Abstract
An increasing number of novel therapeutic agents are targeted at cannabinoid receptors. Drug development programmes of new cannabinoid drugs may be facilitated by the identification of useful biomarkers. This systemic literature review aims to assess the usefulness of direct biomarkers for the effects of cannabis and tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in healthy volunteers. One hundred and sixty-five useful articles were found that investigated the acute effects of cannabis or THC on the central nervous system (CNS) and heart rate in healthy volunteers. Three hundred and eighteen tests (or test variants) were grouped in test clusters and functional domains, to allow their evaluation as a useful biomarker and to study their dose–response effects. Cannabis/THC affected a wide range of CNS domains. In addition to heart rate, subjective effects were the most reliable biomarkers, showing significant responses to cannabis in almost all studies. Some CNS domains showed indications of depression at lower and stimulation at higher doses. Subjective effects and heart rate are currently the most reliable biomarkers to study the effect of cannabis. Cannabis affects most CNS domains, but too many different CNS tests are used to quantify the drug–response relationships reliably. Test standardization, particularly in motor and memory domains, may reveal additional biomarkers.
Keywords: biomarker, cannabis, THC, healthy volunteer
Introduction
The discovery of cannabinoid receptors and endocannabinoids has pointed to the physiological and possibly pathophysiological relevance of cannabinoids in humans. So far, two cannabis receptors (CB1 and CB2) have been identified with certainty. The CB1 receptors are predominantly situated in the brain and the CB2 receptors are predominantly present in the spleen and in haematopoietic cells. CB2 receptors seem also to be widely distributed in the brain, but their function is still not clear. The discovery of the endocannabinoid system has stimulated the development of synthetic cannabinoids, which have been used in preclinical research to investigate further the role of the endocannabinoid system in health and disease. However, the clinical development of cannabinoids as medicines is only just beginning. At present, most research in humans has been performed with tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), a CB1/CB2 agonist and the main psychoactive ingredient of cannabis. THC is a highly lipophilic compound that is rapidly absorbed and distributed to highly vascularized tissues, including the brain, where it causes its pleasurable effects. Smoking is the preferred route of cannabis use, with high bioavailability of the THC content that is not lost by combustion or vaporization. In humans, plasma THC concentration profiles are similar after smoking or intravenous administration, with prompt onset and steady decline. In contrast, slow absorption and limited and variable bioavailability are observed after oral administration.
Although a large number of studies have been performed with cannabis and THC in healthy volunteers, it is not clear which biomarkers are useful in early cannabinoid drug development, and how cannabis affects different central nervous system (CNS) functions. The effects of THC/cannabis can provide important tools during the early development of cannabinoid agonists and antagonists, if the effects can be qualified as valid biomarkers. A biomarker is a characteristic that is measured and evaluated as an indicator of normal biological processes, pathogenic processes, or pharmacological responses to a therapeutic intervention [1]. A validated biomarker in early Phase I studies that provides useful information on the potential therapeutic effects of an investigational drug could support the drug development programme of the new compound. In general, a useful biomarker for activity of a drug class should meet the following criteria: (i) a clear, consistent response across studies (from different research groups) and drugs from the same class; (ii) a clear response of the biomarker to therapeutic doses; (iii) a dose (concentration)–response relationship; and (iv) a plausible relationship between the biomarker, the pharmacology of the drug class and/or the pathogenesis of the therapeutic area. Previously, these criteria have been used to evaluate the literature for the usefulness of biomarkers for the effects in healthy volunteers of antipsychotic drugs [2], benzodiazepines [3], selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors [4] and 3,4-methylene-dioxy-methamphetamine (ecstasy) [5]. In the current review, the effects of cannabis and THC in healthy volunteers were systematically evaluated using the same methodology.
Methods
Structured literature evaluation
A literature search was performed up to 15 November 2007 using MedLine, Web of Science and Embase. The following keywords were used: marijuana, marihuana, cannabis, THC, tetrahydrocannabinol and delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol. The searches were limited to healthy adults and papers in English. The resulting studies were subject to several selection criteria.
This review aimed to assess the usefulness of direct CNS biomarkers and heart rate for studies of cannabinoids in healthy volunteers. Reviews, studies in experimental animals or patients, and studies of interactions of cannabis use with personality features, behavioural characteristics, metabolic variations, other drugs, pain models or environmental factors (including secondary or subgroup analyses) were excluded from this review.
Studies with <10 subjects were not included. Study participants were divided into non-users and users. No distinction was made according to the levels of previous or current usage, which ranged from occasional to chronic frequent use and was rarely documented in detail. Frequent and infrequent users were grouped as users. The review was restricted to the effects of acute cannabis exposure. Hence, abstinence effects, ‘morning after effects’ (including sleep effects after dosing on the preceding day), long-term effects in chronic users or effects of repeated dosing were not incorporated in this review.
The study characteristics and each individual test result of all articles that complied with the criteria were put into a database (Microsoft Excel) (Appendix S1). The following items were recorded: number of subjects, sex (male; female), age, past cannabis use (users; non-users; unknown), abstinence period (yes; no; unknown), blinding (double blind; single blind; open; unknown), design (crossover; partial crossover; parallel; unknown), drug name (cannabis, including hashish and marijuana); THC(/dronabinol)), dose, route of administration (oral; intrapulmonary; intravenous; unknown), THC equivalence (<7 mg; 7–18 mg; >18 mg), test name, test effect, test cluster and functional domain. Most studies used different tests on different doses of cannabis, which were all regarded as independent measures of the cannabis effect. Thus, the total number of evaluated tests (cases) was a product of the number of articles, drugs, doses and tests (including secondary outcomes).
Individual test results
Based on previous reviews, it was anticipated that in most cases no consistent quantitative results could be recorded for individual tests, because of the large diversity of methods, parameters and treatments. Therefore, the ability of a test to show a statistically significant difference from placebo or baseline was scored as + (improvement/increase), = (no significant effect) or – (impairment/decrease). Subjective assessments with a desirable effect (e.g. increase of a high scale) were scored as an improvement/increase, and unwanted effects (e.g. increase of sedation) as an impairment/decrease.
Different parameters of a single test were always grouped together if they provided information on the same cluster. Many single tasks provided different outcome parameters, which sometimes showed apparently opposite responses. If these opposite responses were part of the same cluster, two items were scored for the same test: e.g. one (+) and one (−). More frequently, one of the parameters that improved was from a different functional cluster than the one that deteriorated. In these cases, both items were scored separately on different clusters. In the table an asterisk was added to the item that was considered a secondary parameter from a test of a different primary function.
Some studies explicitly reported the use of several different tests in the methods section, without presentation of the results for any apparent reason. In these cases, it was assumed that these tests had not shown any significant effects. In some studies with different drug doses, overall significances were reported for drug effects, without (post hoc) quantifications of the statistical significance levels for each individual dose. In these cases, efforts were made to estimate the individual dose effects from graphs or tables provided in the article. If this was impossible, only the effect of the highest dose was assumed to be significant (in case of overall statistical significance) and lower doses were considered nonsignificant.
Grouping of individual test results
Because of apparent lack of standardization between the studies even for the same tests, a structured procedure was adopted as described previously [2–5] in order to obtain an overview. This approach allowed the preservation of individual study data in early stages, followed by a progressive condensation of results into logical test clusters and functional domains. For the subjective assessments, most subjective scales can, for example, be grouped under scores of feeling high, craving, alertness, general drug effect, etc. A compendium of neuropsychological tests from Strauss et al. [6] was primarily consulted to group functional tests into clusters of related tests or test variants. If necessary, the compendium of Lezak was consulted [7]. Sometimes, these compendia did not mention the specific test. In these cases, the author's classification was followed or, if necessary, the test was looked up in other literature and classified by consensus. A single, more complicated test can sometimes measure several aspects (e.g. of memory, executive function, subjective effects, etc.) and can therefore provide information on different clusters. Examples are Babcock Story Recall Test, Buschke Selective Reminding Test, Digit Recall Test, Ratings of Narrative Quality.
Tests and clusters were grouped further into domains that represent higher aggregates of integration of subjective, neuropsychological, neuroendocrine, neurophysiological or autonomic functions. For each test (cluster), the compendia and other literature were used to determine which function was principally assessed by the test. Neuropsychological domains consisted of executive functions, memory, attention, motor functions, language and perception. Some tests provided different parameters with information on more than one functional domain. The results of the effects of a single test on different domains were scored separately, and the secondary effects were marked.
Results from tests that were used only occasionally or tests used only by a single research group could not be generalized. Therefore, these were not analysed individually, but grouped with other comparable tests. This step started with the grouping of tests that could be regarded as variants from a basic form (e.g. individual scores that are also part of more comprehensive tools such as Profiles of Mood States, Addiction Research Center Inventory (ARCI) or Bond & Lader Visual Analogue Scales (VAS) [8]). Subscales of such inventories were grouped if they fell in the same cluster. Within such clusters, all scales showing a significant effect were grouped, whereas all scales showing no effect were grouped separately. In this way, scales within the same cluster that showed mixed results were scored equivocally. Comprehensive scoring instruments like Waskow's Drug Effect Questionnaire can be subdivided into different subjective clusters (e.g. drug effect, high effect, etc.), but these subscales were not always reported separately. In these cases, the results were presented as part of the overall scale drug effect cluster. In a few articles, a couple of composite scores of different CNS functions were presented that could not be grouped according to the clusters or domains used in this review. These tests were not included in the analysis.
All subdivisions of the tests and effect scores were initially performed by two of the authors (E.M. and A.E.I.) and subsequently checked and discussed by the other authors (L.Z., A.E.I. and J.M.A.v.G.).
Dose–effect relationships
The chance that a test will detect a difference from placebo is expected to increase with dose. For each test that was used ≥10 times and for all clusters, potential dose–response relationships were determined. Dose-related increases or decreases of the average percentages of tests or clusters were reported without formal statistical analyses. Since the review yielded no immediately quantitative test effects, dose relationships were represented by the proportions of statistically significant results for a given test or cluster. Similarly, since THC doses were not reported uniformly, cannabis/THC dosages were pooled into ‘lower’, ‘medium’ and ‘higher’ dosages. The ‘lower’ dose was chosen to be a dose <7 mg (roughly corresponding to half a cigarette), the ‘medium’ dose lay between 7 and 18 mg (approximately corresponding to one to one-and-a-half cigarette), and the ‘higher’ doses were all dosages >18 mg (comparable with one-and-a-half cigarettes or more) [9–11].
Cigarette smoking was the predominant form of administration. In many articles the exact THC content of a cigarette was mentioned. However, some articles mentioned the THC contents in percentage without the weight of the cigarette. In these cases a cigarette weight of 700 mg was assumed since most cigarettes weigh 500–900 mg. In other articles the number of puffs taken was documented. In these instances the dose was calculated as eight puffs corresponding to one marijuana cigarette [11]. Some studies provided weight-adjusted doses, without specifying the (average) body weight. In these cases, the 70 kg adult general population body weight was used to calculate the average administered dose.
