndromes. Indeed, the PPI deficits in schizophrenia patients, like the cognitive deficits that are core features of schizophrenia, are not reversed by most antipsychotic drugs. Recently, the MATRICS program has fostered new approaches to develop treatments for cognitive deficits and other specific aspects of schizophrenia independently of treating psychosis. It appears that the cognitive deficits that are refractory to existing treatments represent a critical unmet treatment need. Accordingly, the possible relationship of PPI deficits to cognitive deficits is of current interest in both human and animal studies. Although PPI cannot be considered to be a cognitive process per se, abnormalities in pre-attentive information processing may be predictive of or even lead to complex cognitive deficits. Animal models of PPI deficits produced by dopamine agonists reliably predict existing antipsychotics. Given that the patients will already be treated with antipsychotics having antagonist actions at dopamine D2 receptors, however, dopamine agonist models are inappropriate for use in identifying pro-cognitive co-treatments in schizophrenia. Since neither PPI nor cognitive deficits in schizophrenia are ameliorated by most existing antipsychotics, current research is exploring the predictive value of non-dopaminergic PPI models in identifying treatments for gating disturbances independently of their relevance to schizophrenia, bipolar mania, panic disorder, or other psychiatric disorders. Perhaps the most promising of such models is that based on PPI disruptions induced by acute NMDA antagonists. As discussed above, the PPI-disruptive effects of NMDA antagonists in rats and mice are clearly insensitive to most first-generation antipsychotic treatments but are attenuated by clozapine and some other second-generation anti-psychotics. Hence, the rodent model based on the disruption of PPI produced by NMDA antagonists may reveal information that is specifically relevant to the responsiveness of some neuroleptic-resistant patients to second-generation antipsychotics such as clozapine.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (MH071916, MH61326, MH42228), NARSAD, and the Veteran’s Administration VISN 22 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center. MAG holds an equity interest in San Diego Instruments.
References
- Abduljawad KA, Langley RW, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E. Effects of bromocriptine and haloperidol on pre-pulse inhibition: comparison of the acoustic startle eyeblink response and the N1/P2 auditory-evoked response in man. J Psychopharmacology. 1999;13:3–9. doi: 10.1177/026988119901300101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bakshi VP, Geyer MA. Multiple limbic regions mediate the disruption of prepulse inhibition produced in rats by the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist, dizocilpine. J Neurosci. 1998;18:8394–8401. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-20-08394.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braff DL, Geyer MA. Sensorimotor gating and schizophrenia: human and animal model studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47:181–188. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810140081011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braff D, Stone C, Callaway E, Geyer M, Glick I, Bali L. Prestimulus effects on human startle reflex in normals and schizophrenics. Psychophysiology. 1978;15:339–343. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1978.tb01390.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braff DL, Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR. Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:234–258. doi: 10.1007/s002130100810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody SA, Conquet F, Geyer MA. Effect of antipsychotic treatment on the prepulse inhibition deficit of mgluR5 knockout mice. Psychopharmacology. 2004;172:187–195. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1635-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buchanan RW, Davis M, Goff D, et al. A summary of the FDA-NIMH-MATRICS workshop on clinical trial design for neurocognitive drugs for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:5–19. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cilia J, Hatcher PD, Reavill C, Jones DNC. Long-term evaluation of isolation-rearing induced prepulse inhibition deficits in rats: an update. Psychopharmacology. 2005;180:57–62. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2139-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenton WS, Stover EL, Insel TR. Breaking the logjam in treatment development for cognition in schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology. 2003;169:365–366. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1564-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floresco S, Geyer MA, Gold LH, Grace A. Developing predictive animal models and establishing a preclinical trials network for assessing treatment effects on cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:888–894. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankland PW, Wang Y, Rosner B, Shimizu T, Balleine BW, Dykens EM, Ornitz EM, Silva AJ. Sensorimotor gating abnormalities in young males with Fragile X syndrome and Fmr1-knockout mice. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9:417–425. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freedman R, Adler LE, Leonard S. Alternative phenotypes for the complex genetics of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45:551–558. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00321-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Braff DL. Startle habituation and sensorimotor gating in schizophrenia and related animal models. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:643–668. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.4.643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Ellenbroek BA. Animal behavior models of the mechanisms underlying antipsychotic atypicality. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2003;27:1071–1079. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2003.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Heinssen R. New approaches to measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia (MATRICS) Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:806–809. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Markou A. The role of preclinical models in the development of psychotropic drugs. In: Davis KL, Charney D, Coyle JT, Nemeroff C, editors. Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress. Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; Philadelphia, PA: 2002. pp. 445–455. [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Moghaddam B. Animal models relevant to schizophrenia disorders. In: Davis KL, Charney D, Coyle JT, Nemeroff C, editors. Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress. Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; Philadelphia, PA: 2002. pp. 689–701. [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Tamminga CA. Measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia: neuropharmacological aspects. Psychopharmacology. 