Skip to main content
Archives of Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition logoLink to Archives of Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition
. 2007 Feb 16;92(4):F265–F270. doi: 10.1136/adc.2006.104000

Children born weighing less than 1701 g: visual and cognitive outcomes at 11–14 years

Terence Stephenson 1,2,3,4,5,6, Sharon Wright 1,2,3,4,5,6, Anna O'Connor 1,2,3,4,5,6, Alistair Fielder 1,2,3,4,5,6, Ann Johnson 1,2,3,4,5,6, Sonia Ratib 1,2,3,4,5,6, Michael Tobin 1,2,3,4,5,6
PMCID: PMC2675424  PMID: 17307810

Abstract

Background and objective

Few studies of low birthweight children have explored the relationship between later visual morbidity and neuropsychological function. This study evaluated these outcomes using a geographically defined cohort.

Methods

Prospective study of retinopathy of prematurity (ROP) in infants born weighing <1701 g, undertaken in 1985–7. 254 of the survivors consented to ophthalmic examination at 10–13 years. Four children were severely disabled and could not complete the tests. 198 of the remaining agreed to neuropsychological assessment at 11–14 years (British Ability Scales II (BAS), Movement Assessment Battery (ABC), Neale Analysis of Reading Ability).

Results

At 10–13 years, 99/198 children had an adverse ophthalmic outcome (AOO) (reduced acuity n = 48, myopia n = 40, strabismus n = 36, colour defect n = 2, field defect n = 1). There were no significant differences between children with AOO and those with a normal ophthalmic outcome with regard to sex, gestation, birth weight, neonatal cranial scan appearances and social class. 106/198 had ROP; 98 had mild ROP with no increased risk of AOO in later childhood. All eight children with severe ROP had an AOO in later childhood. Children with an AOO performed worse on the BAS, ABC and reading ability tests.

Conclusions

At age 10–13, 50% of children born <1701 g have an AOO. These children are not simply those with earlier gestations, lower birth weight or ROP. Children with AOO have a worse neuropsychological outcome. The next step is to determine whether there are visual interventions which can improve ophthalmic outcome and whether a better neuropsychological outcome follows.

Keywords: prematurity, low birthweight, retinopathy of prematurity, ophthalmic outcome


Survivors of preterm birth are at increased risk of cognitive impairment1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9 and poor school performance.2,3,7,9,10 In particular, specific difficulties in reading, spelling, number skills and handwriting have been described. Low birthweight children are also at risk of later vision disorders, such as strabismus, defective visual acuity and myopia,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18 whether or not they had retinopathy of prematurity (ROP).19 The relationship between disorders of vision and cognitive deficits is not well understood as the two areas of function have rarely been studied in detail and concurrently in the same children.20 In low birthweight children, it is not known if vision and cognitive disorders have a common causal pathway or whether problems in the visual pathways impair specific areas of school performance.

We studied this relationship in a geographically defined cohort of low birthweight children to test the hypothesis that in school‐aged children who were born with low birth weight, there is an association between neuropsychological outcome and visual morbidity, and that this association is independent of social and perinatal factors.

Methods

Subjects

A geographically defined, prospective study of ROP was undertaken in 1985–7.21 Infants born to mothers resident in the East Midlands region in the UK, with birth weight <1701 g and surviving three weeks, were enrolled from five neonatal units in Nottingham, Derby and Leicester. This cohort of 505 infants (mean gestation 31.1 weeks; mean birth weight 1345 g) had ophthalmic examinations from 3 to 12 weeks of age and at 6 months corrected age. Of these, 248 developed ROP, of whom 4% had severe ROP (stage 3 or worse).

Tracing the subjects

Of the 505 infants, 29 had died, 5 had left the UK and 23 could not be traced. Two families were not contacted at their doctor's request. Sixteen families declined to participate and 176 did not give written consent despite repeated reminders (three postal and two telephone). Thus 254 families consented to ophthalmic examination, conducted by one orthoptist.19 However, four children were too disabled to undertake neuropsychological assessments. Of 250 families, 198 gave written informed consent to neuropsychological assessment (approximately one year after ophthalmic assessment) conducted by one psychologist. All investigators were masked to the child's perinatal history and original ROP status. We chose not to study a matched control group of term infants as we were principally interested in within‐cohort analyses.

