Abstract
Animal models have long been used to explore hypotheses regarding the neurobiological substrates of and treatments for psychiatric disorders. Early attempts to develop models that mimic the entirety of the diagnostic syndromes in psychiatry have evolved into more appropriate efforts to model specific symptoms. Such an approach reflects the facts that even in patients, clinical symptoms transcend diagnostic categories, and the specific etiologies of psychiatric disorders are unknown. An animal model can only be identified adequately by specifying both the manipulation (drug, lesion, strain) used to induce abnormalities and the measure(s) used to characterize them. A wide range of pharmacological, lesion, and developmental manipulations have been combined with various measures of information processing to develop useful animal models that parallel human tests. Prepulse inhibition of startle, event-related potential (ERP) measures of auditory gating, and Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB) measures of cognition are examples of measures that can be used in both rodents and humans and that are robustly altered in both psychiatric disorders and animals manipulated with appropriate drugs or lesions.
The further development of cross-species models is critically important, given the new opportunities for the development and registration of specific treatments for the cognitive disorders of schizophrenia that are not ameliorated by available drugs. In moving beyond the focus on psychotic symptoms to the cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia, animal models that do not involve manipulations of dopamine D2 receptors but that do utilize information processing measures that are correlated with cognitive disturbances are receiving increased attention.
Here, selected examples of how cross-species measures of psychiatric disorders are developed and validated are discussed. Specific animal paradigms that parallel the specific domains of cognition that are altered in schizophrenia provide one focus of the review. Another focus includes efforts to develop new human models of psychiatric symptoms that are designed to parallel existing tests used in rodents.
Keywords: Translational biomarkers, Schizophrenia, Bipolar mania, Prepulse inhibition
INTRODUCTION
As summarized by Hagan and Jones (2005), the drug discovery process involves multiple steps that feed forward and backward upon one another. The beginning of the process necessarily involves the construct, the conceptual identification of a molecular pharmacological target that might beneficially modify a clinically defined deficit. This initial idea must first be evolved into some small drug-able molecules that can constitute the first actual step in the process. These small molecules or proteins are used to confirm actions at the relevant biological mechanism using in vitro and eventually in vivo model systems. The resulting tool compounds are then used in appropriate animal models, for example in models of the cognitive deficits in schizophrenia or manic depressive psychoses. Due to the high costs of studies in human volunteers, higher throughput animal models are typically required for this phase of the process. Studies in healthy humans then proceed and, if they are successful, progress to initial studies in small numbers of affected patients. Only after these steps are completed are larger clinical trials undertaken to assess the appropriate dose levels, treatment regimens, and eventually clinical efficacy of a candidate compound. Throughout this drug discovery process, the information from each successive stage is fed back into the preceding stages, prompting appropriate refinements in an iterative process. Thus, early studies in healthy humans might identify unwanted side effects that prompt a return to earlier stages to identify alternative compounds having similar on-target effects but with different side effect profiles. Similarly, observations of preferential effects in some domains of cognition over others might prompt a revised selection of test paradigms in the animal model proof of concept studies, which in turn might influence subsequent more specific studies of specific cognitive tests in healthy volunteers and/or small studies of relevant patients. As noted by Hagan and Jones (2005, page 832), “Rational preclinical strategies for hypothesis testing and compound characterisation are therefore essential in order to enable informed choices between mechanisms and molecules. These strategies should not be restricted to behavioural models and a range of functional techniques may eventually prove useful. Establishing robust links between each stage of testing and building predictive algorithms is essential if pre-clinical models are to be relied upon to predict clinical efficacy.” It is the thesis of this review that this essential bidirectional cross-talk between the preclinical models and the clinical assessments of efficacy is best accomplished by developing and using physiological and behavioral measures and test paradigms that maximize the similarity of the observed phenomena across species. It is readily apparent that such efforts will be most cost-effective if the focus of decision making is on early proof of principle studies in late Phase I or early Phase II studies, given the enormous costs and risks of large-scale Phase III clinical trials. Thus, extremely valuable information could be derived from experimental paradigms based on comparable measures in both preclinical animal models and in early small-scale clinical tests. In addition, this focus on early clinical assessments enables the use of behavioral, imaging, or physiological assessments that more closely link to the preclinical tests without requiring the simplicity and low-cost needed for large-scale clinical trials.