2004;174:1–2. [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR, Mansbach RS, Braff DL. Startle response models of sensorimotor gating and habituation deficits in schizophrenia. Brain Res Bull. 1990;25:485–498. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(90)90241-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Krebs-Thomson K, Braff DL, Swerdlow NR. Pharmacological studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia: a decade in review. Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:117–154. doi: 10.1007/s002130100811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, McIlwain KL, Paylor R. Mouse genetic models for prepulse inhibition: an early review. Mol Psychiatry. 2002;7:1039–1053. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman-Rakic PS, Castner SA, Svensson TH, Siever LJ, Williams GV. Targeting the dopamine D1 receptor in schizophrenia: insights for cognitive dysfunction. Psychopharmacology. 2004;174:3–16. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1793-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graham FK. The more or less startling effects of weak prestimuli. Psychophysiology. 1975;12:238–248. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1975.tb01284.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MF. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psych. 1996;153:321–330. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.3.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MF, Kerns SR, Heaton RK. Longitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: implications for MATRICS. Schizophr Res. 2004a;72:45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Gold JM, et al. Approaching a consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials in schizophrenia: the NIMH-MATRICS conference to select cognitive domains and test criteria. Biol Psychiatry. 2004b;56:301–307. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagan JJ, Jones DN. Predicting drug efficacy for cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:830–853. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawk LW, Yartz AR, Pelham WE, Lock TM. The effects of methylphenidate on prepulse inhibition during attended and ignored prestimuli among boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychopharmacology. 2003;165:118–127. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1235-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hejl A, Glenthøj B, Mackeprang T, Hemmingsen R, Waldemar G. Prepulse inhibition in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:1045–1050. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoenig K, Hochrein A, Quednow BB, Maier W, Wagner M. Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1153–1158. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman SE, Fenton WS. What are the right targets for psychopharmacology? Science. 2003;299:350–351. doi: 10.1126/science.1077141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karper LP, Freeman GK, Grillon C, Morgan CA, 3rd, Charney DS, Krystal JH. Preliminary evidence of an association between sensorimotor gating and distractibility in psychosis. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1996;8:60–66. doi: 10.1176/jnp.8.1.60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krystal JH, Karper LP, Seibyl JP, Freeman GK, Delaney R, Bremner JD, Heninger GR, Bowers MB, Jr, Charney DS. Subanesthetic effects of the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist, ketamine, in humans. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51:199–214. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950030035004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumari V, Sharma T. Effects of typical and atypical antipsychotics on prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia: a critical evaluation of current evidence and directions for future research. Psychopharmacology. 2002;162:97–101. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1099-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumari V, Soni W, Sharma T. Normalization of information processing deficits in schizophrenia with clozapine. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:1046–1051. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.7.1046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linn GS, Negi SS, Gerum SV, Javitt DC. Reversal of phencyclidine-induced prepulse inhibition deficits by clozapine in monkeys. Psychopharmacology. 2003;169:234–239. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1533-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludewig S, Ludewig K. No prepulse inhibition deficits in patients with unipolar depression. Depress Anxiety. 2003;17:224–225. doi: 10.1002/da.10109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludewig S, Ludewig K, Geyer MA, Hell D, Vollenweider FX. Prepulse inhibition deficits in patients with panic disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2002;15:55–60. doi: 10.1002/da.10026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludewig K, Geyer MA, Vollenweider FX. Deficits in prepulse inhibition and habituation in never-medicated first-episode schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:121–128. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01925-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludewig S, Geyer MA, Ramseier M, Vollenweider FX, Rechsteiner E, Ludewig K. Information processing deficits and cognitive dysfunctions in panic disorder. J Psychiatr Neurosci. 2005;30:37–43. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malhotra AK, Pinals DA, Weingartner H, Sirocco K, Missar CD, Pickar D, Breier A. NMDA receptor function and human cognition: the effects of ketamine in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1996;14:301–307. doi: 10.1016/0893-133X(95)00137-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malhotra AK, Adler CM, Kennison SD, Elman I, Pickar D, Breier A. Clozapine blunts N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist-induced psychosis: a study with ketamine. Biol Psychiatry. 1997a;42:664–668. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00546-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malhotra AK, Pinals DA, Adler CM, Elman I, Clifton A, Pickar D, Breier A. Ketamine-induced exacerbation of psychotic symptoms and cognitive impairment in neuroleptic-free schizophrenics. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997b;17:141–150. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00036-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marder SR, Fenton W. Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia: NIMH MATRICS initiative to support the development of agents for improving cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:5–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin LF, Kem WR, Freedman R. Alpha-7 nicotinic receptor agonists: potential new candidates for the treatment of schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology. 2004;174:54–64. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1750-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAlonan GM, Daly E, Kumari V, Critchley HD, van Amelsvoort T, Suckling J, Simmons A, Sigmundsson T, Greenwood K, Russell A, Schmitz N, Happe F, Howlin P, Murphy DG. Brain anatomy and sensorimotor gating in Asperger’s syndrome. Brain. 