Assessment protocol

Ophthalmic investigation

The ophthalmic tests are described in detail elsewhere.19 In summary, these were: visual functions (logMAR distance and near acuity,22,23 contrast sensitivity, stereoacuity, perimetry, colour vision); strabismus (cover test and prism tests); and refractive state and eye dimensions. Corrected visual acuity was assessed using the child's own spectacles or a pinhole if refractive error had not been previously detected or acuity remained below 0.0 logMAR units even with glasses.

Cognitive ability

The overall measure of the British Ability Scales II (BAS II)24 is the general conceptual ability (GCA) score with mean (SD) of 100 (15) in the normal population. The GCA score sums three clusters: verbal, non‐verbal and spatial abilities. Individual BAS items have a mean of 50 (10). In addition, there are “diagnostic scales” which do not contribute towards the GCA score (eg speed of information processing).

Motor ability

The Movement Assessment Battery for Children (ABC) 25 tests dexterity, ball skills and balance. A score below the 5th percentile of the normal population is classified as a definite motor problem, and between 5th and 15th percentiles as borderline.

Reading ability

The Neale Analysis of Reading Ability—Revised is an oral test for children aged 6–12 years to assess accuracy, rate and comprehension.26

Statistical methods

We calculated either the means and standard deviations or, for variables not normally distributed, medians and interquartile ranges (IQRs). All data were investigated for normality, by histograms, to determine whether parametric or non‐parametric methods should be used. The unpaired t test was used for normally distributed variables and the Mann–Whitney U test for variables not normally distributed. χ2 tests were performed on categorical data. When comparing more than two groups, one‐way analysis of variance or the Kruskal–Wallis test was used for continuous data and the χ2 test for categorical data. Multivariate linear regression was used to control for potential confounders. Testing was in all cases two‐sided. We analysed all data using the statistics packages SPSS (version 8) and STATA (version 5).

Results

Ophthalmic assessment was carried out between the ages of 10 and 13 years (mean 11 years, 6 months). Neuropsychological assessment was carried out between the ages of 11 and 14 years (mean 12 years, 8 months). Of the 198 children who had a neuropsychological assessment, 174 were white, 12 were Asian, 6 were black and 6 were mixed race. We did not find any significant differences between the 198 children assessed and the 307 not assessed with regard to birth weight, gestational age, sex, incidence of ROP or cranial ultrasound abnormalities (table 1). Parental occupation was not recorded at birth. Current social class of children not assessed was not derived because their parents declined consent to providing information.

Table 1 Comparison of the neonatal characteristics of children assessed and those who could not be assessed from the original cohort of 505.

Children assessed (n = 198) Children not assessed (n = 307) p Value
n (%) n (%)
Sex
 Male 99 (50) 152 (49.5) 0.855
 Female 99 (50) 147 (47.9) 2, 1 df)
 Unknown 0 8 (2.6)
Birth weight (g) 0.369
 Mean 1335.4 1351 (Mann–Whitney)
 Number ⩽1000 g 24 (12.1) 46 (15)
 Unknown 0 1
 Median 1377.5 1420
 IQR 1147.5, 1560 1137.5, 1580
Gestational age (weeks) 0.086
 Mean (SD) 30.8 ( 3.04) 31.3 (3.01) (t test)
 Number ⩽28 weeks 51 (25.8) 52 (16.9)
 Unknown 0 1
Retinopathy of prematurity status 0.394
 None 92 (46.5) 165 (53.7) 2, 3 df)
 Stage 1 67 (33.8) 84 (27.4)
 Stage 2 31 (15.7) 46 (15)
 Stage 3/4 8 (4) 12 (3.9)
Neonatal cranial ultrasound 0.136
 Abnormal 61 (30.8) 91 (29.6) 2, 2 df)
 Normal 58 (29.3) 69 (22.5)
 Not done 79 (39.9) 147 (47.9)

Ophthalmic findings

Of the 198 children, 99 (50%) were classified as having an adverse ophthalmic outcome at 10–13 years.19 An adverse ophthalmic outcome was defined as corrected binocular visual acuity below the norm (6/6 Snellen, 20/20 in the USA or 0.0 logMAR units) (n = 48, 24%) or the presence of strabismus (n =  36, 18%), myopia (n = 40, 20%), colour vision defect (n = 2, 1%) or field defect (n = 1, 0.5%). Some children had more than one defect. These outcomes result in a referral to ophthalmic services in the UK. All children who had strabismus before 7 months corrected age still had strabismus at 12 years. Even those who had surgery still had some residual deviation. We found no statistically significant differences between the children with an adverse ophthalmic outcome and those with a normal ophthalmic outcome with regard to sex, gestational age, birth weight, neonatal cranial ultrasound scan appearances and current social class (table 2).