COGNITIVE DEFICITS IN PSYCHOTIC DISORDERS
As reviewed elsewhere (Geyer, 2006), Fenton and colleagues (Fenton et al., 2003) identified a critical bottleneck limiting the development of treatments specifically directed at the cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. The domain of cognitive deficits has been long recognized as being a core aspect of the group of schizophrenia disorders that is not treated adequately by current antipsychotic treatments. Similar problems are evident in the treatment of bipolar mania. Although many antipsychotic treatments have been identified and marketed, the cognitive deficits remain as clinical problems in schizophrenia and most patients are burdened by significant psychosocial deficits (Green, 1996). In response, NIMH developed an initiative called “Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia” or MATRICS, which developed a broad consensus regarding how the cognitive impairments in schizophrenia might be assessed and treated (Marder and Fenton, 2004). MATRICS helped establish a way for FDA to consider registering compounds intended to treat cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, independent of treating psychosis per se.
Given the absence of any treatments known to ameliorate the cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, preclinical drug discovery programs have difficulty assessing the predictive validity of the many cognitive tests available (Floresco et al., 2005). As a result, current efforts are based primarily on our understanding of the theoretical constructs and neurobiology related to cognition. The cognitive constructs or domains identified by the MATRICS program as being the primary cognitive deficits in schizophrenia include: working memory; attention/vigilance; verbal learning and memory; visual learning and memory; speed of processing; reasoning and problem-solving; and social cognition (Nuechterlein et al., 2004). MATRICS also developed a Neurocognitive Battery of appropriate cognitive tests to be used in clinical assessments of potential cognitive enhancers, which is now publicly available. A subsequent program - “Cognitive Neuroscience measures of Treatment Response of Impaired Cognition in Schizophrenia” (CNTRICS) - is designed to bring the modern tools and concepts of cognitive neuroscience to bear upon the assessment of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia and the efficacy of pharmacotherapeutics in ameliorating these deficits (see http://cntrics.ucdavis.edu). The need to identify and develop cross-species tools with which to predict and evaluate novel treatments of cognitive deficits is being addressed by another NIMH-funded program: “Treatment Units for Research on Neurocognition in Schizophrenia” (TURNS). This multi-site clinical trials network is implementing the clinical trial design developed by the MATRICS program (see http://www.turns.ucla.edu). TURNS includes a Biomarkers Subcommittee designed to facilitate the inclusion of specific biomarkers in conjunction with clinical tests (see http://www.turns.ucla.edu). The supplementation of clinical neurocognitive assessments with biochemical, genetic, psychophysiological, or brain imaging measures having the potential to serve as biomarkers should facilitate the processes of drug discovery and development. There is a clear need for the identification of predictive pathways from the preclinical rodent models that are essential for the rapid screening of compounds of interest to the first proof-of-concept studies in humans that are essential for go versus no-go decisions in drug development (Floresco et al., 2005; Hagan and Jones, 2005). Many groups are now exploring translational paradigms that have construct validity for the assessment of cognition and may be applicable across species. Accordingly, a Preclinical Subcommittee of TURNS has surveyed experts from academia and industry and assembled a listing of preclinical tests that may have utility as predictive measures of the clinical treatment of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia (Young et al., 2006). These preclinical models, as ranked in this survey, are now being used especially by pharmaceutical companies to guide their preclinical drug discovery and validation programs.