2002;125(part 7):1594–1606. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meincke U, Mörth D, Voss T, Thelen B, Geyer MA, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E. Prepulse inhibition of the acoustically evoked startle reflex in patients with an acute schizophrenic psychosis - a longitudinal study. Eur Arch Psychiatr Clin Neurosci. 2004;254:415–421. doi: 10.1007/s00406-004-0523-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meltzer HY, McGurk SR. The effects of clozapine, risperidone, and olanzapine on cognitive function in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1999;25:233–255. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nuechterlein KH, Barch DM, Gold JM, Goldberg TE, Green MF, Heaton RK. Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pen G, Moreau JL. Disruption of prepulse inhibition of startle reflex in a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: reversal by clozapine, olanzapine and risperidone but not by haloperidol. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:1–11. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00383-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perriol M-P, Dujardin K, Derambure P, Marcq A, Bourriez J-L, Laureau E, Pasquier F, Defebvre L, Deste’e A. Disturbance of sensory filtering in dementia with Lewy bodies comparison with Parkinson’s disease dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76:106–108. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.035022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry W, Geyer MA, Braff DL. Sensorimotor gating and thought disturbance measured in close temporal proximity in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:227–281. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.3.277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry W, Minassian A, Feifel D, Braff DL. Sensorimotor gating deficits in bipolar disorder patients with acute psychotic mania. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:418–424. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01184-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry W, Minassian A, Feifel D. Prepulse inhibition in patients with non-psychotic major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2004;81:179–184. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00157-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pouretemad HR, Thompson PJ, Fenwick BC. Impaired sensorimotor gating in patients with non-epileptic seizures. Epilepsy Res. 1998;31:1–12. doi: 10.1016/s0920-1211(98)00014-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powell SB, Geyer MA. Developmental markers of psychiatric disorders as identified by sensorimotor gating. Neurotoxicity Res. 2002;4:489–502. doi: 10.1080/10298420290030578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quednow BB, Kuhn KU, Stelzenmueller R, Hoenig K, Maier W, Wagner M. Effects of serotonergic and noradrenergic antidepressants on auditory startle response in patients with major depression. Psychopharmacology. 2004;175:399–406. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1842-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ralph RJ, Caine SB. Dopamine D1 and D2 agonist effects on prepulse inhibition and locomotion: comparison of Sprague-Dawley rats to Swiss-Webster, 129X1/SvJ, C57BL/6J, and DBA/2J Mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2005;312:733–741. doi: 10.1124/jpet.104.074468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ralph RJ, Varty GB, Kelly MA, Wang Y-M, Caron MG, Rubinstein M, Grandy DK, Low MJ, Geyer MA. The dopamine D2 but not D3 or D4 receptor subtype is essential for the disruption of prepulse inhibition produced by amphetamine in mice. J Neurosci. 1999;19:4627–4633. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04627.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ralph RJ, Paulus MP, Fumagalli F, Caron MG, Geyer MA. Prepulse inhibition deficits and perseverative motor patterns in dopamine transporter knockout mice: differential effects of D1 and D2 receptor antagonists. J Neurosci. 2001;21:305–313. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-01-00305.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ralph-Williams RJ, Lehmann-Masten V, Otero-Corchon V, Low MJ, Geyer MA. Differential effects of direct and indirect dopamine agonists on prepulse inhibition: a study in D1 and D2 receptor knockout mice. J Neurosci. 2002;22:9604–9611. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-21-09604.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ralph-Williams RJ, Lehmann-Masten V, Geyer MA. Dopamine D1 rather than D2 receptor agonists disrupt pre-pulse inhibition of startle in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:108–118. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rich BA, Vinton D, Grillon C, Bhangoo RK, Leibenluft E. An investigation of prepulse inhibition in pediatric bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2005;7:198–203. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00183.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sobin C, Kiley-Brabeck K, Karayiorgou M. Lower prepulse inhibition in children with the 22q11 deletion syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1090–1099. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Taaid N, Geyer MA. Assessing the validity of an animal model of deficient sensorimotor gating in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51:139–154. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950020063007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerdlow NR, Martinez ZA, Hanlon F, Platten A, Farid M, Auerbach P, Braff DL, Geyer MA. Towards understanding the biology of a complex phenotype: rat strain and substrain differences in the sensorimotor gating-disruptive effects of dopamine agonists. J Neurosci. 2000;20:4325–4336. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-11-04325.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Braff DL. Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:194–215. doi: 10.1007/s002130100799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerdlow NR, Eastvold A, Karban B, Ploum Y, Stephany N, Geyer MA, Cadenhead KS, Auerbach PP. Dopamine agonist effects on startle and sensorimotor gating in normal control subjects: time course studies. Psychopharmacology. 2002;161:189–201. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1040-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerdlow NR, Krupin AS, Bongiovanni MJ, Shoemaker JM, Goins JC, Hammer RP. Heritable differences in the dopaminergic regulation of behavior in rats: relationship to D2-like receptor G-protein function. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006 doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vollenweider FX, Geyer MA. A systems model of altered consciousness: integrating natural and drug-induced psychoses. Brain Res Bull. 2001;56:495–507. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00646-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]