Table 2 Comparison of the characteristics of children with a normal or an adverse ophthalmic outcome at mean age 11 years and 6 months.

Normal ophthalmic outcome Adverse ophthalmic outcome p Value
(n = 99) (n = 99)
n (%) n (%)
Sex
 Male 50 (50.5) 49 (49.5) 0.887
 Female 49 (49.5) 50 (50.5) 2, 1 df)
Age of neuropsychological assessment
 Mean (SD) 12.77 (0.58) 12.67 (0.62) 0.240
(t test)
Birth weight (g)
 Mean 1363.3 1307.5 0.145
 Number ⩽1000 g 10 (10.1) 14 (14.1) (Mann–Whitney)
 Median 1450 1300
 IQR 1150, 1560 1140, 1540
Gestational age (weeks)
 Mean (SD) 31.1 ( 2.79) 30.5 (3.26) 0.169
 Number ⩽28 weeks 21 (21.2) 30 (30.3) (t test)
Retinopathy of prematurity status
 None 49 (49.5) 43 (43.4) 0.013
 Stage 1 37 (37.4) 30 (30.3) (Fisher's exact test)
 Stage 2 13 (13.1) 18 (18.2)
 Stage 3 or 4 0 8 (8.1)
Neonatal cranial ultrasound
 Abnormal 34 (30.8) 27 (27.3) 0.488
 Normal 26 (29.3) 32 (32.3) 2, 2 df)
 Not done 39 (39.9) 40 (40.4)
Social class (mother)
 1 and 2 23 (23.2) 16 (16.2) 0.283
 3 32 (32.3) 37 (37.4) 2, 3 df)
 4 and 5 20 (20.2) 28 (28.3)
 Unknown 24 (24.2) 18 (18.2)
Social class (father)
 1 and 2 27 (27.3) 28 (28.3) 0.444
 3 28 (28.3) 37 (37.4) 2, 3df)
 4 and 5 16 (1.2) 11 (11.1)
 Unknown 28 (28.3) 23 (23.2)

Of the 198 children, 106 had ROP in the neonatal period. Unsurprisingly, children with ROP were smaller and less mature at birth (table 3). The presence or stage of ROP in the neonatal period, excluding the eight children with severe ROP, did not predict which children would have an adverse ophthalmic outcome at 10–13 years (table 2). On excluding these eight with stage 3/4 ROP, there was no longer a significant association between ROP status and adverse ophthalmic outcome (p = 0.45 rather than 0.013).

Table 3 Comparison of birth weight and gestational age between no retinopathy of prematurity (ROP) and ROP groups.

No ROP (n = 92) ROP (n = 106) p Value
Birth weight (g) <0.001
 Median 1500 1190 Mann–Whitney U test
 Interquartile range 1320, 1600 1048, 1485
Gestational age (weeks) <0.001
 Mean 32.45 29.43 t test
 SD 2.67 2.64 Mean difference (95% CI) 3.01 (2.27 to 3.76)

Cognitive findings

First, the cohort performed below average in almost all areas (table 4). However, children who had ROP in the neonatal period did not have lower BAS II scores overall 11–14 years later.

Table 4 British Ability Scale (BAS) II scores of children with a normal or an adverse ophthalmic outcome, (mean age 12 years 8 months).

