Abstract
Background
Strong evidence supports the association between childhood trauma and psychotic disorders. In two different high-risk populations, we looked for a correlation between the magnitude of schizotypal dimensions and the importance of self-reported childhood trauma.
Methods
A sample of 138 unaffected first-degree relatives was recruited (67 relatives of schizophrenic probands and 71 relatives of bipolar probands). The relationship between schizotypal dimensions and childhood trauma scores was analyzed by partial correlations.
Results
A positive correlation was found between childhood trauma scores and total schizotypal scores in first-degree relatives of schizophrenic subjects but not in first-degree relatives of bipolar probands. This correlation was primarily due to a strong association with the positive dimension of schizotypy.
Conclusions
The significant correlation between childhood trauma and schizotypal dimensions in subjects at high genetic risk for schizophrenia suggests that susceptibility genes for schizophrenia may interact with childhood trauma to induce the emergence of schizotypal dimensions, mainly positive psychotic features.
Introduction
Among the several explanations invoked to explain failures to find susceptibility genes, gene-environment interaction (GxE) suggests that “ignoring nurture may have handicapped the field’s ability to understand nature” (Moffitt et al., 2005). GxE can be defined as the genetic control of sensitivity to environmental factors, or as the environmental control of gene expression (Kendler & Eaves, 1986). Several recent studies demonstrated that the GxE approach can be of practical benefit as a tool in the search for genes connected with mental disorders (Kahn et al., 2003; Caspi et al., 2003, 2005; Kendler et al., 2005).
Childhood trauma has been repeatedly associated with psychotic features not only in schizophrenia (for review, see Read et al., 2005) but also in bipolar disorders (Hammersley et al., 2003; Neria et al., 2005) and schizotypy (Yen et al., 2002; Berenbaum et al., 2003). Therefore, childhood trauma may be a good candidate environmental risk factor for psychotic disorders. Schizotypy is a multidimensional construct that represents a mild imitation of the symptoms of schizophrenia (Vollema & Postma, 2002). Although schizotypy is the core feature of schizotypal personality disorder, it is thought to be a continuous or dimensional, rather a categorical construct (Irwin, 2001). Schizotypal dimensions have been described as a potential intermediate phenotype for both schizophrenia and bipolar disorders and could be the consequence of a gene-environment interaction. This association is difficult to examine prior to the identification of disease genes. The study of a population at “high genetic risk” which carries several unknown genes related to the disorder, may improve the possibility of identifying environmental risk factors. This approach may be more appropriate than studying the interaction between an environmental factor and a specific candidate gene. Studying the impact of childhood trauma on schizotypal dimensions among unaffected relatives has several advantages. Firstly, dimensional models of psychosis (positive, negative and disorganized symptoms) are becoming established as conceptually and clinically useful (Van Os et al., 2002). Secondly, first-degree relatives are free from the multiple artifacts that potentially confound research in schizophrenia, including the effects of long-term and usually ongoing medication treatment, multiple hospitalizations or institutionalization, and prolonged functional impairment secondary to chronic disease and social deterioration.
We investigated whether childhood trauma is correlated with the phenotypic expression of schizotypyal dimensions (positive, negative and disorganized) in two different populations of unaffected first-degree relatives. We hypothesized that the correlation between childhood trauma and schizotypal dimensions could be different among these two high-risk populations.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Consecutively admitted probands meeting DSM-IV criteria for bipolar disorder or schizophrenia were recruited from a university-affiliated hospital (Psychiatry department, Paris XII University). Their unaffected first-degree relatives were contacted and asked to participate in the study. Relatives were interviewed with the French version of the Diagnostic interview of Genetic Studies (DIGS) (Nurnberger et al., 1994; Preisig et al., 1999) to confirm the absence of diagnosis of schizophrenia or bipolar disorders. We used the validated French translation (Dumas et al., 2000) of the self-rating Schizotypal Personality Questionnnaire (SPQ) (Raine, 1991), to measure schizotypal dimensions in the two different groups of first-degree relatives. This questionnaire contains 74 items with yes/no answers and identifies the three classical dimensions of psychosis (positive, negative and disorganized). The SPQ can be used in the general population to identify individual differences in schizotypal dimensions.