To be able to compare the test results obtained for oral and intravenous administration with the results obtained for smoking, all doses were normalized to smoking. After smoking, roughly 50% of the THC contents of a cigarette is delivered into the smoke [12] and another 50% of the inhaled smoke is exhaled again [13]. In practice, smoking a cannabis cigarette of 10 mg causes 50% loss due to heating, which leaves 5 mg. Next, half the inhaled 5 mg is exhaled again. Ultimately, 2.5 mg or 25% of the 10-mg cigarette is delivered to the systemic circulation. Bioavailability after oral administration was assumed to be around 10% [14, 15]. Consequently, 25 mg THC would have to be ingested to get the same 2.5 mg systemic exposure as after smoking a 10-mg cigarette. This means that oral doses were divided by 2.5 to calculate the equivalent intrapulmonary THC doses. The THC plasma concentrations after smoking a 19-mg marijuana cigarette are equal to intravenous administration of 5 mg THC [16]. Therefore, all intravenous dosages were multiplied by four for dose normalization. In this way, all routes of administration could be compared.
Results
Study design
The literature search yielded 165 different studies on cannabis and THC that met all criteria, published between 1966 and 15 November 2007. The numbers of participants ranged from 10 to 161, where 115 studies (70%) included 10–20 subjects and six studies included >75 subjects (9%). Ages ranged from 18 to 59 years, but the vast majority were young adults aged 18–35 years. In 57% of studies only healthy men were included, and 2% of studies included only women. Thirty-three percent of studies included men and women, whereas the sex of the subjects was not mentioned in 8%.
Most studies (80%) included subjects that were familiar with the effects of cannabis. In contrast, non-users were included in only 3%. Eleven percent of studies reported inclusion of both cannabis users and non-users. Previous cannabis use was not mentioned in 6% of studies. A small majority of studies (53%) described an abstinence period or the use of a THC drug screen. Four percent of studies reported the lack of an abstinence period, whereas 44% did not mention this topic.
Fifty-seven percent of the reviewed studies had a double-blinded design; 26% were single-blinded; 7% had an open design and for 10% the blinding was unknown. In addition, a small majority of the studies had a crossover design (60%), 3% had a partial crossover design, 33% had a parallel design and 4% of studies did not mention the study design.
Study drug and dosing
Cannabis is also known as marijuana, and dronabinol is an analogue of THC, the predominant psychoactive component of cannabis. Cannabis was used in 63% of studies and THC in 34%. Intrapulmonary administration was the preferred route of administration in 71% of studies. Oral administration of the drug was mentioned in 25% of studies and intravenous administration was used in only 3%. Three percent of studies did not describe which form of cannabis was used and 1% did not mention the route of administration. In these cases it could be inferred from the doses and the design that cannabis was smoked.
Tests, clusters and domains
In total, 318 different tests were used. Only a minority of tests were used frequently enough to allow individual analysis. The majority of tests (196 tests, 61.6%) were used only once, and only heart rate (0.3%) was used >50 times (in 92 articles) (Table 1). VAS scale high/stoned was studied in 30 articles, whereas the subjective effect rating scale high/stoned/euphoria was assessed in 28 articles (Table 1). Taken together, the subjective high phenomenon was measured in >50 (35.2%) articles as well (Table 1). The Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) or variants such as the Symbol Digit Substitution Test were the most frequently used neuropsychological tests (22 times) (Table 1). The ARCI was used in 18 articles (Table 1).
Table 1.
Test name | Frequency |
---|---|
Heart rate | 92 |
VAS (scales high/stoned) | 30 |
Subjective Effect Rating Scale (scales high/stoned/euphoria) | 28 |
Digit Symbol Substitution Test | 22 |
Addiction Research Center Inventory (scale drug effect) | 18 |
POMS (scales anger/friendliness/hostility) | 18 |
POMS (scales confusion/clear headedness/energy/confused-bewildered/vigour/stimulation) | 18 |
VAS (scales sedation/stimulation-alertness/attentiveness/interest/clear headed/confused/energetic/ sluggish/sleepiness/drowsy/concentration/ forgetful) | 18 |
POMS (scales anxiety-tension/tension/arousal) | 17 |
Subjective Effect Rating Scale (scales intoxication/drunk/drug effect/placebo-THC/feel marijuana effect) | 16 |
POMS (scales anxiety-tension/anxiety) | 15 |
POMS (scales composure/depression/depression-dejection/elation/(positive)mood) | 15 |
POMS (scale fatigue) | 14 |
Potency Rating Scale | 14 |
VAS [scales (good/bad) drug effect/feel drug/intoxication/drunk/comparison to usual smoke] | 14 |
Time Estimation Task | 13 |
VAS (scale anxiety/anxious/panic) | 13 |
Pleasantness Rating Scale | 12 |
VAS (scales content/down/mood/withdrawn/sociability feelings) | 11 |
VAS (scales feelings of tranquillity/calm/relaxed/mellow/arousal) | 11 |
VAS (scales hungry/hunger) | 11 |
POMS, Profiles of Mood States; VAS, visual analogue scale.
Although many different tests and test variants were used to evaluate the effects of cannabis, most actually measured a limited number of core features. Therefore, tests were grouped further into clusters and subsequently in domains. Table 2a–d is a progressive condensation of all reported tests, from test to cluster to domain, and includes the overall calculated significant drug effects on each cluster (impairment/decrease, no change or improvement/increase).
Table 2.
Effects (%) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Domain cluster | Tests | – | = | + | Reference (frequency; n) |
(a)(Neuro)Endocrine | |||||
Cortisol | Cortisol | 0 | 0 | 100 | [20] (n = 1) |
Prolactin | Prolactin | 0 | 100 | 0 | [20] (n = 1) |
Autonomic | |||||
Heart rate | Heart rate | 1 | 7 | 92 | [17, 21–111] (n = 92) |
Pupil size | Pupil size | 24 | 59 | 18 | [21, 22, 29, 44, 68, 112, 113] (n = 7) |
Temperature | Temperature | 12 | 88 | 0 | [21, 68, 101, 105] (n = 4) |
Neurophysiological | |||||
EEG | EEG | 29 | 43 | 29 | [17, 43, 114] (n = 3) |
EEG alpha | EEG alpha | 17 | 22 | 61 | [17, 22, 84, 85, 88, 93, 115–117] (n = 9) |
EEG beta | EEG beta | 59 | 35 | 6 | [17, 22, 84, 88, 93, 115, 117] (n = 7) |
EEG delta | EEG delta | 0 | 100 | 0 | [17, 22, 84, 115, 117] (n = 5) |
EEG theta | EEG theta | 6 | 88 | 6 | [17, 22, 84, 93, 115, 117] (n = 6) |
Evoked potential | Auditory evoked potentials, contingent negative variation (CNV), evoked potentials, visually evoked potentials | 20 | 45 | 35 | [22, 43, 93, 115, 118–122] (n = 9) |
Eye movements – nystagmus | Electronystagmography recordings, electro-oculographic recordings | 0 | 100 | 0 | [69, 123] (n = 2) |
Eye movements – pursuit | Electro-oculographic recordings, Eye Performance System (EPS-100), eye-point of regard system, tracking a pendulum | 38 | 63 | 0 | [21, 69, 123, 124] (n = 4) |
Eye movements – saccadic | Electro-oculographic recordings, eye-point of regard system, saccadic eye movement | 0 | 80 | 20 | [123–126] (n = 4) |
(b) Memory | |||||
Auditory/verbal memory: delayed recall | Babcock Story Recall Test, Buschke Selective Reminding Test, colour-number matching task, digit recall task, free recall of story test, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, memory assessment of POMS scores, orienting word task, prose recall task, Randt Memory Battery, recognition task, semantic memory retrieval task, text learning task, verbal recognition & recall task, word list, word recall task | 53 | 47 | 0 | [20, 23, 51–53, 55, 64, 66, 91, 94, 107, 127–136] (n = 21) |
Auditory/verbal memory: delayed recognition | Cued recall of story test, delayed story recognition task, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, name and address recognition task, verbal recognition & recall task, word list, word recognition task | 27 | 73 | 0 | [20, 23, 52, 53, 55, 56, 94, 107, 131, 135] (n = 10) |
Auditory/verbal memory: immediate recall | Babcock Story Recall Test, Benton Sentence Repetition Task, Buschke Selective Reminding Test, colour-number matching task, digit recall task, free recall of story test, free recall test, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, list learning task, orienting word task, prose recall task, Randt Memory Battery, seashore tonal memory task, syllable list learning task, text learning task, word anagram solution task, word list, word recall task | 60 | 40 | 0 | [20, 23, 25, 30, 32, 50–53, 55, 57, 64, 66, 91, 107, 127–130, 132, 135–140] (n = 26) |
Implicit memory | Common facts recall task, detailed recall task, perceptual priming task, remote memory task, word list | 0 | 100 | 0 | [64, 128, 131, 141] (n = 4) |
Learning | Artificial conditioned speech connections, word presentation memory task, driving task*, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test*, intelligence structure test, memory for designs test*, method of artificial conditioned speech connections, paired associate learning task, Randt Memory Battery, repeated acquisition task, tactual performance test, word list* | 38 | 62 | 0 | [20, 25, 28, 45, 54, 66, 75, 91, 93, 129, 132, 138, 139, 142–144] (n = 16) |
Visual/spatial memory: delayed recognition | Benton Visual Retention Test | 0 | 100 | 0 | [28] (n = 1) |
Visual/spatial memory: immediate recall | Memory for designs test, Peterson Visual Memory Test, picture recall test | 100 | 0 | 0 | [32, 54, 138] (n = 3) |
Executive | |||||
Driving | Driving task, flight simulator task | 62 | 38 | 0 | [24, 45, 79, 97, 145–149] (n = 9) |
inhibition | Central and peripheral light flashes task*, word presentation memory task*, decision making task, delay discounting task, digit recall test with signal detection task*, divided attention task (DAT)*, go/no-go task, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test*, memory for designs test*, monetary stimulation task, Randt Memory Battery*, ratings of narrative quality, stop task, Stroop Colour and Word Test, temporally controlled operant task, thematic apperception test (TAT), verbal fluency task*, word list learning*, word recall task* | 52 | 48 | 0 | [20, 23, 25, 30, 34, 41, 52–54, 66, 85, 86, 93, 107, 137, 140, 150–154] (n = 21) |