TRANSLATIONAL MEASURES OF GATING DEFICITS
Two instructive exemplars relevant to developing cross-species tests having utility in psychiatric drug discovery were developed as measures of the abnormalities in gating mechanisms that are hypothesized to contribute to deficits in attention, vigilance, and cognition in psychotic disorders. Specifically, measures of sensory or sensorimotor gating are among the most widely studied physiological markers used in laboratory studies of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. The first of these exemplars is the auditory “sensory gating” paradigm pioneered by Freedman’s group. This procedure involves a condition-test paired-stimulus paradigm in which the P50 event-related potential (ERP) elicited by the second of two audible clicks is normally reduced relative to the ERP elicited by the first click (Martin et al., 2004). In schizophrenia or manic patients, however, this suppression of the P50 is diminished, apparently due to a reduction in short-term habituation. An analogous paradigm has been developed for use in rodents (Freedman et al., 1999). This cross-species ERP paradigm has been critical in the identification of the α7 nicotinic receptor as a potential target for pro-cognitive co-treatments in schizophrenia (Freedman et al., 1999).
Another cross-species gating paradigm, prepulse inhibition of startle (PPI), also assesses the gating construct and yet differs qualitatively from the P50 ERP paradigm. Since it involves both sensory stimuli and motor responses, PPI is considered a measure of “sensorimotor gating” rather than sensory gating (Braff and Geyer, 1990). In PPI, the startle response elicited by a strong sudden stimulus, usually acoustic or tactile, is measured in the presence or absence of a weak prepulse stimulus, which may be in the same or a different modality. The weak prepulse robustly inhibits the response to the subsequent startling stimulus. In contrast to P50 suppression, PPI is clearly not a form of habituation. In humans, startle is usually assessed via the eyeblink component of startle, using electromyography. In animals, the whole-body flinch aspect of the startle response is quantified using an accelerometer that is sensitive to dynamic movements. Like P50 gating, PPI is reduced in schizophrenia and manic bipolar patients (Braff et al., 2001; Perry et al., 2001). The early demonstrations of PPI deficits in schizophrenia were based on groups of patients who were, for the most part, treated with first generation or so-called typical antipsychotic drugs. More recent studies have demonstrated similar deficits even in first-break patients who had never been treated with any antipsychotics (Ludewig et al., 2003). Thus, deficient PPI in schizophrenia is not attributable to medications or the course of illness, but it is also not reversed by first-generation antipsychotic treatments.
As reviewed elsewhere (Geyer et al., 2001; Swerdlow et al., 2001), the cross-species nature of startle and PPI facilitates the use of animal models of gating deficits that are extremely similar to the abnormalities seen in schizophrenia. Since the early finding that dopamine agonists disrupt PPI in rats, rodent PPI tests have evolved into at least four distinct models. These models have PPI measures in common but are distinguished by the manipulations used to disrupt PPI: 1) psychostimulant dopamine agonists; 2) hallucinogenic serotonin agonists; 3) psychotomimetic N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonists; and 4) developmental manipulations such as isolation rearing or neonatal lesions of the ventral hippocampus. The first three models are based on changes induced by acutely administered drugs and have helped to identify relevant neural substrates. Nevertheless, they do not assess environmental or developmental contributions to PPI deficits as does the PPI model based on the deficits seen in adult rats reared in social isolation post-weaning (Powell and Geyer, 2002; Cilia et al., 2005). Considerable evidence indicates that the dopamine PPI model is reliably predictive of existing antipsychotics, and that the NMDA PPI model is insensitive to typical antipsychotics but responsive to clozapine and some other multi-receptor antagonists. Given the MATRICS program, the utility of PPI models in the discovery process for pro-cognitive co-treatments becomes an important question. Although PPI cannot be considered to be a cognitive process per se, abnormalities in pre-attentive gating may be predictive of or contribute to complex cognitive deficits. Since the anticipated application is for co-treatments to be used in patients already treated with antipsychotic drugs, animal models that are responsive to first-generation antipsychotics are likely to be uninformative. It appears that neither PPI nor cognitive deficits in schizophrenia are reversed by most existing antipsychotics but may be partially ameliorated by drugs such as clozapine (Kumari et al., 1999; Meltzer and McGurk, 1999). Since the same can be said of the PPI disruptions produced by NMDA antagonists (Geyer and Ellenbroek, 2003), the glutamate PPI model may have utility in the identification of pro-cognitive adjunctive treatments.