Subjects also completed the French translation (Paquette et al., 2004) of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) (Bernstein & Fink, 1998), a 28-item self-report questionnaire, which gives scores for physical, emotional and sexual abuse, physical and emotional neglect, and a total weighted score.
The Research Ethics Board reviewed and approved the study. After completely describing the study, written informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Data analysis
Differences between groups were tested using a two-tailed t-test for continuous variables and a chi-squared test for discrete variables.
The relationships between schizotypal dimensions and CTQ scores were analyzed by partial correlation to control for the potential confounding influences of sex and age. Sex was used as covariable because of its influence on both CTQ and SPQ scores (Paquette et al., 2004; Dumas et al., 2000). Age at interview was also used as a covariable because some studies have observed a negative correlation between age of the subjects and scores for the SPQ scale and sub-scales (Venables & Bailes, 1994; Mata et al., 2005). To limit the risk of type I error due to multiple statistical tests, we examined only the total score of the CTQ, and we used Bonferroni procedure to establish the significance threshold (p < 0.05 divided by 12 statistical analyses; we retained p < 0.004). We also analyzed the variables dichotomously and calculated odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals (CI) for schizotypy given childhood trauma in the two populations of relatives. To do that, we determined the presence or absence of childhood trauma according to the cut-off of the French validation of the CTQ. The cut-off for the schizotypal dimensions was determined as the mean score of the SPQ full-score for the whole sample.
Results
The study group was composed of 67 first-degree relatives of schizophrenic probands (31men and 36 women, mean age (SD) = 54.2 (15.4) years) and 71 first-degree relatives of bipolar probands (37 men and 34 women, mean age (SD) = 53.1 (15) years). The sex ratio (χ2 = 0.47, p = 0.49) and the age at interview (t = 0.43, p = 0.67) did not differ between groups. The samples of first-degree relatives are composed of different types of relatives. In the two samples of relatives, the majority of the relatives are parents of the proband with an over-representation of parents in the sample of first-degree relatives of schizophrenic subjects (80.5% vs 59.2%). The highest completed school grade (education level) was recorded, according to usual conventions (Pichot et al., 1993) as a trichotomous variable (1 = elementary school; 3 = at least high school completed; 2 = intermediate between 1 and 3). However, we finally decided to pool the first two levels together because level 1 was infrequent in all groups. The two samples did not differ in terms of educational level (p = 0.40). In the sample of schizophrenic relatives: the mean SPQ scores (full-scale and for the positive, negative and disorganized dimensions) were 10.2 + 7.9; 3.9 + 3.8; 4.7 + 4.4 and 1.6 + 1.7 respectively; the mean CTQ score was 38.6 + 8.5. In the sample of bipolar relatives: the mean SPQ scores (full-scale and for the positive, negative and disorganized dimensions) were 9.6 + 7.5; 3.3 + 3.4; 4.3 + 4.7 and 1.9 + 2 respectively; the mean CTQ score was 39.3 + 9.8. The SPQ full-score and the CTQ score did not differ between the two groups (SPQ full-score: p = 0.73 and CTQ full-score: p = 0.95). The mean CTQ and SPQ scores were similar between males and females (p = 0.58 and p = 0.77, respectively). In the whole sample of relatives, we found a significant positive correlation between the CTQ score and the SPQ full-score (r = 0.27; p = 0.001). This correlation was fully explained by a correlation in the sample of first-degree relatives of schizophrenic subjects (r = 0.43; p < 0.0001), whereas no significant correlation was found in the first-degree relatives of bipolar probands (r = 0.13; p = NS). This positive correlation was found with the positive and negative dimensions of schizotypy (positive schizotypy: r = 0.41 and negative schizotypy: r = 0.36) but not with the disorganized dimension (Table 1). We have also investigated partial correlation coefficients between each schizotypal dimension and childhood trauma adjusted for the two other schizotypal dimensions. Only the correlation between positive dimension and childhood trauma remains significant.