BAS II component All children (n = 198) Mean (SD) Normal ophthalmic outcome (n = 99) Mean (SD) Adverse ophthalmic outcome (n = 99) Mean (SD) Comparing adverse and normal outcome (p value) Children with retinopathy of prematurity in newborn period (n = 106) Mean (SD)
General conceptual ability standard score 93.0 (18.2) 95.7 (17.6) 90.4 (18.5) 0.04 94.1 (19.1)
Verbal reasoning cluster 96.5 (17.2) 98.2 (16.4) 94.8 (17.9) 0.172 98.2 (17.4)
 Word definition 45.4 (12.4) 46.7 (11.9) 44.1 (12.8) 0.131 46.6 (12.9)
 Verbal similarities 52 (46, 56)* 53 (46, 56)* 51 (45, 56)* 0.399† 53 (47, 57)*
Non‐verbal reasoning cluster 90.2 (17.8) 92.3 (16.8) 88.2 (18.7) 0.099 91.5 (17.1)
 Matrices 44.0 (10.2) 44.5 (9.3) 43.6 (11) 0.518 44.6 (10.3)
 Quantitative reasoning 45.2 (12.3) 46.6 (12.8) 43.8 (11.5) 0.101 45.6 (12.7)
Spatial ability cluster 96.0 (17.4) 99.2 (17) 92.8 (17.4) 0.01 96.2 (18.4)
 Recall of designs 47.6 (12.5) 49.5 (12.5) 45.8 (12.3) 0.037 48.1 (12.3)
 Pattern construction 48.3 (10) 50.1 (9.3) 46.4 (10.4) 0.009 47.9 (11.2)
Diagnostic scales
 Speed of information processing 51.4 (13.6) 53.8 (12.6) 49 (14.1) 0.012 52 (14.1)
 Recall of digits forward 49.5 (11.5) 51.1 (9.6) 48 (11.8) 0.055 51.2 (11)
 Recall of digits backwards 44.5 (36,51) 46 (37, 52)* 43 (32, 49)* 0.016† 47 (36, 54)*
 Recall of pictures 50.6 (10.2) 51.7 (9.6) 49.6 (10.8) 0.157 50.5 (11.1)
 Recall of objects—immediate verbal 49.3 (12) 51 (10.9) 47.6 (12.8) 0.044 50.2 (12.4)
 Recall of objects—delayed verbal 53 (44, 60)* 53 (48, 60)* 52 (39, 60)* 0.182† 53 (43, 60)*
 Recall of objects—immediate spatial 49 (43, 53)* 49 (43, 53)* 49 (41, 53)* 0.869† 49 (43, 53)*
 Recall of objects—delayed spatial 49 (44, 53)* 49 (44, 53)* 49 (44, 53)* 0.424† 49 (44, 53)*

*Median and interquartile range; †Mann–Whitney U test.

Second, we compared BAS II scores for children with and without an adverse ophthalmic outcome. There was an association between long‐term ophthalmic outcome and performance on the BAS II in almost all areas (statistically significant for spatial ability, p = 0.01). Excluding the eight with stage 3/4 ROP (all had an adverse ophthalmic outcome), this association between adverse ophthalmic outcome and spatial ability remained strong (p = 0.02). Children with an adverse ophthalmic outcome performed significantly worse on both design recall and pattern construction within the spatial ability cluster (p = 0.037 and p = 0.009, respectively). Those with an adverse ophthalmic outcome performed worse on speed of information processing and in recall within the diagnostic scales.

Third, the results of multivariate linear regression showed that after controlling for ROP stage, gestational age, birth weight, cranial ultrasound, social class and sex, the BAS scores (table 5) were still associated with adverse ophthalmic outcome. The associations were, therefore, independent of these potential confounding factors, insofar as we were able to test them. A cerebral scan was not done for 39 (39.9%) of the children with normal ophthalmic outcome and 40 (40.4%) with adverse ophthalmic outcome. (Scanning was not routinely done in the East Midlands in 1985–7.) Maternal social class data were not available for 24 (24.2%) of the children with normal ophthalmic outcome and 18 (18.2%) with adverse ophthalmic outcome because the parent declined this information. Oxygen dependence at 28 or 36 weeks was not recorded in the original dataset.

Table 5 Results of regression models for significant outcomes only.