The PPI paradigm has also proven to be informative in studies of normal and genetically engineered mice (Geyer et al., 2002). One striking aspect of PPI in mice that was not so apparent in earlier studies of rats is that antipsychotics improve baseline PPI. Hence, it has been suggested that strains of mice that exhibit low baseline levels of PPI may provide simple approaches to the identification of novel antipsychotic drugs (Olivier et al., 2001; Ouagazzal et al., 2001). It had been noted some time ago that clozapine, and not haloperidol, slightly improved baseline PPI in rats (Swerdlow and Geyer, 1993). More recent work has shown that antipsychotics, especially atypical compounds such as clozapine, produce relatively robust increases in baseline PPI in rat strains characterized by low PPI (Palmer et al., 2000; Feifel et al., 2007). Capitalizing on the cross-species nature of PPI testing, researchers have now begun to test the ability of antipsychotic drugs to improve PPI in healthy humans who are selected on the basis of having low baseline PPI. Indeed, when healthy volunteers are stratified in terms of low versus high PPI, atypical antipsychotics such as clozapine or quetiapine increase PPI in low responders while having no effect in high responders (Swerdlow et al., 2006; Vollenweider et al., 2006). As with the PPI deficits in schizophrenia, this effect of clozapine-like drugs is not evident with haloperidol (Csomor et al., 2008). Further research is warranted to determine whether this simple test might provide a proof of concept biomarker for the identification of either atypical antipsychotics or potentially procognitive adjunctive treatments.
BI-DIRECTIONAL DEVELOPMENT OF TRANSLATIONAL MEASURES
The preceding example of cross-species development of psychiatric test paradigms illustrated transitions from human to animal and back to human tests. After the original demonstration of gating deficits in patients with schizophrenia, research naturally progressed to models in which pharmacological, developmental, and genetic manipulations were used to mimic the deficits in animals in order to develop predictive tools for drug discovery. Recently, this animal work revealed simple models of low versus high levels of PPI that could be readily translated into comparable human tests. Another example of translating an established animal paradigm into a human experimental tool is provided by the recent effort to develop the human counterpart to the rodent Behavioral Pattern Monitor, an established offshoot of the classical animal Open Field paradigm (Young et al., 2007a). This effort was initiated primarily because of the difficulties in assessing the differential predictive validity of animal models for schizophrenia versus bipolar mania. Patients with either diagnosis exhibit both PPI deficits and perseverative behaviors and both are commonly treated with antipsychotic drugs. One marked difference between the two conditions is that mania, in contrast to schizophrenia, is characterized by motoric hyperactivity. Indeed, hyper-activity is a cardinal symptom of mania that is usually measured only by observer-rated scales. Given the importance of hyperactivity in many psychiatric disorders in general and in bipolar mania in particular, it is surprising that experimental approaches to measure locomotor behavior empirically in humans have not been more abundant in the literature (Einat, 2006; Cryan and Slattery, 2007). Our understanding of motor activity in rodents is quite sophisticated, due to extensive studies of the neurobiology and neuropsychopharmacology of this behavior. Exploratory locomotor behavior in rodents involves complex patterns of behavior that are not characterized sufficiently only by quantifying the amount of behavior (Geyer et al., 1986; Drai and Golani, 2001; Eilam et al., 2006). Instead, measures that quantify the temporal, spatial, and dynamic organization of the subject’s movements and interactions with objects have proven to be valuable tools to differentiate the contributions of different neural transmitter systems on locomotor and exploratory behavior (Geyer and Paulus, 1996). Multivariate assessments of unconditioned locomotor behavior have shown that hyperactivity includes complex multifaceted behaviors, which has been effectively characterized using the rodent Behavioral Pattern Monitor. The Behavioral Pattern Monitor has been used to demonstrate differential effects of drugs on locomotor activity and exploratory behavior in rats and both normal and genetically modified mice (Risbrough et al., 2007). Of relevance to the suggested relevance of psychostimulant effects to both schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, these studies in rodents have enabled clear distinctions to be made between various psychostimulants that have different primary mechanisms of pharmacological action, such as the dopamine releaser amphetamine, the direct dopamine agonist apomorphine, the serotonin releaser 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine, and the NMDA antagonist phencyclidine (Geyer et al., 1986; Callaway et al., 1990). Clearly, detailed assessments of exploratory motor behavior can identify pharmacologically specific and behaviorally differentiated forms of hyperactivity in rodents that are not detectable by simple measures of the amount of movement.