Table 1.
SPQ full-score | Positive | Negative | Disorganized | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean score CTQ | ||||
Total sample (n = 138) | 0.27* | 0.30* | 0.13 | 0.20 |
First-degree relatives of schizophrenic subjects (n = 67) | 0.42* | 0.41* | 0.35* | 0.14 |
First-degree relatives of bipolar subjects (n = 71) | 0.13 | 0.21 | − 0.03 | 0.22 |
Statistical significance after Bonferroni correction
p<0.004
Based on a dichotomous approach, the calculation of the odds ratios leads to similar results. The odds of having high schizotypal traits in presence of childhood trauma were higher in the population of schizophrenic relatives (OR = 3.6, 95% CI [1.09 – 11.8]) than in the population of the bipolar relatives (OR = 1.64, 95% CI [0.57 – 4.72]), which were not significant.
Discussion
We found a significant positive correlation between history of childhood trauma and schizotypal dimensions in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenic subjects but not in relatives of bipolar probands. This correlation was mainly due to a strong association with the positive dimension of psychosis. To our knowledge, this is the first study exploring the influence of an environmental risk factor in two populations with different genetic backgrounds. Even though schizotypy was shown to be elevated in relatives of bipolar subjects (Kety et al., 1994; Schürhoff et al., 2005) suggesting some shared genetic vulnerability factors, the only difference between our two populations is that one is at genetic risk for schizophrenia and the other for bipolar disorders. We observed that the impact of childhood trauma on the expression of schizotypal dimensions was strongest among subjects who were at high genetic risk for schizophrenia suggesting that susceptibility genes specific to schizophrenia may interact with childhood trauma to induce the emergence of schizotypal dimensions, mainly positive psychotic features. This is in accordance with several previous reports from the literature (for review, see Read et al., 2005) showing a high influence of childhood trauma on positive dimension in schizophrenia and schizotypy. In line with our results, using a follow-up design, Spauwen et al. (2006) found an association between self-reported trauma and psychosis proneness. The strength of the association increased with a narrower psychosis definition. These authors have speculated that extended exposure to trauma may increase the risk for positive symptoms through direct effects on dopamine function. Along similar lines, Read et al. (2005) have suggested that early, prolonged, and severe traumas increase the risk for later positive symptoms through lasting on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Even if associations between negative dimension and childhood trauma have been previously reported (Resnick et al., 2003; Lysaker et al., 2001), this association is more difficult to interpret. A plausible explanation is that traumatic avoidance, emotional numbing and reduced responsiveness may resemble negative dimension and may be interpreted as a reaction to the trauma. Conversely, in rodents and nonhuman primates, trauma (for example, forced swim test) has been used as a model of the negative symptoms of schizophrenia such as flattening of affect and avolition (Corbett et al., 1999). Concerning the disorganized dimension, as in previous studies, we did not show any correlation with childhood trauma (Read & Argyle, 1999; Hammersley et al., 2003). However, this could be also explained by the small variation of this dimension. Our study could be considered as an example of how an environmental factor interacting with specific susceptibility genes can influence susceptibility to a disorder or to a particular dimension of a disorder. Our results can suggest that the same dimensions have different genetic background in bipolar disorders and schizophrenia. However, there is also a more plausible explanation. The same genes cause susceptibility for a common phenotype in bipolar disorders and schizophrenia, in particular the positive dimension (Berretini, 2003; Schürhoff et al., 2003). In order to become manifest, this susceptibility needs to interact with some environmental factors. However, other factors, disorder-specific, can exert a protective or permissive role on the action of these environmental factors. For example, traits that have been associated with genetic susceptibility to bipolar disorders (i.e cyclothymic, hyperthymic or depressive temperaments) might have a protective effect on the emergence of positive schizotypal traits in the presence of childhood trauma.