Outcome Adverse ophthalmic regression coefficient (SE) p Value
General conceptual ability standard score 9.26 (0.03) 0.028
Verbal standard score 9.78 (3.78) 0.012
Word definition T score 7.9 (2.8) 0.006
Recall of objects—delayed verbal 8.14 (2.9) 0.006
Spatial standard score 8.93 (4.16) 0.035
Recall of objects—immediate verbal 9.74 (2.88) 0.001

Fourth, the presence of strabismus, abnormal three‐dimensional vision (stereopsis) and decreased visual acuity with correction were all significantly associated with poor spatial ability (see table 6 for the p vaues). Binocular contrast sensitivity with correction and refractive error were not associated with BAS II spatial ability scores (data not shown).

Table 6 Association between performance on the British Ability Scale (BAS) II spatial ability cluster and measures of specific ophthalmic functions.

BAS II spatial score p Value
Strabismus n Mean SD
 Yes 36 88.3 17.7 0.003
 No 162 97.7 16.9 (unpaired t test)
Stereopsis n Median IQR
 Normal (TNO ⩽60) 136 98 88, 111 0.001
 Slightly reduced (TNO >60 but <480) 29 99 83, 107 (Kruskal–Wallis test)
 Moderately reduced (TNO ⩾480) 33 86 69, 95
Binocular distance acuity with correction (log units) n Median IQR
 Normal (⩽0.0) 151 98 87, 110 0.007
 Slightly reduced (0.0–0.3) 43 95 82, 105 (Kruskal–Wallis test)
 Moderately reduced (>0.3) 4 67 55.3, 81.8

IQR, interquartile range.

TNO (Netherlands Organisation for Applied Scientific Research) test for stereoscopic depth sense uses images of two colours, representing the two retinal views of a three‐dimensional scene, presented to each eye separately using coloured spectacles. Stereoscopic vision can be quantified over the range 15–1980 s of arc.27

Binocular acuity with correction: 0.0 is equivalent to Snellen 6/6 (normal) and 1.0 is equivalent to 6/60 (very poor vision).

Fifth, neonatal ROP status was examined in relation to BAS II performance (table 7). Only 8 (4%) of the cohort had severe ROP (stage 3 or above), none had stage 5 ROP (total retinal detachment), and none were registered blind. Those children with ROP stage 3 or 4 achieved lower BAS II GCA and cluster scores. There was no systematic trend in scores between those who never had ROP and those with stages 1 and 2.

Table 7 British Ability Scale (BAS) II performance in association with neonatal retinopathy of prematurity (ROP) status.

Neonatal ROP status Number tested General conceptual ability standard score Spatial ability
Mean (SD) No. (%) with score <85* Mean (SD) No. (%) with score <85*
All children 198 93.0 (18.2) 56 (28.3) 96 (17.4) 49 (25)
ROP status
 None 92 92 (17.2) 28 (30.4) 95.9 (16.4) 20 (21.7)
 Stage 1 67 96.1 (18.9) 17 (25.4) 98.4 (17.9) 17 (25.4)
 Stage 2 31 93.1 (16.6) 6 (19.4) 93.6 (17.5) 8 (25.8)
 Stage 3/4 8 79.3 (24.4) 5 (62.5) 84.4 (22.8) 4 (50.0)
p Value (one‐way analysis of variance 0.078 0.174

*More than 1 SD below mean.

Reading ability

Children with an adverse ophthalmic outcome performed less well in all three areas of reading ability than those with normal ophthalmic outcome (up to 0.3 SD worse), but this difference was not statistically significant (data not shown).

Movement ability

In movement ability tests, 113 (57%) of the cohort scored below the 15th percentile for the normal population (data not shown). Children with an adverse ophthalmic outcome had worse median scores in all four areas of the ABC (statistically significant for ball skills, p = 0.01).

Discussion

Our geographical cohort study is unusual because it combines neonatal and long‐term ophthalmic data with later neuropsychological data. The return rate of 254 children examined at 11–14 years out of 505 at 3–12 weeks is a criticism, but it follows 10 years of no contact. Other follow‐up studies from preterm to teenage show comparable rates.28 There were no statistically significant differences in gestational age, birth weight and incidence of ROP between the 254 tested and the 222 children not tested but alive.19 We suggest therefore that follow‐up of these 254 children over a long period is both valuable and representative of the original cohort. Sensitivity analysis showed that the adverse ophthalmic outcome rate in those not followed up would have to be 3/222 (1.4%) to overturn the statistical significance of the study cohort rate of adverse ophthalmic outcome (50%).