Judgement | Flexibility and closure test, Iowa Gambling Task, scores of willingness to drive | 25 | 75 | 0 | [105, 110, 146] (n = 3) |
Planning | Goal-directed serial alternation task, thematic apperception test (TAT) | 86 | 14 | 0 | [153, 155] (n = 2) |
Reasoning/association | Alternate use task, analogy task, association IV, associative processing test, Baddeley reasoning task, categorization task, concept formation task, contingent categorization task, free and constrained associations test, Halstead Category Test, hidden word test, Iowa Test of Educational Development*, letter series test, logical reasoning task, numerical reasoning task, object description test*, object-match task, picture arrangement test, production and recall of free associations test, ratings of narrative quality, thematic apperception test (TAT), water-jar test, word grouping test | 37 | 63 | 0 | [21, 30, 33, 85, 128–131, 134, 135, 138, 151–154, 156–160] (n = 20) |
Set shifting | Delayed auditory feedback device (DAF), object-match task*, trail making test B-A* | 20 | 80 | 0 | [37, 100, 131, 137, 161] (n = 5) |
Time estimation | Time estimation task | 18 | 33 | 48 | [23, 29, 30, 42, 50, 64, 82, 83, 88, 102, 152, 155, 162] (n = 13) |
Working memory | Alphabet task, boggle word construction test, word presentation memory task, conceptual clustering memory test, cued recall of story test, delayed auditory feedback device (DAF), digit recall task, digit span (backward), matching to sample task, mental calculation task, picture recognition test, rapid information processing task, running memory span, serial addition/subtraction task, spatial N-Back task, Sternberg Memory Scanning Task, story recognition task, word anagram solution task, word list*, visual continuous word recognition task | 40 | 60 | 0 | [12, 20, 21, 23, 26, 30, 33, 37, 52, 55, 58, 66, 70, 85, 93, 100, 107, 116, 121, 127, 128, 131, 138, 139, 155, 157, 161–163] (n = 30) |
Motor | |||||
Motor control | Card sorting task, choice reaction time task*, driving task*, finger tapping test, finger tremor test, foot tapping test, Klove Grooved Steadiness Task, Klove Static Steadiness Task, manual dexterity test, Minnesota Rate of Manipulation – block turning, pegboard test, tapping task, toe tapping test | 36 | 64 | 0 | [50, 97, 130, 138, 149, 158, 159, 161, 164, 165] (n = 12) |
Postural stability | Body sway, equitest, finger tapping test*, foot tapping test*, Klove Grooved Steadiness Task, Klove Static Steadiness Task, standing steadiness task, wobble board | 54 | 46 | 0 | [24, 26, 30, 31, 70, 97, 100, 103, 158, 159, 161] (n = 12) |
Visuo-motor control | Bender-Gestalt Test, circular lights task, driving task*, efficiency test system, Gibson spiral maze, groove pegboard task, hand maze task, hand steadiness task, horizontal groove task, Klove Maze Coordination Task, one-hole test, pursuit meter/motor/rotor task, rod and frame deviation task, spiral rotor task, star tracing task, tracking task, trail making test A, vertical groove task, Vienna Determination Apparatus (VDA) | 55 | 45 | 0 | [21, 26, 28, 29, 31, 37, 40, 88, 94, 100, 103, 116, 128, 130, 137, 138, 149, 152, 158, 159, 161, 162, 164–167] (n = 25) |
(c) Attention | |||||
Divided attention | Choice reaction time task*, dichotic listening task, digit recall test with signal detection task, distraction task, divided attention task (DAT), Landolt C-Rings Test, matching to sample task*, trail making test B | 37 | 59 | 4 | [30, 33, 36, 41, 58, 70, 91, 111, 124, 130–132, 137, 161] (n = 14) |
DSST-like | Barrage de Signe, digit symbol substitution test (DSST), digit symbol substitution with memory test | 42 | 58 | 0 | [12, 23, 29, 30, 31, 38, 46, 50, 58, 70–73, 83, 88, 91, 109, 126, 131, 132, 139, 152, 162] (n = 23) |
Flicker discrimination | Critical flicker fusion test, critical stimulus duration task | 33 | 56 | 11 | [89, 97, 129, 130, 168, 169] (n = 6) |
Focused/selective attention | 3 × 3 block matrix task, Arbeit und Konzentrationstest Geräte, arithmetic task, auditory reaction time task*, continuous performance task*, D2 attention test, digit span (forward), digit recall test with signal detection task, double target digit cancellation task, number facility test, paced auditory serial addition test, P-deletion test, single target digit cancellation task, Stroop Colour and Word Test* | 35 | 65 | 0 | [20, 23, 26, 41, 58, 66, 70, 85, 105, 111, 126, 128, 131, 136, 137, 159, 162] (n = 17) |
Reaction time | Alerting task, auditory reaction time task, central and peripheral light flashes task, choice reaction time task, complex reaction time task, contingent categorization task*, contingent negative variation (CNV)*, dichotic listening task*, discrimination reaction time task, driving task*, Iowa Gambling Task*, letter matching task, matching to sample task*, perceptual speed task, peripheral visual detection task*, rapid information processing task, reaction time task, simple auditory reaction time task, simple reaction time task, simple visual reaction time task, spatial N-back task*, Stroop Color and Word Test*, visual reaction time task, word recognition task* | 48 | 51 | 1 | [12, 26, 31, 33, 34, 40, 66, 84, 85, 91, 93, 94, 97, 103, 110, 111, 118, 121, 126, 128–132, 139, 145, 158, 159, 161, 170–172] (n = 32) |
Sustained attention (vigilance) | Continuous performance task, Mackworth Clock-Vigilance Task, pursuit meter/motor/rotor task*, visual search task | 14 | 86 | 0 | [12, 20, 29, 31, 35, 83, 124, 161] (n = 8) |
Language | |||||
Comprehension | Text learning task* | 100 | 0 | 0 | [129] (n = 1) |
Production | Cloze Method, controlled oral word association test (COWAT), object description test, spontaneous speech, thematic apperception test (TAT), verbal fluency task, word recall task* | 23 | 69 | 8 | [20, 39, 66, 128, 140, 151, 153, 160] (n = 8) |
Semantics | Iowa Test of Educational Development*, orienting word task* | 0 | 100 | 0 | [51, 129] (n = 2) |
Perception | |||||
Auditory perception | Auditory rhythm test, auditory threshold test | 17 | 83 | 0 | [130, 173] (n = 2) |
Tactile perception | Tactual performance test, vibratory sense appreciation test | 33 | 50 | 17 | [116, 130, 138] (n = 3) |
Visual/spatial perception | Archimedian spiral after effect, binocular depth inversion test, block design test, clock faces task, closure speed test, dot tests, driving task*, glare recovery task, group embedded figures test, hidden figures task, mannequin task, peripheral visual detection task, size-weight illusion test, visual acuity task, visual autokinetic motion task, visual brightness test, visual information processing task, visual recognition task | 27 | 73 | 0 | [21, 27, 40, 54, 97, 113, 130, 131, 135, 152, 157, 165, 172–176] (n = 17) |
(d) Subjective experience | |||||
Scale aggression | Brief psychiatric rating scale (scale hostility), Clyde Mood Scale (Scales Friendly/Aggressive), Gottschalk-Gleser Content Analysis (Scales Social Alienation/Hostility), Jackson Personality Research Form (Scale Autonomy), Jackson Personality Research Form (Scale Dominance), POMS (scales anger/friendliness/hostility), primary affect scale (PAS) (scale anger), ratings of narrative quality, thematic apperception test (TAT), VAS (scales friendly/social) | 27 | 68 | 5 | [23, 30, 38, 49, 50, 60–63, 67, 71, 91, 96–98, 105, 108, 133, 139, 151, 153, 154, 158, 165, 175, 177] (n = 26) |
Scale alertness | Addiction Research Center Inventory (ARCI) (scale stimulated), brief psychiatric rating scale (scale activation), Clyde Mood Scale (scales sleepy/clear thinking), comprehensive psychiatric rating scale (AMDP) (scale alertness), drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scales sluggish/ stuffy feeling/thinking clearer/concentration), feeling scale of Janke (composite scale vital), medical questionnaire (scale impaired concentration), observer rated signs, POMS scales confusion/clear headedness/energy/confused-bewildered/vigour/stimulation, scale stimulated, subjective effect rating scale (scales concentration impairment/interest), VAS scales sedation/stimulation alertness/attentiveness/interest/clear headed/confused/ energetic/sluggish/ sleepiness/drowsy/concentration/forgetful | 39 | 50 | 11 | [21, 23, 24, 28, 30, 37, 38, 40, 46, 49, 50, 58, 60–63, 67, 70–73, 96, 97, 105, 108, 126, 128, 130–133, 139, 146, 149, 150, 158, 175, 178] (n = 38) |
Scale anxiety | Ditman's DWM Scale (6–8), drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scale anxiety), Gottschalk-Gleser Content Analysis Scales (scale anxiety), POMS scales anxiety-tension/anxiety, primary affect scale (PAS) (scale fear), state trait anxiety inventory, Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale (MAS), thematic apperception test (TAT), VAS (scale anxiety/anxious/panic) | 29 | 67 | 5 | [20, 23, 24, 30, 33, 38, 49, 50, 58–62, 70–73, 96, 97, 101, 108, 133, 139, 151, 153, 158, 165, 179, 180] (n = 29) |
Scale calmness | Ditman's DWM Scale (1–20), drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scales relaxation/tension/excited), feeling relaxed, feeling scale of Janke (scale passive), POMS (scales anxiety-tension/tension/arousal), primary affect scale (PAS) (scale arousal), VAS (scales feelings of tranquillity/calm/relaxed/mellow/arousal) | 23 | 53 | 24 | [20, 23, 24, 30, 33, 38, 49, 50, 58–60, 63, 67, 71, 78, 91, 96–98, 106, 108, 128, 130–133, 146, 149, 150, 158, 165, 178, 179] (n = 33) |
Scale craving | Drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scales like drug/want more/take drug again), end-of-session questionnaire (scales dislike/like a lot), pleasantness rating scale (scale craving), subjective effect rating scale (scales feel like smoking/like drug effect/want more/price willing to pay), VAS (scales like drug/like effect/desire) | 50 | 18 | 32 | [17, 21, 23, 25, 30, 32, 38, 49, 52–57, 70–73, 78, 91, 107, 128, 132] (n = 23) |
Scale dizziness | Clyde Mood Scale (scale dizzy), Ditman's DWM Scale (1–20), drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scale dizzy), medical questionnaire (scales disturbed equilibrium/faintness), subjective effect rating scale (scale dizziness) | 58 | 42 | 0 | [105, 128, 130, 149] (n = 4) |