The possibility that susceptibility genes for schizophrenia may increase exposure to childhood trauma can be excluded as rates of childhood trauma were similar among the two groups of relatives. However, because of the design of our study, two alternative hypotheses could be proposed. Indeed, our results could also be explained by shared family environmental factors amongst the schizophrenia relatives rather than genetic factors – this could only be ruled out by employing an adoption study design. In other words, the childhood trauma could be a proxy for other environmental risks such as social disadvantage or family discord/chaos specific to schizophrenic families. Moreover, due to the retrospective assessment of the childhood trauma, we cannot exclude that subjects reported high level of childhood trauma because they have a high level of schizotypal features. Nevertheless, the fact that no association is found in the relatives of bipolar probands suggests that a bias cannot entirely explain the association found in relatives of schizophrenic subjects between trauma and schizotypal dimensions. It is also interesting to note that despite the fact that relatives tend to be defensive in their response, the use of the SPQ discriminates the two samples of relatives. Overall, our results support the assumption that the clinical expression of the vulnerability induced by environmental risk factors depends on gene-environment interactions. Speculation about the implications of this finding should be guarded until these results are replicated as we cannot formally exclude the possibility that childhood trauma interacted with other specific environmental risk factors.
In conclusion, our findings suggest that susceptibility genes specific to schizophrenia may influence response to pathogenic environments. It is clear that if a gene’s connection to disorder is conditional to the environment, this will have the natural consequence of diminishing researchers’ capacity to detect the association between the gene and disorder. Research to uncover specific environmental risk factors for diseases may be more effective if we recruit samples on the basis of their high genetic loading and if we investigate which genes discriminate between exposed individuals who did, versus did not, become ill.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by grants from the Agence Nationale de la Recherche (Manage-BP), the Fondation pour la Recherche sur le Cerveau, The RTRS (Réseau Thématique Translationel de Recherche et de Soins) and the Institut de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM). The authors would like to thank Marie-José De Sousa and Emmanuelle Abadie for their technical assistance. We also thank an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments.
Footnotes
Declaration of interest
None
References
- Berenbaum H, Valera EM, Kerns JG. Psychological trauma and schizotypal symptoms. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2003;29:143–152. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein DP, Fink L. Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: a retrospective self-report manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Berrettini W. Evidence for shared susceptibility in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Part C) 2003;123:59–64. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.c.20014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, McLay J, Mill J, Martin J, Braithwaite A, Poulton R. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301:386–389. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Cannon M, McLay J, Murray R, Harrington H, Taylor A, Arseneault L, Williams B, Braithwaite A, Poulton R, Craig IW. Moderation of the effect of adolescent-onset cannabis use on adult psychosis by a functional polymorphism in the catechol-O-methyltransferase: longitudinal evidence of a gene X environment interaction. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:1117–1127. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbett R, Zhou L, Sorensen SM, Mondadori C. Animal models of negative symptoms: M100907 antagonizes PCP-induced immobility in a forced swim test in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;21:S211–S218. [Google Scholar]
- Dumas P, Bouafia S, Gutknecht C, Saoud M, Dalery J, d’Amato T. Validation of the French version of the Raine Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire – categorical and dimensional approach to schizotypal personality traits in a normal student population. Encephale. 2000;26:23–29. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammersley P, Dias A, Todd G, Bowen-Jones K, Reilly B, Bentall RP. Childhood trauma and hallucinations in bipolar affective disorder: preliminary investigation. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;182:543–547. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.6.543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irwin HJ. The relationship between dissociative tendencies and schizotypy: an artefact of childhood trauma? Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2001;57:331–342. doi: 10.1002/jclp.1015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahn RS, Khoury J, Nichols WC, Lanphear BP. Role of the dopamine transporter genotype and maternal prenatal smoking in childhood hyperactive-impulsive, inattentive, and oppositional behaviors. Journal of Pediatrics. 2003;143:104–110. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(03)00208-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Eaves LJ. Models for the joint effect of genotype and environment on liability to psychiatric illness. Amercian Journal of Psychiatry. 1986;143:279–289. doi: 10.1176/ajp.143.3.279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Kuhn JW, Vittum J, Prescott CA, Riley B. The interaction of stressful life events and a serotonin transporter polymorphism in the prediction of episodes of major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:529–535. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kety SS, Wender PH, Jacobsen B, Ingraham LJ, Jansson L, Faber B, Kinney DK. Mental illness in the biological and adoptive relatives of schizophrenic adoptees. Replication of the Copenhagen study in the rest of Denmark. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1994;51:442–455. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950060006001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lysaker PH, Meyer PS, Evans JD, Clementz CA, Marks KA. Childhood sexual trauma and psychosocial functioning in adults with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Services. 2001;52:1485–1488. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.52.11.1485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mata I, Mataix-Cols D, Peralta V. Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire-Brief: factor structure and influence of sex and age in a nonclinical population. Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;38:1183–1192. [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M. Strategy for investigating interactions between measured genes and measured environments. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:473–481. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neria Y, Bromet EJ, Carlson GA, Naz B. Assaultive trauma and illness course in psychotic bipolar disorder: findings from the Suffolk country mental health project. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2005;111:380–383. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00530.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nurnberger JI, Jr, Blehar MC, Kaufmann CA, York-Cooler C, Simpson SG, Harkavy-Friedman J, Severe J, Malaspina D, Reich T. Diagnostic interview for genetic studies. Rationale, unique features, and training. NIMH Genetics Initiative. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1994;51:849–859. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950110009002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paquette D, Laporte L, Bigras M, Zoccolillo M. Validation of the French version of the CTQ and prevalence of the history of maltreatment. Santé Mentale au Quebec. 2004;29:201–220. doi: 10.7202/008831ar. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pichot P, Lebeaux MO, Penhouët C, Simon M. L’estimatin du niveau intellectuel prémorbide à la WAIS-R. Comparaison entre la méthode de Barona et l’Index de deterioration de Wechsler. Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée. 1993;43:291–301. [Google Scholar]
- Preisig M, Fenton BT, Matthey ML, Berney A, Ferrero F. Diagnostic interview for genetic studies (DIGS): inter-rater and test-retest reliability of the French version. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 1999;249:174–179. doi: 10.1007/s004060050084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raine A. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1991;17:555–564. doi: 10.1093/schbul/17.4.555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Read J, Argyle N. Hallucinations, delusions and thought disorders among adult psychiatric inpatients with a history of child abuse. Psychiatry Services. 1999;50:1467–1472. doi: 10.1176/ps.50.11.1467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Read J, Van Os J, Morrison AP, Ross CA. Childhood trauma, psychosis and schizophrenia: a literature review with theoretical and clinical implications. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2005;112:330–350. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00634.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resnick S, Bond G, Mueser K. Trauma and PTSD in people with schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:415–423. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.112.3.415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schürhoff F, Szöke A, Méary A, Bellivier F, Rouillon F, Pauls D, Leboyer M. Familial aggregation of delusional proneness in schizophrenia and bipolar pedigrees. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1313–1319. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.7.1313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schürhoff F, Laguerre A, Szöke A, Méary A, Leboyer M. Schizotypal dimensions: continuity between schizophrenia and bipolar disorders. Schizophrenia Research. 2005;80:235–242. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spauwen J, Krabbendam L, Lieb R, Wittchen HU, Jansen JA, Van Os J. Impact of psychological trauma on the development of psychotic symptoms: relationship with psychosis proneness. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;188:527–533. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.011346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Os J, Gilvarry C, Bale R, Van Horn E, Tattan T, White I, Murray R. A comparison of the utility of dimensional and categorical representations of psychosis. UK 700 group. Psychological Medicine. 2002;29:595–606. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798008162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venables PH, Bailes K. The structure of schizotypy, its relation to subdiagnoses of schizophrenia and to sex and age. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1994;33:277–294. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1994.tb01124.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vollema MG, Postma B. Neurocognitive correlates of schizotypy in first degree relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2002;28:367–377. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yen S, Shea MT, Battle CL, Johnson DM, Zlotnick C, Dolan-Sewell R, Skodol AE, Grilo CM, Gunderson JG, Sanislow CA, Zanarini MC, Bender DS, Rettew JB, McGlashan TH. Traumatic exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder in borderline, schizotypal, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders: findings from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2002;190:510–518. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200208000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]