An adverse ophthalmic outcome at age 10–13 years was found in 99/198 children examined (50%); 18% had strabismus, consistent with other studies.29,30 Adverse ophthalmic outcome was not simply a product of being born more preterm or being born more socially deprived (table 2). This confirms the findings of Powls et al17 but they were unable to exclude the possible confounder of neonatal ROP as their very low birthweight cohort were not screened ophthalmologically. In our study, 106 children had ROP in the neonatal period (table 3). ROP still occurs,31 but stage 1 and 2 ROP do not predict ophthalmic problems 10–13 years later (table 2).

Only one other long‐term, follow‐up, geographical study of ophthalmic and cognitive outcomes in a cohort of preterm infants has been published.20 In that study, 279 children out of a potential 382 born before 32 weeks' gestation in Liverpool during 1991–2 and attending mainstream schools (thus excluding children with major disability) were re‐examined at seven years. The children's original ROP status and neonatal cranial ultrasound scan data were extracted retrospectively from the clinical case records of the eight hospitals involved. As in our study, mothers not resident in the defined geographical region at the time of the infant's birth were excluded. A total of 143 infants weighed less than 1500 g at birth, and of these 115 had been examined for ROP. Thirty‐one were recorded as having had stage 1, 20 stage 2 and 8 stage 3 ROP. Infants over 1500 g birth weight were not routinely screened for ROP. At seven years, preterm infants were considerably more likely to wear glasses, to have poor visual acuity, reduced stereopsis and strabismus than term controls. The strongest associations in performance were between visual outcome and motor impairment. Ophthalmic impairments were considerably related to poorer scores on visual‐motor integration testing, the Movement ABC and Wechsler intelligence quotient (IQ) tests, with the exception of verbal IQ. Ophthalmic impairments were not significantly related to neonatal cranial ultrasound appearances. Stage 3 retinopathy was related to poorer subsequent acuity but not ROP stage 1 or 2. Our findings at secondary school (11–14 years) are therefore entirely consistent with this previous follow‐up in primary school at seven years. This adds weight to the general conclusions of both studies, particularly as our findings come from a different geographical cohort and time period with different investigators using different methods. The Liverpool cohort was selected by gestation and geographical area whereas ours was selected by birth weight and geographical area. Moreover, the ROP data used in our analysis were collected prospectively by a single examiner 21 and we did not routinely exclude children with major disability from ophthalmic follow‐up (although four were unable to perform the cognitive tests).

Our cohort had lower cognitive attainments at school age consistent with previous research.4,32,33,34,35,36 Our motor findings are comparable with another preterm cohort from a similar era.37 Roth et al,38 reporting outcomes at 14–15 years in a cohort born <33 weeks' gestation between 1979 and 1982, also found reading age was lower compared with standardised norms, but they had no detailed ophthalmic outcome data.

Our most striking finding was that cognitive attainment of low birthweight children at age 11–14 was more closely associated with their current visual status than with retinopathy in the neonatal period. Those with an adverse ophthalmic outcome scored, on average, 6.4 points less on spatial abilities (table 4). It is biologically plausible that the spatial ability cluster of the BAS would be strongly associated with an adverse visual outcome. The association between BAS II scores and ophthalmic outcome remained after controlling for ROP, gestation, birth weight, cranial ultrasound, social class and sex. We do not have data on postnatal dexamethasone usage, which could affect both cognitive and visual outcomes by impairing grey matter growth.39 However, if postnatal steroids explained the association of BAS II scores and an adverse ophthalmic outcome, we would expect gestation and birth weight to be significant confounders, which they were not, as steroids were most frequently used in such infants.

Strabismus, abnormal three‐dimensional vision (stereopsis) and decreased corrected visual acuity were associated with BAS II spatial ability score (table 6). Strabismus can also be due to cerebral palsy and neuromuscular impairments rather than ocular damage alone. However, irrespective of underlying mechanism, an adverse ophthalmic outcome results from lack of binocular functions plus amblyopia (reduced acuity).

It is sometimes not appreciated that a child can have reduced acuity without being myopic. The observed association of BAS II spatial ability with reduced acuity must be via visual pathway impairments rather than the refractory apparatus (cornea, lens, eyeball) because corrected visual acuity was assessed. Acuity was tested with spectacles (43/198 children wore glasses) so the reduced acuity was not due to myopia. Myopia was not associated with poor spatial ability. Finally, previous mild ROP does not explain these later problems with spatial ability (table 7).