Scale drug effect | Addiction Research Center Inventory (ARCI) (scale drug effect), Ditman's DWM scale (6–8), drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scales good/bad/strong/feel effect), end-of-session questionnaire (scales like/feel/strength), estimation of received drug, feeling of intoxication, numeric scale cannabis, observer rated signs, potency rating scale, psychological subjective effect ratings (scale drug effect), scale intoxication, subjective effect rating scale (scales intoxication/drunk/drug effect/placebo-THC/feel marijuana effect), subjective psychological effects ratings, VAS (scales drug effect/feel drug/intoxication/drunk/good drug effect/bad drug effect/comparison to usual smoke) | 7 | 26 | 67 | [21, 23–26, 29–33, 37–41, 44, 46, 49–57, 63, 65–67, 70–73, 79, 81, 82, 88, 91, 93–102, 107–110, 128, 130–132, 139, 145, 146, 150, 152, 161, 165, 167, 178, 179] (n = 67) |
Scale fatigue | Drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scale fatigue), Karolonsika Sleepiness Rating (scale tiredness), POMS (scale fatigue), VAS (scale tired) | 33 | 67 | 0 | [20, 23, 30, 38, 49, 50, 62, 63, 67, 71, 93, 96, 97, 106, 130, 133, 139, 158] (n = 18) |
Scale high | Addiction Research Center Inventory (ARCI) (scale high), drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scales high/euphoria), feeling high, subjective effect rating scale (scales high/stoned/euphoria), VAS (scales high/stoned) | 0 | 4 | 96 | [17, 20, 21, 23, 24, 27, 30, 33, 38–40, 42, 44, 47–50, 58–61, 64, 65, 70–74, 76–79, 83–89, 91–93, 96, 97, 104, 106, 110, 111, 116–119, 125, 128, 130–132, 134, 135, 139, 144, 146, 154, 156, 157, 161, 173, 178, 181, 182] (n = 70) |
Scale mood | Brief psychiatric rating scale (scales anergia-depression/anergia), Clyde Mood Scale (scale unhappy), comprehensive psychiatric rating scale (AMDP) (scales sexual desire/euphoria), Ditman's DWM Scale (1–20), Ditman's DWM Scale (6–8), drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scales well-being/dysphoria/feel free/feel serious), Jackson Personality Research Form (scale exhibitionism), observer rated signs, pleasantness rating scale (scale mood), POMS (scales composure/depression/depression-dejection/ elation/mood/positive mood), positive and negative symptom scale (PANSS) (scale mood), primary affect scale (PAS) (scale depression), primary affect scale (PAS) (scale happiness), scale depression, subjective effect rating scale (scales enjoyability/pleasantness), thematic apperception test (TAT), VAS (scales content/down/mood/withdrawn/sociability feelings) | 23 | 61 | 17 | [20, 23, 28, 30, 37, 38, 40, 46, 49–51, 62, 63, 67, 70, 71, 83, 84, 88, 91, 96, 97, 105, 114, 126, 128, 130–133, 139, 146, 149, 150, 153, 158, 165, 175, 177] (n = 39) |
Scale performance | Drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scales psychomotor activity/control/control/accelerated-improved cognition), instructor's performance rating, mental status examination (scale intellectual efficiency), subjective effect rating scale (scales difficulty/driving ability/impaired/motivation/memory impairment/performance), subjective effect rating scale (scales difficulty/driving ability/impaired/motivation/memory impairment/performance), subjective performance rating, subjective performance rating, VAS (scale impaired) | 65 | 24 | 12 | [21, 24, 40, 58, 70, 79, 93, 97, 116, 128, 130, 145, 146, 161] (n = 14) |
Scale psychotomimetic | Brief psychiatric rating scale (scale thought disorder), clinician administered dissociative symptoms scale (CADSS) (scale perceptual alternations), comprehensive psychiatric rating scale (AMDP) (scale thought disorder), depersonalization inventory, Ditman's DWM scale (1–20), Ditman's DWM scale (6–8), drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scales weird/silly/increased sensitivity/perceptual and sensorysharpness/timesense/dreamlike/ giddy/floating/unreal perception/detachment/ enhanced awareness/slow speech/fast thoughts), mental status examination (scales illusions/hallucinations/paranoid/delusional), positive and negative symptom scale (PANSS) (scale psychotomimetic), ratings of narrative quality, temporal disintegration inventory, vividness of imageries | 80 | 20 | 0 | [20, 28, 33, 46, 59, 60, 61, 88, 101, 108, 114, 116, 130, 144, 149, 151, 175, 179] (n = 18) |
Scale satiety | Drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scale hunger), feeling hungry, food intake, VAS (scales hungry/hunger) | 52 | 48 | 0 | [30, 38, 46, 49, 50, 70–73, 80, 127, 131, 132, 139] (n = 14) |
Scale sensory | Comprehensive psychiatric rating scale (AMDP) (scale disturbance of sensory perception), medical questionnaire (scales heat evaluations/cold sensation), scale taste/harshness/draw, subjective effect rating scale (scale enhanced sensations), VAS (scale loud noise) | 9 | 64 | 27 | [24, 28, 93, 97, 131, 149] (n = 6) |
Scale sleep | Sleep questionnaire | 0 | 100 | 0 | [49] (n = 1) |
Scale symptoms | Comprehensive psychiatric rating scale (AMDP) (scales headache/ nausea), Cornell Medical Index (CMI), Ditman's DWM scale (1–20), drug effect questionnaire (DEQ) (scales sick feeling/symptoms/heart pounding/dry throat), medical questionnaire (scales tremor/headache/ dysphagia), observer rated signs, somatic sensation scale, subjective effect rating scale (scales heart pounding/dry mouth), VAS (scale nauseous/symptoms) | 52 | 46 | 2 | [28, 33, 37, 46, 59, 60, 70, 71, 80, 85, 87, 91, 100, 108, 128, 130–132, 149, 161, 179] (n = 21) |
+, improvement/increase; =, no significant effect; –, impairment/decrease.
The item was a secondary parameter from a test of another primary cluster.
Table 2a–d shows that most drug-sensitive clusters demonstrate consistent functional impairment, and some an enhancement (heart rate, scale high). A few clusters show both impairments and improvements (e.g. time estimation, EEG alpha and evoked potential measurements, and scales for calmness, craving, mood and performance). Only a few frequently-used (>10 times) test clusters showed significant responses to THC/cannabis in >80% of studies, notably heart rate (n = 85/92), scale high (n = 67/70) and scale psychotomimetic (n = 14/18). Most other clusters reported significant drug effects in only about 30–50% of studies (Table 2a–d). All tests that were used five times or more showed a significant THC effect in at least one case, except for EEG delta, which never responded in any study.
Dose–response relationships
Tests and clusters that were used in >10 articles were inspected for potential dose–response relationships (Table 3). Heart rate showed a statistically significant increase in 78% of measurements in the THC equivalence dose group <7 mg, which increased to 99 and 98% after the use of 7–18 and >18 mg THC, respectively. The subjective high feeling included many different scoring methods, varying from observer rating scales to individual VAS scores, either in isolation or as part of multidimensional inventories (Table 2d). Despite this variability, the cluster scale high showed very consistent effects for all dose groups. The lowest dose group of <7 mg THC already showed a response of 94%, and the middle (7–18 mg) and highest dose group (>18 mg) scored close to 100%. The related subjective cluster scale psychotomimetic also showed consistent deterioration (i.e. an increase in psychotomimetic scores) with THC/cannabis of 76–83% without a clear dose–response relationship. A small increase with dose (from 56 to 78%) was observed for the cluster scale drug effect.
Table 3.
<7 mg | 7–18 mg | >18 mg | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Domain cluster | – | = | + | – | = | + | – | = | + |
Autonomic | |||||||||
Heart rate | 0 | 22 | 78 | 0 | 1 | 99 | 2 | 0 | 98 |
Motor | |||||||||
Motor control | 71 | 29 | 0 | 50 | 50 | 0 | 27 | 73 | 0 |
Visuomotor control | 68 | 32 | 0 | 64 | 36 | 0 | 19 | 81 | 0 |
Memory | |||||||||
Auditory/verbal memory delayed recall | 23 | 77 | 0 | 63 | 38 | 0 | 78 | 22 | 0 |
Auditory/verbal immediate recall | 50 | 50 | 0 | 75 | 25 | 0 | 45 | 55 | 0 |
Attention | |||||||||
DSST-like | 31 | 69 | 0 | 50 | 50 | 0 | 47 | 53 | 0 |
Focused selective attention | 57 | 43 | 0 | 33 | 67 | 0 | 14 | 86 | 0 |
Reaction time | 46 | 54 | 0 | 52 | 45 | 3 | 47 | 53 | 0 |
Executive | |||||||||
Inhibition | 50 | 50 | 0 | 52 | 48 | 0 | 57 | 43 | 0 |
Working memory | 52 | 48 | 0 | 42 | 58 | 0 | 9 | 91 | 0 |
Reasoning/association | 33 | 67 | 0 | 37 | 63 | 0 | 43 | 57 | 0 |
Subjective experiences | |||||||||
Scale aggression | 20 | 80 | 0 | 24 | 71 | 5 | 40 | 50 | 10 |
Scale alertness | 43 | 50 | 7 | 43 | 50 | 7 | 35 | 51 | 14 |
Scale anxiety | 11 | 83 | 6 | 35 | 62 | 4 | 33 | 63 | 4 |
Scale calmness | 10 | 60 | 30 | 31 | 50 | 19 | 26 | 48 | 26 |
Scale craving | 53 | 22 | 25 | 61 | 11 | 28 | 20 | 20 | 60 |
Scale drug effect | 12 | 32 | 56 | 4 | 18 | 78 | 3 | 21 | 76 |
Scale high | 0 | 6 | 94 | 0 | 0 | 100 | 0 | 5 | 95 |
Scale mood | 29 | 61 | 10 | 17 | 66 | 17 | 19 | 59 | 22 |
Scale psychotomimetic | 83 | 17 | 0 | 81 | 19 | 0 | 76 | 24 | 0 |
Scale symptoms | 64 | 36 | 0 | 58 | 37 | 5 | 41 | 59 | 0 |
DSST, digit symbol substitution test.
The relationship between memory and doses of THC/cannabis was more complex. Impairment increased with dose for auditory/verbal delayed recall (from 23% with the lowest doses to 78% with the highest dose range), but the effects were less clear for immediate recall (Table 3). Auditory/verbal delayed recognition also deteriorated with dose (from 17 to 50%), but this was assessed in only 11 studies. Working memory impairment, on the other hand, seemed to decrease with dose, from 52% impairments in the lowest dose group to 9% in the highest (Table 3). Other clusters that also appeared to show an inverse dose–response association were the DSST-like cluster, focused selective attention and tests of motor and visuomotor control (Table 3). The proportion of significant effects of THC/cannabis within the cluster scale aggression increased slightly with dose (from 20 to 40%). No clear dose–response relationships were observed for inhibition, reasoning/association or reaction time, or for most subjective scales (Table 3). For studies with different doses, we scored significance for the highest dose only, if significance was merely reported for the overall group effect, without allowing an estimate of the individual dose effects from graphs or tables. Although in such cases we could have artificially induced a dose–response relationship, this was observed in only 3% of all test scores.