As an extension of the extensive work on patterns of locomotor and exploratory behavior in rodents, we developed the human Behavioral Pattern Monitor in order to characterize motor activity in manic patients (Young et al., 2007a). Increased activity, object interactions, and altered locomotor patterns provide multidimensional phenotypes to model in the rodent Behavioral Pattern Monitor. This “reverse-translational” approach to modeling mania provides an opportunity to identify the neurobiology underlying bipolar mania and test novel antimanic agents. The current version of the human Behavioral Pattern Monitor has been established as a room on a locked inpatient psychiatry unit, equipped with a file cabinet, bookcase, desk, and no chair. Ten interesting and manipulable objects are placed around the room. Behavior is monitored by both a vest equipped with an accelerometer and physiological detectors and by a camera embedded in the ceiling that provides MPEG video files. The video images are then processed for spatial patterns of movements, as is done with video-tracking systems for rodents, and by visual scoring of object interactions and other observable behaviors. Preliminary findings using this approach have demonstrated that acutely ill bipolar manic patients exhibit dramatic increases in the amount of motor activity (Minassian et al., 2007) and in interactions with objects in the room (Kincaid et al., 2007). The normal habituation of activity exhibited by control subjects was diminished in manic patients (Minassian et al., 2007; Paulus et al., 2007). Additional analyses suggest that the spatial patterns of the movements of manic patients are similar in structure to those exhibited by rodents in which the dopamine transporter is disrupted either pharmacologically or genetically (Paulus et al., 2007; Young et al., 2007b). Groups of patients with other psychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, do not exhibit profiles of behavior in this paradigm that match that of bipolar manic patients. Thus, this simple objective measurement of specific behaviors in patients has the potential to discriminate between different diagnostic groups and parallel the profiles of activity associated with specific psychostimulant treatments in rodents. It is noteworthy that the profiles of behavior seen in bipolar mania and not in schizophrenia patients are similar to those produced by manipulations that induce hyperdopaminergia in rodents. Although rodent hyperactivity is often used to assess potential models related to schizophrenia, it does not appear that schizophrenia patients are motorically hyperactive as are bipolar manic patients. Hence, rodent models of hyperactivity may be generally more relevant to mania than schizophrenia.
CONCLUSIONS
Animal models have long been used to explore hypotheses regarding the neurobiological substrates of and treatments for psychiatric disorders. Early attempts to develop models that mimic the entirety of the diagnostic syndromes in psychiatry have evolved into more appropriate efforts to model specific symptoms. Such an approach reflects the facts that even in patients, clinical symptoms transcend diagnostic categories and the specific etiologies of psychiatric disorders are unknown. An animal model can only be identified adequately by specifying both the manipulation (drug, lesion, strain) used to induce abnormalities and the measure(s) used to characterize them. Many different pharmacological, lesion, and developmental manipulations have been combined with various measures of information processing to develop useful animal models that parallel human tests. The further development of cross-species models is critically important, given the new opportunities for the development and registration of specific treatments for the cognitive disorders of schizophrenia that are not ameliorated by available drugs. In moving beyond the focus on psychotic symptoms to the cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia, animal models that provide translational equivalents across species are receiving increased attention. Prepulse inhibition of startle, ERP measures of auditory gating, and Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB) measures of cognition are examples of measures that can be used in both rodents and humans and that are robustly altered in both psychiatric disorders and animals manipulated with appropriate drugs or lesions. The use of enriched populations of normal subjects, such as volunteers having low levels of gating, may enable proof of principle studies to be accomplished early in the drug development process using behavioral measures that mirror those used in the preclinical phases of the drug discovery process. Following such examples provided by the gating paradigms, recent efforts have begun to develop and evaluate the use of human equivalents of the established open field and behavioral pattern monitor paradigms used in rodents. In rodents, such multivariate assessments are able to differentiate among a variety of psychostimulants that have been used to assess various neurochemical hypotheses regarding the substrates of and treatments for disorders such as schizophrenia and bipolar mania. New parallel studies in which the exploratory motor behavior of humans is measured objectively may enable similar distinctions to be made among subgroups of psychiatric patients. If successful, this approach would further demonstrate the value of bi-directional development of objective behavioral tests that assess similar responses in both rodents and humans to facilitate the process of drug development and validation.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (MH071916), the National Institute on Drug Abuse (DA02925), and the Veteran’s Administration VISN 22 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center. The author thanks the many colleagues who have contributed to this work over the years.