Does the adverse ophthalmic outcome cause the poorer cognitive performance or are these associated because of damage to both the visual pathways and the cognitive centres? If the causal explanation is correct, it is possible that early identification of subtle visual disturbance may allow ophthalmic and educational intervention, concentrating on improving spatial skills. Against simple association is that visual problems at secondary school age are not explained by greater prematurity, lower birth weight or more abnormal neonatal ultrasound scan appearances (table 2), all factors in poor cognitive outcome. 33,40 Similarly, when Cooke et al20 compared infants with birth weights above and below 1500 g, there were no significant differences in visual outcomes at seven year follow‐up. Furthermore, in that study, strabismus, poor visual acuity, absent stereopsis and low contrast sensitivity were also unrelated to the maximum extent of periventricular haemorrhage or periventricular leukomalacia on neonatal cranial ultrasound scan.20 However, the lack of a relationship between neonatal ultrasound scan appearances and the more subtle visual problems which we and Cooke et al20 describe do not exclude the possibility of a common hypoxic/ischaemic insult. Lower birth weight or major cranial ultrasound abnormalities37,40 can certainly predict major disabilities.32,34,41,42,43,44,45 Marked visual disability can also be predicted from cranial ultrasound abnormalities.46,47,48,49,50

However, in contrast to major disabilities, ultrasound prediction of learning difficulties, possibly due to less severe diffuse white matter damage,51,52,53 is less precise.54 The sensitivity of ultrasound to detect small areas of white matter abnormality is relatively poor.55,56,57 Furthermore, the effects of hypoxia/ischaemia may not be apparent on imaging until 32 weeks postconceptual age58,59 and may cause reduced grey matter volume60 which ultrasound cannot easily measure.

All but one of the subscales of the BAS II in which there were significant differences between the children with adverse and normal ophthalmic outcome involve a timed component. The education system places a great emphasis on timed procedures, such as school examinations. The potential additive effect of ophthalmic problems and underachieving in timed tasks might be substantial. Coupled with motor problems (which could cause poor handwriting), reduced reading comprehension and behaviour disorders,32 there are several additive factors which could lead to poor educational performance in ex‐preterm children without overt disability.

Conclusions

The main finding of the present study is that half of children born at less than 1701 g birth weight have an adverse ophthalmic outcome associated with poorer cognitive outcome at 11–14 years. Neither outcome is due to mild ROP. Rather, poor cognitive attainment is associated with adverse visual outcomes which manifest after the perinatal period. To determine causality, the effect of early detection and management of visual defects on later cognitive function could be studied. Low birthweight children should undergo early, regular visual assessment to detect visual impairment and enable intervention.

What is already known on this topic

  • At 7 years of age, the survivors of preterm birth are at increased risk of later vision disorders, such as strabismus, defective visual acuity and myopia.

  • Ophthalmic impairments at 7 years are strongly related to poorer scores on visuomotor integration testing, the Movement Assessment Battery for Children and Wechsler IQ tests. Stage 3 retinopathy is related to poorer subsequent acuity but not stage 1 or 2.

What this study adds

  • Half of a geographical cohort of children born weighing less than 1701 g and followed to 11–14 years had an adverse ophthalmic outcome (reduced visual acuity, strabismus, myopia, colour vision defect or field defect), and this was associated with poorer cognitive outcome.

  • Neither poor cognitive nor poor ophthalmic outcome at 11–14 years are associated with previous stage 1 or 2 retinopathy of prematurity. Rather, poor cognitive attainment is associated with adverse visual outcomes which manifest after the perinatal period.

Abbreviations

ABC - Movement Assessment Battery for Children

BAS - British Ability Scales

GCA - general conceptual ability

ROP - retinopathy of prematurity

Footnotes

This study was funded by Action Medical Research.

Competing interests: None.

Ethics approval was granted by University Hospital, Nottingham LREC.