Discussion
This review aimed to evaluate systematically the usefulness of tests for the effects of cannabis and THC in healthy volunteers. It should be noted that cannabis cigarettes contain a mixture of psychoactive compounds, which in combination may contribute differently to the psychological and physical effects of cannabis compared with single THC administration. However, since THC is the main psychoactive ingredient of cannabis, in this review studies with cannabis and THC were used. The results were comparable to those of similar reviews of biomarkers of different CNS-active drugs in healthy volunteers [2–5]. A striking number of 318 different tests or test variants were described, of which 61.6% were used only once. Grouping of tests in clusters and domains was required to evaluate the general usefulness of functional measurements, but this inevitably led to a loss of information. Even clustering tests with the same name and/or description could have bypassed differences among research groups or tests variants. In addition, this review investigated biomarkers for the effects of cannabis and THC in healthy volunteers, i.e. often with relatively small subject numbers; 70% of the studies had ≤20 participants. It is possible that some tests will be useful biomarkers in patient studies or studies with large numbers of subjects, or if their value is demonstrated in more studies. The observed variability in test results may have been enhanced by differences in prior cannabis use (non-users, occasional and frequent users). In this review these differences were not taken into consideration, although most participants were occasional cannabis users and only 3% had not used cannabis before. Chronic and occasional cannabis users show similar drug effects, although chronic users generally require higher doses and thus seem to be less sensitive [17]. A small majority of articles mentioned an abstinence period, but it is likely that this was also included in many other studies, without being reported. The neglect of prior use intensity or abstinence duration may have confounded the detection of dose–response relationships, which was only roughly possible in any case because of the many different doses and administration forms.
Useful cannabinoid biomarkers
The effects of cannabis were observed on all clusters and all domains and in almost all individual tests, which might be due to the wide distribution of cannabinoid receptors in the brain [18]. An increase in heart rate was the most consistent result (Tables 1 and 2a), and almost all studies with heart rate measurements showed statistically significant effects. This was expected, since heart rate shows a sharp increase and rapid decline after intrapulmonary THC administration that is clearly concentration related and already considerable at low THC levels [9, 19]. Feeling high has previously also been shown to be closely related to THC plasma concentrations [19]. The high phenomenon was measured in many different ways, but despite this variability almost all studies showed statistically significant subjective drug effects. The predicted and highly consistent effects of THC/cannabis on the most clearly concentration-related effects (heart rate and feeling high [9, 19]) in this review also support the methodological approach that was adopted, to integrate the widely variable study designs, drug forms and doses, and tests reported in the literature. Feeling high seems to be the most sensitive CNS biomarker for the effects of cannabis, irrespective of how it is measured. The scales psychotomimetic and drug effect are not quite as sensitive, but they address subjective changes that are less specific for THC/cannabis. This is clearly illustrated by the only negative scores on the drug effect cluster, which are all due to the reductions on the benzedrine scale (BG scale) of the ARCI, which is considered as a measure of subjective stimulation. Most other clusters show low to medium sensitivity for the effects of THC/cannabis, with significant drug effects in roughly 30–60% of cases (Table 2a–d). These findings are similar to other drug classes, which show very comparable sensitivities of neurophysiological, neuropsychological and subjective tests of 30–60% with benzodiazepines [3] and neuroleptics [2]. In these reviews, saccadic peak velocity was highly sensitive to benzodiazepines in 100% [3], and prolactin release to neuroleptics in 96% [2]. These parameters were not particularly responsive to THC/cannabis in the current review, where heart rate and subjective high feeling scored 92–96%. This illustrates the differential effect profiles of different pharmacological groups, even among drug classes that are generally considered to be ‘CNS depressant’. Such variability should be considered when methods are selected to study the CNS effects of neuropsychiatric agents. Furthermore, tests that showed a medium chance on an effect (approximately 60%) should also be critically considered.
Dose–response relationships
A useful biomarker should show a dose–response relationship starting at a low therapeutic dose. Consideration of dose–response relationships is also essential to compare the effects of THC (at different doses and administration routes) across studies. In this review, doses could be grouped only roughly, and effects could be scored only as either statistically significant or not. Moreover, hardly any test was measured frequently and quantified consistently enough for a meaningful analysis of dose–response associations. Perhaps due to these limitations, dose–response relationships were found for only a few clusters (Table 3). THC doses were categorized in a low (<7 mg, half a cannabis cigarette), medium (7–18 mg, one to one-and-a-half cigarette) and high (>18 mg, one-and-a-half cigarettes or more) dose. This pragmatic division was not based on well-established relations between doses, plasma concentrations and CNS effects. Nonetheless, it led to roughly similar numbers of tests at the three different dose levels (623–852 in each dose group), and thus reflects practical dose selection in the literature. However, this practice could be based on the habit of subjects to smoke enough cannabis to elicit a desirable subjective state that does not cause unpleasant effects. It is not illogical to assume that this is reflected in the dose of one cigarette, and that a ‘standard dose’ is near the maximum tolerated dose for most subjects (still pleasant and devoid of intolerable adverse effects). In this review, lower doses (<7 mg) were used in only about 30% of cases, and even this dose range caused subjective high feeling in 94% of cases. In a recent pharmacokinetic–pharmacodynamic (PK–PD) study, heart rate, VAS high and alertness, and postural stability were already sensitive to levels as low as 2 mg of THC administration by inhalation, and PK–PD effect relationships showed that near-maximum effects are reached with THC doses corresponding to roughly 10 mg of cannabis [9, 19], which corresponds to one or two cannabis cigarettes.
The memory effects of cannabis showed some dose–response relationships, but this differed for the various types of memory tests. Impairments increased with dose for auditory/verbal delayed recall and to a lesser extent for immediate recall and auditory/verbal delayed recognition (Table 3). Working memory (which included immediate recognition) on the other hand seemed to improve (i.e. normalize) with dose, with 52% impairments in the lowest dose group to 9% in the highest (Table 3). The clusters of focused selective attention and of motor and visuomotor control also appeared to show an inverse dose–response association (Table 3). All these functions are highly influenced by attention and concentration [6]. Decreases in subjective alertness were noted in 43% with the lowest doses and 35% with the highest. This may have been accompanied by some agitation. At the same time, dose-related increases in (subjective) aggression (which increased with dose from 20 to 40%) and anxiety (from 11 to 33%) were observed. All this suggests that lower doses of THC/cannabis generally cause pleasant effects of relaxation and reduced attention, whereas with high doses CNS depression is partly overcome by more stimulatory effects.
These results suggest that most CNS measurements are sensitive to the effects of THC/cannabis. Some parameters were very sensitive to THC/cannabis. For such biomarkers, most doses studied in the literature may have been too high to show clear dose–response relationships. The proportion of significant effects did not increase much at the highest dose range compared with the medium range, although this does not exclude a dose-related increase in the size of the effects per se. Except for some subjective effects (feeling high, psychotomimetic feelings, drug effects) and heart rate, most tests did not show consistent effects even at quite high THC/cannabis doses. It could be argued that less sensitive CNS tests would have shown effects at even higher doses, well above 18 mg. For most subjects, particularly for inexperienced cannabis users, two or more cannabis cigarettes would cause an overdose, with questionable pharmacological and functional selectivity and an unacceptable adverse event profile. Hence, sensitive biomarkers at low to medium doses are needed to characterize the concentration–effect relationships of CB1/CB2 agonists. At present, the literature indicates that the choice is limited to heart rate and subjective effects.
Summary
Biomarkers are useful tools to study drug effects, since they can provide information on the potential pharmacological effects of the investigational drug in early-phase drug development. However, the number of tests and test variants that is used in studies of THC and cannabis seems excessively large. This abundance thwarts good assessment of the physiological, neuropsychological and subjective effects of this drug class, and there is dire need for test standardization in these areas. In general, the doses studied in the literature reflect the patterns of recreational use, and are often too high to determine accurately pharmacological dose–response relationships. Cannabis/THC has an effect on a wide range of CNS domains. At lower doses, THC/cannabis seems to be relaxant and to reduce attention, which is accompanied by impaired performance on other CNS tests that require motivation and active participation. At high doses, the drug seems to be more stimulatory. Subjective effects are the most reliable biomarkers to study the effects of cannabis, in addition to heart rate increases that reflect peripheral cannabinoid activation. This review indicates that these parameters are useful biomarkers that can be used in future studies to investigate the effects of THC/cannabis and other cannabinoid agonists on the CNS.
Acknowledgments
This review was performed on behalf of the Biomarker Working Group of the German Association for Applied Human Pharmacology AGAH.
Competing interests
None declared.
Supporting Information
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article:
Appendix S1
Database of study characteristics and each individual study result of all articles that complied with the inclusion criteria
Please note: Wiley-Blackwell are not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting materials supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing material) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.