References
- Braff DL, Geyer MA. Sensorimotor gating and schizophrenia: human and animal model studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47:181–188. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810140081011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braff DL, Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR. Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:234–258. doi: 10.1007/s002130100810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callaway CW, Wing LL, Geyer MA. Serotonin release contributes to the locomotor stimulant effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1990;254:456–464. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cilia J, Hatcher PD, Reavill C, Jones DNC. Long-term evaluation of isolation-rearing induced prepulse inhibition deficits in rats: an update. Psychopharmacology. 2005;180:57–62. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2139-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cryan JF, Slattery DA. Animal models of mood disorders: recent developments. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20:1–7. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280117733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Csomor PA, Stadler RR, Feldon J, Yee BK, Geyer MA, Vollenweider FX. Haloperidol differentially modulates prepulse inhibition and P50 suppression in healthy humans stratified for low and high gating levels. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:497–512. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drai D, Golani I. SEE: a tool for the visualization and analysis of rodent exploratory behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25:409–426. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00022-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eilam D, Zor R, Szechtman H, Hermesh H. Rituals, stereotypy and compulsive behavior in animals and humans. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30:456–471. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Einat H. Modelling facets of mania - new directions related to the notion of endophenotypes. J Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 2006;20:714–722. doi: 10.1177/0269881106060241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feifel D, Melendez G, Priebe K, Shilling PD. The effects of chronic administration of established and putative antipsychotics on natural prepulse inhibition deficits in Brattleboro rats. Behav Brain Res. 2007;181:278–86. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.04.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenton WS, Stover EL, Insel TR. Breaking the logjam in treatment development for cognition in schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology. 2003;169:365–366. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1564-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floresco S, Geyer MA, Gold LH, Grace A. Developing predictive animal models and establishing a preclinical trials network for assessing treatment effects on cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:888–894. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freedman R, Adler LE, Leonard S. Alternative phenotypes for the complex genetics of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45:551–558. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00321-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA. Are cross-species measures of sensorimotor gating useful for the discovery of procognitive cotreatments for schizophrenia? Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2006;8:9–16. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2006.8.1/mgeyer. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Ellenbroek BA. Animal behavior models of the mechanisms underlying antipsychotic atypicality. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2003;27:1071–1079. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2003.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Paulus M. Multivariate analyses of locomotor and investigatory behavior in rodents. In: Ossenkopp K, Kavaliers M, Sanberg PR, editors. Measuring Movement and Locomotion: From Invertebrates to Humans. Chapman & Hall; New York, NY: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Russo PV, Masten VL. Multivariate assessment of locomotor behavior: pharmacological and behavioral analyses. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1986;25:277–288. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(86)90266-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, Krebs-Thomson K, Braff DL, Swerdlow NR. Pharmacological studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia: a decade in review. Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:117–154. doi: 10.1007/s002130100811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer MA, McIlwain KL, Paylor R. Mouse genetic models for prepulse inhibition: an early review. Mol Psychiatry. 2002;7:1039–1053. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MF. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153:321–330. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.3.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MF, Kerns SR, Heaton RK. Longitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: implications for MATRICS. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagan JJ, Jones DN. Predicting drug efficacy for cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:830–853. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kincaid MJ, Minassian A, Ferguson EJ, Geyer MA, Paulus MP, Perry W. Exploratory behavior differentiates patients with acute psychotic mania from schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:139S. [Google Scholar]
- Kumari V, Soni W, Sharma T. Normalization of information processing deficits in schizophrenia with clozapine. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:1046–1051. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.7.1046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludewig K, Geyer MA, Vollenweider FX. Deficits in prepulse inhibition and habituation in never-medicated first-episode schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:121–128. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01925-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marder SR, Fenton W. Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia: NIMH MATRICS initiative to support the development of agents for improving cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:5–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin LF, Kem WR, Freedman R. Alpha-7 nicotinic receptor agonists: potential new candidates for the treatment of schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology. 2004;174:54–64. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1750-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meltzer HY, McGurk SR. The effects of clozapine, risperidone, and olanzapine on cognitive function in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1999;25:233–255. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minassian A, Geyer MA, Paulus MP, Ferguson EJ, Sharp R, Perry W. Quantification of motor hyperactivity in mania: the human behavioral pattern monitor. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:222S. [Google Scholar]
- Nuechterlein KH, Barch DM, Gold JM, Goldberg TE, Green MF, Heaton RK. Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olivier B, Leahy C, Mullen T, Paylor R, Groppi FE, Sarnyai Z, Brunner D. The DBA/2J strain and prepulse inhibition of startle: a model system to test antipsychotics? Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:284–290. doi: 10.1007/s002130100828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ouagazzal A-M, Jenck F, Moreau J-L. Drug-induced potentiation of prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle reflex in mice: a model for detecting antipsychotic activity. Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:273–283. doi: 10.1007/s002130100763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer AA, Dulawa SC, Mottiwala AA, Conti LH, Geyer MA, Printz MP. Prepulse startle deficit in the Brown-Norway rat. A potential genetic model. Behav Neurosci. 2000;114:374–388. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.2.374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus MP, Minassian A, Masten V, Feifel D, Geyer MA, Perry W. Human behavioral pattern monitor differentiates activity patterns of bipolar manic and attention deficit hyperactivity subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:227S. [Google Scholar]
- Perry W, Minassian A, Feifel D, Braff DL. Sensorimotor gating deficits in bipolar disorder patients with acute psychotic mania. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:418–424. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01184-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powell SB, Geyer MA. Developmental markers of psychiatric disorders as identified by sensorimotor gating. Neurotox Res. 2002;4:489–502. doi: 10.1080/10298420290030578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Risbrough VB, Masten VL, Caldwell S, Paulus MP, Low MJ, Geyer MA. Differential contributions of dopamine D(1), D(2), and D(3) receptors to MDMA-induced effects on locomotor behavior patterns in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:2349–2358. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA. Clozapine and haloperidol in an animal model of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1993;44:741–744. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(93)90193-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Braff DL. Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:194–215. doi: 10.1007/s002130100799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerdlow NR, Talledo J, Sutherland AN, Nagy D, Shoemaker JM. Antipsychotic effects on prepulse inhibition in normal ‘low gating’ humans and rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:2011–2021. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vollenweider FX, Barro M, Csomor PA, Feldon J. Clozapine enhances prepulse inhibition in healthy humans with low but not with high prepulse inhibition levels. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:597–603. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young J, Geyer MA the TURNS Preclinical Subcommittee. Cognitive task list and preclinical task survey. 2006 http://www.turns.ucla.edu/preclinical-TURNS-report-2006b.pdf.
- Young JW, Minassian A, Paulus MP, Geyer MA, Perry W. A reverse-translational approach to bipolar disorder: rodent and human studies in the behavioral pattern monitor. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007a;31:882–896. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young JW, Goey AKL, Masten VL, Sharp R, Paulus MP, Perry W, Geyer MA. Genetic and acute pharmacological animal models of bipolar disorder mania: mimicking recent insights of disordered movement and exploration. Biol Psychiatry. 2007b;61:38S. [Google Scholar]