REFERENCES
- 1.Colburn WA. Biomarkers in drug discovery and development: from target identification through drug marketing. J Clin Pharmacol. 2003;43:329–41. doi: 10.1177/0091270003252480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.de Visser SJ, van der Post J, Pieters MS, Cohen AF, van Gerven JM. Biomarkers for the effects of antipsychotic drugs in healthy volunteers. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2001;51:119–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2125.2001.01308.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.de Visser SJ, van der Post JP, de Waal PP, Cornet F, Cohen AF, van Gerven JM. Biomarkers for the effect of benzodiazepines in healthy volunteers. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2003;55:39–50. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2125.2002.t01-10-01714.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Dumont GJ, de Visser SJ, Cohen AF, van Gerven JM. Biomarkers for the effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) in healthy subjects. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2005;59:495–510. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2125.2005.02342.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dumont GJ, Verkes RJ. A review of acute effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine in healthy volunteers. J Psychopharmacol. 2006;20:176–87. doi: 10.1177/0269881106063271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Spreen O, Stretton CD. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests; Administration, Norms and Commentary. 2nd edn. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc; 1998. ISBN 0-19-510019-0. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neurosphychological Assesment. 4th edn. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bond A, Lader M. The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. Br J Med Psychol. 1974;47:211–8. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Zuurman L, Roy C, Schoemaker RC, Hazekamp A, den Hartigh J, Bender JCME, Verpoorte R, Pinquier JL, Cohen AF, van Gerven JMA. Effect of intrapulmonary THC administration in humans. J Psychopharmacol. 2008;22:707–16. doi: 10.1177/0269881108089581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Heishman SJ, Arasteh K, Stitzer ML. Comparative effects of alcohol and marijuana on mood, memory, and performance. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;58:93–101. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(96)00456-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Gritz ER. Patterns of puffing in cigarette smokers. NIDA Res Monogr. 1978;20:221–35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Casswell S. Cannabis intoxication: effects of monetary incentive on performance, a controlled investigation of behavioural tolerance in moderate users of cannabis. Percept Mot Skills. 1975;41:423–34. doi: 10.2466/pms.1975.41.2.423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hazekamp A, Ruhaak R, Zuurman L, van Gerven J, Verpoorte R. Evaluation of a vaporizing device (Volcano) for the pulmonary administration of tetrahydrocannabinol. J Pharm Sci. 2006;95:1308–17. doi: 10.1002/jps.20574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Grotenhermen F. Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of cannabinoids. Clin Pharmacokinet. 2003;42:327–60. doi: 10.2165/00003088-200342040-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Iversen L. The Science of Marijuana. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ohlsson A, Lindgren JE, Wahlen A, Agurell S, Hollister LE, Gillespie HK. Plasma delta-9 tetrahydrocannabinol concentrations and clinical effects after oral and intravenous administration and smoking. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1980;28:409–16. doi: 10.1038/clpt.1980.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Fink M. Effects of acute and chronic inhalation of hashish, marijuana, and delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol on brain electrical activity in man: evidence for tissue tolerance. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1976;282:387–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1976.tb49911.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Herkenham M, Lynn AB, Little MD, Johnson MR, Melvin LS, de Costa BR, Rice KC. Cannabinoid receptor localization in brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1990;87:1932–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.5.1932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Strougo A, Zuurman L, Roy C, Pinquier JL, Cohen AF, van Gerven JMA, Schoemaker RC. Modelling of the concentration–effect relationship of THC on central nervous system parameters and heart rate. insight into its mechanisms of action and a tool for clinical research and development of cannabinoids. J Psychopharmacol. 2008;22:717–26. doi: 10.1177/0269881108089870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.D'Souza DC, Perry E, MacDougall L, Ammerman Y, Cooper T, Wu YT, Braley G, Gueorguieva R, Krystal JH. The psychotomimetic effects of intravenous delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in healthy individuals: implications for psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1558–72. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Fant RV, Heishman SJ, Bunker EB, Pickworth WB. Acute and residual effects of marijuana in humans. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1998;60:777–84. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(97)00386-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Stefanis C. Biological aspects of cannabis use. NIDA Res Monogr. 1978;20:149–78. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.McDonald J, Schleifer L, Richards JB, de Wit H. Effects of THC on behavioral measures of impulsivity in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:1356–65. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Liguori A, Gatto CP, Jarrett DB. Separate and combined effects of marijuana and alcohol on mood, equilibrium and simulated driving. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;163:399–405. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1124-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Miller L, Cornett T, Mcfarland D. Marijuana: an analysis of storage and retrieval deficits in memory with the technique of restricted remiding. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1978;8:327–32. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(78)90065-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Belgrave BE, Bird KD, Chesher GB, Jackson DM, Lubbe KE, Starmer GA, Teo RK. The effect of (-) trans-delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol, alone and in combination with ethanol, on human performance. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1979;62:53–60. doi: 10.1007/BF00426035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Adams AJ, Brown B, Flom MC, Jones RT, Jampolsky A. Alcohol and marijuana effects on static visual acuity. Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1975;52:729–35. doi: 10.1097/00006324-197511000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kurzthaler I, Hummer M, Miller C, Sperner-Unterweger B, Gunther V, Wechdorn H, Battista HJ, Fleischhacker WW. Effect of cannabis use on cognitive functions and driving ability. J Clin Psychiatry. 1999;60:395–9. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v60n0609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Weil AT, Zinberg NE, Nelsen JM. Clinical and psychological effects of marihuana in man. Science. 1968;162:1234–42. doi: 10.1126/science.162.3859.1234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Chait LD, Perry JL. Acute and residual effects of alcohol and marijuana, alone and in combination, on mood and performance. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1994;115:340–9. doi: 10.1007/BF02245075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wilson WH, Ellinwood EH, Mathew RJ, Johnson K. Effects of marijuana on performance of a computerized cognitive-neuromotor test battery. Psychiatry Res. 1994;51:115–25. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(94)90031-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Miller LL, McFarland DJ, Cornett TL, Brightwell DR, Wikler A. Marijuana: effects on free recall and subjective organization of pictures and words. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1977;55:257–62. doi: 10.1007/BF00497857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Peeke SC, Jones RT, Stone GC. Effects of practice on marijuana-induced changes in reaction time. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1976;48:159–63. doi: 10.1007/BF00423255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.MacAvoy MG, Marks DF. Divided attention performance of cannabis users and non-users following cannabis and alcohol. Psychopharmacologia. 1975;44:147–52. doi: 10.1007/BF00421001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Sharma S, Moskowitz H. Effects of two levels of attention demand on vigilance performance under marihuana. Percept Mot Skills. 1974;38:967–70. doi: 10.2466/pms.1974.38.3.967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Casswell S, Marks D. Cannabis induced impairment of performance of a divided attention task. Nature. 1973;241:60–1. doi: 10.1038/241060b0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Manno JE, Kiplinger GF, Scholz N, Forney RB. The influence of alcohol and marihuana on motor and mental performance. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1971;12:202–11. doi: 10.1002/cpt1971122part1202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wachtel SR, ElSohly MA, Ross SA, Ambre J, de Wit H. Comparison of the subjective effects of Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol and marijuana in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;161:331–9. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1033-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Higgins ST, Stitzer ML. Acute marijuana effects on social conversation. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1986;89:234–8. doi: 10.1007/BF00310635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Schaefer CF, Gunn CG, Dubowski KM. Dose-related heart-rate, perceptual, and decisional changes in man following marihuana smoking. Percept Mot Skills. 1977;44:3–16. doi: 10.2466/pms.1977.44.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Moskowitz H. Effects of marihuana on auditory signal detection. Psychopharmacologia. 1974;40:137–45. doi: 10.1007/BF00421363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Tinklenberg JR, Roth WT, Kopell BS. Marijuana and ethanol: differential effects on time perception, heart rate, and subjective response. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1976;49:275–9. doi: 10.1007/BF00426830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Low MD, Klonoff H, Marcus A. The neurophysiological basis of the marijuana experience. Can Med Assoc J. 1973;108:157–65. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Brown B, Adams AJ, Haegerstrom-Portnoy G, Jones RT, Flom MC. Pupil size after use of marijuana and alcohol. Am J Ophthalmol. 1977;83:350–4. doi: 10.1016/0002-9394(77)90732-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Klonoff H. Marijuana and driving in real-life situations. Science. 1974;186:317–24. doi: 10.1126/science.186.4161.317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Jones RT. Marihuana-induced ‘high’: influence of expectation, setting and previous drug experience. Pharmacol Rev. 1971;23:359–69. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Roth WT, Tinkleinberg JR, Kopell BS, Hollister LE. Continuous electrocardiographic monitoring during marihuana intoxication. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1973;14:533–40. doi: 10.1002/cpt1973144part1533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Gong H, Jr, Tashkin DP, Simmons MS, Calvarese B, Shapiro BJ. Acute and subacute bronchial effects of oral cannabinoids. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1984;35:26–32. doi: 10.1038/clpt.1984.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Chait LD, Zacny JP. Reinforcing and subjective effects of oral delta 9-THC and smoked marijuana in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1992;107:255–62. doi: 10.1007/BF02245145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Chait LD, Fischman MW, Schuster CR. Hangover effects the morning after marijuana smoking. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1985;15:229–38. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(85)90002-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Belmore SM, Miller LL. Levels of processing and acute effects of marijuana on memory. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1980;13:199–203. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(80)90073-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Miller LL, Cornett TL, Wikler A. Marijuana: effects on pulse rate, subjective estimates of intoxication and multiple measures of memory. Life Sci. 1979;25:1325–30. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(79)90398-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Miller LL, Cornett TL. Marijuana: dose effects on pulse rate, subjective estimates of intoxication, free recall and recognition memory. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1978;9:573–7. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(78)90205-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Miller L, Cornett T, Nallan G. Marijuana: effect on nonverbal free recall as a function of field dependence. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1978;58:297–301. doi: 10.1007/BF00427394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Miller LL, Cornett TL, Brightwell DR, McFarland DJ, Drew WG, Wikler A. Marijuana: effects on storage and retrieval of prose material. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1977;51:311–6. doi: 10.1007/BF00431642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Miller L, Cornett T, Drew W, Mcfarland D, Brightwell D, Wikler A. Marijuana: dose–response effects on pulse rate, subjective estimates of potency, pleasantness, and recognition memory. Pharmacology. 1977;15:268–75. doi: 10.1159/000136698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Miller L, Cornett T, Brightwell D, Mcfarland D, Drew WG, Wikler A. Marijuana and memory impairment: the effect of retrieval cues on free recall. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1976;5:639–43. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(76)90305-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Heishman SJ, Stitzer ML, Yingling JE. Effects of tetrahydrocannabinol content on marijuana smoking behavior, subjective reports, and performance. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1989;34:173–9. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90369-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Mathew RJ, Wilson WH, Coleman RE, Turkington TG, DeGrado TR. Marijuana intoxication and brain activation in marijuana smokers. Life Sci. 1997;60:2075–89. doi: 10.1016/s0024-3205(97)00195-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Mathew RJ, Wilson WH, Humphreys D, Lowe JV, Weithe KE. Depersonalization after marijuana smoking. Biol Psychiatry. 1993;33:431–41. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(93)90171-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Mathew RJ, Wilson WH. Acute changes in cerebral blood flow after smoking marijuana. Life Sci. 1993;52:757–67. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(93)90239-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Mathew RJ, Wilson WH, Tant SR. Acute changes in cerebral blood flow associated with marijuana smoking. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1989;79:118–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1989.tb08579.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Lex BW, Mendelson JH, Bavli S, Harvey K, Mello NK. Effects of acute marijuana smoking on pulse rate and mood states in women. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1984;84:178–87. doi: 10.1007/BF00427443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Wetzel CD, Janowsky DS, Clopton PL. Remote memory during marijuana intoxication. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1982;76:278–81. doi: 10.1007/BF00432561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Huestis MA, Gorelick DA, Heishman SJ, Preston KL, Nelson RA, Moolchan ET, Frank RA. Blockade of effects of smoked marijuana by the CB1-selective cannabinoid receptor antagonist SR141716. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:322–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.4.322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Hooker WD, Jones RT. Increased susceptibility to memory intrusions and the Stroop interference effect during acute marijuana intoxication. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1987;91:20–4. doi: 10.1007/BF00690920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Benedikt RA, Cristofaro P, Mendelson JH, Mello NK. Effects of acute marijuana smoking in post-menopausal women. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1986;90:14–7. doi: 10.1007/BF00172864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Liakos A, Boulougouris JC, Stefanis C. Psychophysiologic effects of acute cannabis smoking in long-term users. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1976;282:375–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1976.tb49910.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Spector M. Acute vestibular effects of marijuana. J Clin Pharmacol. 1973;13:214–7. doi: 10.1002/j.1552-4604.1973.tb00210.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Greenwald MK, Stitzer ML. Antinociceptive, subjective and behavioral effects of smoked marijuana in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2000;59:261–75. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(99)00128-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Wachtel SR, de Wit H. Naltrexone does not block the subjective effects of oral Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2000;59:251–60. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(99)00127-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Kirk JM, de Wit H. Responses to oral delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol in frequent and infrequent marijuana users. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1999;63:137–42. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(98)00264-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Kirk JM, Doty P, de Wit H. Effects of expectancies on subjective responses to oral delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1998;59:287–93. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(97)00414-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Cami J, Guerra D, Ugena B, Segura J, de la Torre R. Effect of subject expectancy on the THC intoxication and disposition from smoked hashish cigarettes. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1991;40:115–9. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90330-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Capriotti RM, Foltin RW, Brady JV, Fischman MW. Effects of marijuana on the task-elicited physiological response. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1988;21:183–7. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(88)90068-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Lindgren JE, Ohlsson A, Agurell S, Hollister L, Gillespie H. Clinical effects and plasma levels of delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (delta 9-THC) in heavy and light users of cannabis. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1981;74:208–12. doi: 10.1007/BF00427095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Pihl RO, Shea D. Voluntary heart rate changes and the marihuana ‘high. J Clin Psychol. 1978;34:982–7. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(197810)34:4<982::aid-jclp2270340433>3.0.co;2-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Pihl RO, Segal Z, Shea D. Negative expectancy as a mediating variable in marihuana intoxication. J Clin Psychol. 1978;34:978–82. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(197810)34:4<978::aid-jclp2270340432>3.0.co;2-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Janowsky DS, Meacham MP, Blaine JD, Schoor M, Bozzetti LP. Marijuana effects on simulated flying ability. Am J Psychiatry. 1976;133:384–8. doi: 10.1176/ajp.133.4.384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Zimmer BD, Bickel P, Dittrich A. Changes of simple somatic parameters by delta-9-trans-tetrahydrocannabinol (delta-9-THC) in a double-blind-study. Short communication. Arzneimittelforschung. 1976;26:1614–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Clark SC, Greene C, Karr GW, MacCannell KL, Milstein SL. Cardiovascular effects of marihuana in man. Can J Physiol Pharmacol. 1974;52:706–19. doi: 10.1139/y74-090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Carlini EA, Karniol IG, Renault PF, Schuster CR. Effects of marihuana in laboratory animals and in man. Br J Pharmacol. 1974;50:299–309. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.1974.tb08576.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Vachon L, Sulkowski A, Rich E. Marihuana effects on learning, attention and time estimation. Psychopharmacologia. 1974;39:1–11. doi: 10.1007/BF00421453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Volavka J, Crown P, Dornbush R, Feldstein S, Fink M. EEG, heart rate and mood change (‘high’) after cannabis. Psychopharmacologia. 1973;32:11–25. doi: 10.1007/BF00421704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Galanter M, Weingartner H, Vaughan TB, Roth WT, Wyatt RJ. 9-Transtetrahydrocannabinol and natural marihuana. A controlled comparison. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1973;28:278–81. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1973.01750320106016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Cappell H, Webster CD, Herring BS, Ginsberg R. Alcohol and marihuana: a comparison of effects on a temporally controlled operant in humans. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1972;182:195–203. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Galanter M, Wyatt RJ, Lemberger L, Weingartner H, Vaughan TB, Roth WT. Effects on humans of 9-tetrahydrocannabinol administered by smoking. Science. 1972;176:934–6. doi: 10.1126/science.176.4037.934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Jones RT, Stone GC. Psychological studies of marijuana and alcohol in man. Psychopharmacologia. 1970;18:108–17. doi: 10.1007/BF00402390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Braff DL, Silverton L, Saccuzzo DP, Janowsky DS. Impaired speed of visual information processing in marijuana intoxication. Am J Psychiatry. 1981;138:613–7. doi: 10.1176/ajp.138.5.613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Chait LD. Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol content and human marijuana self-administration. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1989;98:51–5. doi: 10.1007/BF00442005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Hart CL, Haney M, Vosburg SK, Comer SD, Foltin RW. Reinforcing effects of oral Delta9-THC in male marijuana smokers in a laboratory choice procedure. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;181:237–43. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2234-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Hollister LE, Gillespie HK, Ohlsson A, Lindgren JE, Wahlen A, Agurell S. Do plasma concentrations of delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol reflect the degree of intoxication? J Clin Pharmacol. 1981;21:171S–177S. doi: 10.1002/j.1552-4604.1981.tb02593.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Ilan AB, Smith ME, Gevins A. Effects of marijuana on neurophysiological signals of working and episodic memory. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;176:214–22. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1868-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Cappell H, Kuchar E, Webster CD. Some correlates of marihuana self-administration in man: a study of titration of intake as a function of drug potency. Psychopharmacologia. 1973;29:177–84. doi: 10.1007/BF00414031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Menkes DB, Howard RC, Spears GF, Cairns ER. Salivary THC following cannabis smoking correlates with subjective intoxication and heart rate. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1991;103:277–9. doi: 10.1007/BF02244217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Chait LD, Evans SM, Grant KA, Kamien JB, Johanson CE, Schuster CR. Discriminative stimulus and subjective effects of smoked marijuana in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1988;94:206–12. doi: 10.1007/BF00176846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Liguori A, Gatto CP, Robinson JH. Effects of marijuana on equilibrium, psychomotor performance, and simulated driving. Behav Pharmacol. 1998;9:599–609. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199811000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Mathew RJ, Wilson WH, Humphreys DF, Lowe JV, Wiethe KE. Regional cerebral blood flow after marijuana smoking. J. Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1992;12:750–8. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.1992.106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Hollister LE, Gillespie H. Interactions in man of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol. II. Cannabinol and cannabidiol. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1975;18:80–3. doi: 10.1002/cpt197518180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Kiplinger GF, Manno JE, Rodda BE, Forney RB. Dose–response analysis of the effects of tetrahydrocannabinol in man. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1971;12:650–7. doi: 10.1002/cpt1971124650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Dittrich A, Woggon B. Subjective syndromes, physiological changes and after-effects of the acute 9-tetrahydrocannabinol intoxication. Two double-blind studies in volunteers. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr. 1972;216:301–9. doi: 10.1007/BF00342650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Karniol IG, Carlini EA. Comparative studies in man and in laboratory animals on 8- and 9-trans-tetrahydrocannabinol. Pharmacology. 1973;9:115–26. doi: 10.1159/000136375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Bird KD, Boleyn T, Chesher GB, Jackson DM, Starmer GA, Teo RK. Intercannabinoid and cannabinoid–ethanol interactions on human performance. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1980;71:181–8. doi: 10.1007/BF00434409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Perez-Reyes M, Timmons MC, Davis KH, Wall EM. A comparison of the pharmacological activity in man of intravenously administered delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol, cannabinol, and cannabidiol. Experientia. 1973;29:1368–9. doi: 10.1007/BF01922823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Hollister LE, Richards RK, Gillespie HK. Comparison of tetrahydrocannabinol and synhexyl in man. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1968;9:783–91. doi: 10.1002/cpt196896783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Flom MC, Adams AJ, Jones RT. Marijuana smoking and reduced pressure in human eyes: drug action or epiphenomenon? Invest Ophthalmol. 1975;14:52–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Miller LL, Mcfarland D, Cornett TL, Brightwell D. Marijuana and memory impairment: effect on free recall and recognition memory. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1977;7:99–103. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(77)90191-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Mathew RJ, Wilson WH, Melges FT. Temporal disintegration and its psychosocial and physiological correlates: changes in the experience of time after marijuana smoking. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 1992;4:235–45. [Google Scholar]
- 109.Jones RT. Tetrahydrocannabinol and marijuana induced social high or effects of mind on marijuana. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1971;191:155. [Google Scholar]
- 110.Vadhan NP, Hart CL, van Gorp WG, Gunderson EW, Haney M, Foltin RW. Acute effects of smoked marijuana on decision making, as assessed by a modified gambling task, in experienced marijuana users. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29:357–64. doi: 10.1080/13803390600693615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.O'Leary DS, Block RI, Koeppel JA, Schultz SK, Magnotta VA, Ponto LB, Watkins GL, Hichwa RD. Effects of smoking marijuana on focal attention and brain blood flow. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2007;22:135–48. doi: 10.1002/hup.832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Hepler RS, Frank IM, Ungerleider JT. Pupillary constriction after marijuana smoking. Am J Ophthalmol. 1972;74:1185–90. doi: 10.1016/0002-9394(72)90741-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Adams AJ, Brown B, Haegerstrom-Portnoy G, Flom MC, Jones RT. Marijuana, alcohol, and combined drug effects on the time course of glare recovery. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1978;56:81–6. doi: 10.1007/BF00571413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Koukkou M, Lehmann D. Correlations between cannabis-induced psychopathology and EEG before and after drug ingestion. Pharmakopsychiatr Neuropsychopharmakol. 1978;11:220–7. doi: 10.1055/s-0028-1094581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Roth WT, Galanter M, Weingartner H, Vaughan TB, Wyatt RJ. Marijuana and synthetic 9-trans-tetrahydrocannabinol: some effects on the auditory evoked response and background EEG in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 1973;6:221–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Rodin EA, Domino EF, Porzak JP. The marihuana-induced ‘social high’. Neurological and electroencephalographic concomitants. JAMA. 1970;213:1300–2. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Lukas SE, Mendelson JH, Benedikt R. Electroencephalographic correlates of marihuana-induced euphoria. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1995;37:131–40. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(94)01067-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Braden W, Stillman RC, Wyatt RJ. Effects of marihuana on contingent negative variation and reaction time. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1974;31:537–41. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1974.01760160077016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Roth WT, Tinklenberg JR, Kopell BS. Ethanol and marihuana effects on event-related potentials in a memory retrieval paradigm. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1977;42:381–8. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(77)90174-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Kopell BS, Tinklenberg JR, Hollister LE. Contingent negative variation amplitudes. Marihuana and alcohol. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1972;27:809–11. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1972.01750300071012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Leweke M, Kampmann C, Radwan M, Dietrich DE, Johannes S, Emrich HM, Munte TF. The effects of tetrahydrocannabinol on the recognition of emotionally charged words: an analysis using event-related brain potentials. Neuropsychobiology. 1998;37:104–11. doi: 10.1159/000026487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Lewis EG, Dustman RE, Peters B, Straight RC, Beck EC. The effects of varying doses of delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol on the human visual and somatosensory evoked response. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1973;35:347–54. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(73)90190-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Baloh RW, Sharma S, Moskowitz H, Griffith R. Effect of alcohol and marijuana on eye movements. Aviat Space Environ Med. 1979;50:18–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Moskowitz H, Ziedman K, Sharma S. Visual search behavior while viewing driving scenes under the influence of alcohol and marihuana. Hum Factors. 1976;18:417–31. doi: 10.1177/001872087601800501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Ploner CJ, Tschirch A, Ostendorf F, Dick S, Gaymard BM, Rivaud-Pechoux S, Sporkert F, Pragst F, Stadelmann AM. Oculomotor effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in humans: implications for the functional neuroanatomy of the brain cannabinoid system. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:1016–23. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.10.1016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Thompson JP, Lam E, Thomas AM, Harry F, Hutchings AD, Marshall RW, Routledge PA. The pharmacokinetics and CNS effects of 5 and 10 mg of oral Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabidinol in man. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2000:385–6. [Google Scholar]
- 127.Abel EL. Effects of marihuana on the solution of anagrams, memory and appetite. Nature. 1971;231:260–1. doi: 10.1038/231260b0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Curran HV, Brignell C, Fletcher S, Middleton P, Henry J. Cognitive and subjective dose–response effects of acute oral Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in infrequent cannabis users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;164:61–70. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1169-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Block RI, Farinpour R, Braverman K. Acute effects of marijuana on cognition: relationships to chronic effects and smoking techniques. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1992;43:907–17. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(92)90424-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Peters BA, Lewis EG, Dustman RE, Straight RC, Beck EC. Sensory, perceptual, motor and cognitive functioning and subjective reports following oral administration of delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol. Psychopharmacologia. 1976;47:141–8. doi: 10.1007/BF00735812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Hart CL, van Gorp W, Haney M, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. Effects of acute smoked marijuana on complex cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2001;25:757–65. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00273-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Hart CL, Ward AS, Haney M, Comer SD, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. Comparison of smoked marijuana and oral Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;164:407–15. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1231-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Cowan J, Neidert G, Miller L. Marijuana and memory for feelings. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 1982;6:63–73. doi: 10.1016/s0364-7722(82)80109-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Block RI, Wittenborn JR. Marijuana effects on semantic memory: verification of common and uncommon category members. Psychol Rep. 1984;55:503–12. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1984.55.2.503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Slaybaugh G. Marihuana and memory: acquisition or retrieval? Science. 1971;173:1038–40. doi: 10.1126/science.173.4001.1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Dittrich A, Battig K, von Zeppelin I. Effects of (-)delta 9-trans-tetrahydrocannabinol (delta 9-THC) on memory, attention and subjective state. A double blind study. Psychopharmacologia. 1973;33:369–76. doi: 10.1007/BF00437515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Drew WG, Kiplinger GF, Miller LL, Marx M. Effects of propranolol on marihuana-induced cognitive dysfunctioning. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1972;13:526–33. doi: 10.1002/cpt1972134526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Klonoff H, Low M, Marcus A. Neuropsychological effects of marijuana. Can Med Assoc J. 1973;108:150–6. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Foltin RW, Fischman MW, Pippen PA, Kelly TH. Behavioral effects of cocaine alone and in combination with ethanol or marijuana in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1993;32:93–106. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(93)80001-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Pfefferbaum A, Darley CF, Tinklenberg JR, Roth WT, Kopell BS. Marijuana and memory intrusions. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1977;165:381–6. doi: 10.1097/00005053-197712000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Darley CF, Tinklenberg JR, Roth WT, Vernon S, Kopell BS. Marijuana effects on long-term memory assessment and retrieval. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1977;52:239–41. doi: 10.1007/BF00426706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Hrbek J, Siroka A, Navratil J, Medek A, Krejci Z, Komenda S. Acute effect of tetrahydrocannabinol (4 Mg, 8 Mg, 16 Mg) on verbal associations. Act Nerv Super (Praha) 1972;14:107–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Hrbek J, Komenda S, Siroka A, Navratil J, Macakova J. The effect of smoking THC on verbal associations. Agressologie. 1978;19:201–2. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Block RI, Wittenborn JR. Marijuana effects on visual imagery in a paired-associate task. Percept Mot Skills. 1984;58:759–66. doi: 10.2466/pms.1984.58.3.759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Ramaekers JG, Robbe HW, O'Hanlon JF. Marijuana, alcohol and actual driving performance. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2000;15:551–8. doi: 10.1002/1099-1077(200010)15:7<551::AID-HUP236>3.0.CO;2-P. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Lamers CT, Ramaekers JG. Visual search and urban driving under the influence of marijuana and alcohol. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2001;16:393–401. doi: 10.1002/hup.307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Robbe H. Marijuana's impairing effects on driving are moderate when taken alone but severe when combined with alcohol. Hum Psychopharmacol Clin Exp. 1998;13:S70–8. [Google Scholar]
- 148.Leirer VO, Yesavage JA, Morrow DG. Marijuana, aging, and task difficulty effects on pilot performance. Aviat Space Environ Med. 1989;60:1145–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Kielholz P, Hobi V, Ladewig D, Miest P, Richter R. An experimental investigation about the effect of cannabis on car driving behaviour. Pharmakopsychiatr Neuropsychopharmakol. 1973;6:91–103. doi: 10.1055/s-0028-1094372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Rogers RD, Wakeley J, Robson PJ, Bhagwagar Z, Makela P. The effects of low doses of delta-9 tetrahydrocannabinol on reinforcement processing in the risky decision-making of young healthy adults. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:417–28. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Weil AT, Zinberg NE. Acute effects of marihuana on speech. Nature. 1969;222:434–7. doi: 10.1038/222434a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Carlin AS, Bakker CB, Halpern L, Post RD. Social facilitation of marijuana intoxication: impact of social set and pharmacological activity. J Abnorm Psychol. 1972;80:132–40. doi: 10.1037/h0033317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Crockett D, Klonoff H, Clark C. The effects of marijuana on verbalization and thought processes. J Pers Assess. 1976;40:582–7. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4006_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Roth WT, Rosenbloom MJ, Darley CF, Tinklenberg JR, Kopell BS. Marihuana effects on TAT form and content. Psychopharmacologia. 1975;43:261–6. doi: 10.1007/BF00429261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Tinklenberg JR, Kopell BS, Melges FT, Hollister LE. Marihuana and alcohol, time production and memory functions. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1972;27:812–5. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1972.01750300074013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Block RI, Wittenborn JR. Marijuana effects on associative processes. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1985;85:426–30. doi: 10.1007/BF00429658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Pearl JH, Domino EF, Rennick P. Short-term effects of marijuana smoking on cognitive behavior in experienced male users. Psychopharmacologia. 1973;31:13–24. doi: 10.1007/BF00429295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Chesher GB, Franks HM, Hensley VR, Hensley WJ, Jackson DM, Starmer GA, Teo RK. The interaction of ethanol and delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol in man: effects on perceptual, cognitive and motor functions. Med J Aust. 1976;2:159–63. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Chesher GB, Franks HM, Jackson DM, Starmer GA, Teo RK. Ethanol and delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol interactive effects on human perceptual, cognitive and motor functions. II. Med J Aust. 1977;1:478–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Tinklenberg JR, Darley CF, Roth WT, Pfefferbaum A, Kopell BS. Marijuana effects on associations to novel stimuli. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1978;166:362–4. doi: 10.1097/00005053-197805000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Evans MA, Martz R, Rodda BE, Lemberger L, Forney RB. Effects of marihuana–dextroamphetamine combination. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1976;20:350–8. doi: 10.1002/cpt1976203350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Dornbush RL, Kokkevi A. Acute effects of cannabis on cognitive, perceptual, and motor performance in chronic hashish users. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1976;282:313–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1976.tb49906.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Darley CF, Tinklenberg JR, Hollister TE, Atkinson RC. Marihuana and retrieval from short-term memory. Psychopharmacologia. 1973;29:231–8. doi: 10.1007/BF00414037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Beautrais AL, Marks DF. A test of state dependency effects in marihuana intoxication for the learning of psychomotor tasks. Psychopharmacologia. 1976;46:37–40. doi: 10.1007/BF00421547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Milstein SL, MacCannell K, Karr G, Clark S. Marijuana-produced impairments in coordination. Experienced and nonexperienced subjects. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1975;161:26–31. doi: 10.1097/00005053-197507000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Salvendy G, McCabe GP., Jr Marijuana and human performance. Hum Factors. 1975;17:229–35. doi: 10.1177/001872087501700301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Roth WT, Tinklenberg JR, Whitaker CA, Darley CF, Kopell BS, Hollister LE. The effect of marihuana on tracking task performance. Psychopharmacologia. 1973;33:259–65. doi: 10.1007/BF00423060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Schwin R, Hill SY, Goodwin DW, Powell B. Marihuana and critical flicker fusion. Evidence for perceptual sharpening. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1974;158:142–4. doi: 10.1097/00005053-197402000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Hill SY, Goodwin DW, Schwin R, Powell B. Marijuana: CNS depressant of excitant? Am J Psychiatry. 1974;131:313–5. doi: 10.1176/ajp.131.3.313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Moskowitz H, Shea R, Burns M. Effect of marihuana on the psychological refractory period. Percept Mot Skills. 1974;38:959–62. doi: 10.2466/pms.1974.38.3.959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Block RI, Wittenborn JR. Marijuana effects on the speed of memory retrieval in the letter-matching task. Int J Addict. 1986;21:281–5. doi: 10.3109/10826088609063457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Moskowitz H, Sharma S, McGlothlin W. Effect of marihuana upon peripheral vision as a function of the information processing demands in central vision. Percept Mot Skills. 1972;35:875–82. doi: 10.2466/pms.1972.35.3.875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Caldwell DF, Myers SA, Domino EF, Merriam PE. Auditory and visual threshold effects of marihuana in man. Percept Mot Skills. 1969;29:755–9. doi: 10.2466/pms.1969.29.3.755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Leweke FM, Schneider U, Thies M, Munte TF, Emrich HM. Effects of synthetic delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol on binocular depth inversion of natural and artificial objects in man. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;142:230–5. doi: 10.1007/s002130050884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Koethe D, Gerth CW, Neatby MA, Haensel A, Thies M, Schneider U, Emrich HM, Klosterkotter J, Schultze-Lutter F, Leweke FM. Disturbances of visual information processing in early states of psychosis and experimental delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol altered states of consciousness. Schizophr Res. 2006;88:142–50. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.07.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.Sharma S, Moskowitz H. Effect of marihuana on the visual autokinetic phenomenon. Percept Mot Skills. 1972;35:891–4. doi: 10.2466/pms.1972.35.3.891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.Abel EL. Changes in personality response ratings induced by smoking marihuana. Br J Addict Alcohol Other Drugs. 1972;67:221–3. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1972.tb01197.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 178.Block RI, Erwin WJ, Farinpour R, Braverman K. Sedative, stimulant, and other subjective effects of marijuana: relationships to smoking techniques. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1998;59:405–12. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(97)00453-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179.Mathew RJ, Wilson WH, Chiu NY, Turkington TG, DeGrado TR, Coleman RE. Regional cerebral blood flow and depersonalization after tetrahydrocannabinol administration. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1999;100:67–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb10916.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180.Abel EL. Changes in anxiety feelings following marihuana smoking. The alternation in feelings of anxiety resulting from the smoking of marihuana (Cannabis sativa L.) Br J Addict Alcohol Other Drugs. 1971;66:185–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1971.tb02384.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 181.Lukas SE, Orozco S. Ethanol increases plasma Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) levels and subjective effects after marihuana smoking in human volunteers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2001;64:143–9. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(01)00118-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 182.Abrams DI, Vizoso HP, Shade SB, Jay C, Kelly ME, Benowitz NL. Vaporization as a smokeless cannabis delivery system: a pilot study. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2007;82:572–8. doi: 10.1038/sj.clpt.6100200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.