References

  • 1.Aylward G, Pfeiffer S, Wright A.et al Outcome studies of low birth weight infants published in the last decade: a metaanalysis. J Pediatr 1989115515–520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Saigal S, Szatmari P, Rosenbaum P.et al Cognitive abilities and school performance of extremely low birth weight children and matched term control children at 8 years: a regional study. J Pediatr 1991118751–760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Horwood L, Mogridge N, Darlow B. Cognitive, educational and behavioural outcomes at 7 to 8 years in a national very low birthweight cohort. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 199879F12–F20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pharoah P, Stevenson C, Cooke R.et al Prevalence of behaviour disorders in low birthweight infants. Arch Dis Child 199470271–274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mutch L, Leyland A, McGee A. Patterns of neuropsychological function in a low birthweight population. Dev Med Child Neurol 199335943–956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ericson A, Kallen B. Very low birthweight boys at the age of 19. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 199878F171–F174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hall A, McLeod A, Counsell C.et al School attainment, cognitive ability and motor function in a total Scottish very‐low‐birthweight population at eight years: a controlled study. Dev Med Child Neurol 1995371037–1050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hunt J, Cooper B, Tooley W. Very low birth weight infants at 8 and 11 years of age: role of neonatal illness and family status. Pediatrics 198882596–603. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ross G, Lipper E, Auld P. Educational status and school‐related abilities of very low birth weight premature children. Pediatrics 1991881125–1134. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hille E, Ouden A D, Bauer L.et al School performance at nine years of age in very premature and very low birth weight infants: Perinatal risk factors and predictors at five years of age. J Pediatr 1994125426–434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Darlow B, Clemett R, Horwood J.et al Prospective study of New Zealand infants with birth weight less than 1500 g and screened for ROP: visual outcome at age 7–8 years. Br J Opthalmol 199781935–940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fledelius H. Pre‐term delivery and subsequent ocular development. 4) Oculometric and other metric considerations. Acta Ophthalmol Scand 19964301–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Fledelius H. Retinopathy of prematurity in Denmark. Epidemiological considerations and screening limits. Eur J Ophthalmol 19966183–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Fledelius H. Pre‐term delivery and subsequent ocular development. 2) Binocular function. Acta Ophthalmol Scand 199674294–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Fledelius H. Pre‐term delivery and subsequent ocular development. 3) Refraction. Myopia of prematurity. Acta Ophthalmol Scand 199674297–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Fledelius H. Pre‐term delivery and subsequent ocular development. 1) Visual function, slit‐lamp findings and fundus appearance. Acta Ophthalmol Scand 199674288–293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Powls A, Botting N, Cooke R.et al Visual impairment in very low birthweight children. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 199776F82–F87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Holmstrom G. Ophthalmological long term follow up of preterm infants: a population based, prospective study of the refraction and its development. Br J Ophthalmol 1998821265–1271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.O'Connor A, Stephenson T J, Johnson A.et al Long term ophthalmic outcome of low birth weight children with and without retinopathy of prematurity. Pediatrics 200210912–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Cooke R W I, Foulder‐Hughes L, Newsham D.et al Ophthalmic impairment at 7 years of age in children born very preterm. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 200489F249–F253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ng Y, Fielder A, Shaw D.et al Epidemiology of ROP. Lancet 1988ii1235–1238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bailey I, Lovie J. New design principles for visual acuity letter charts. Am J Optom Physiol Opt 197653740–745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sparks C, Sparks J, Johnson S. A new look at crowding. Br Orthoptic J 1993507–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Elliot C, Murray D, Pearson L.The British ability scales (revised edition). Windsor: NFER‐Nelson, 1983
  • 25.Henderson S, Sugden D.Movement assessment battery for children. London: The London Psychological Corporation, 1992
  • 26.Neale M D.Neale analysis of reading ability: manual of directions and norms. London: Macmillan, 1966
  • 27.Lameris O. TNO test for stereoscopic vision. Lameris: Utrecht 1972
  • 28.O'Brien F, Roth S, Stewart A.et al The neurodevelopmental progress of infants less than 33 weeks into adolescence. Arch Dis Child 200489207–211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gibson N A, Fielder A R, Trounce J Q.et al Ophthalmic findings in infants of very low birthweight. Dev Med Child Neurol 1990327–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Page J M, Schneeweiss S, Wyte H E A.et al Ocular sequelae in premature infants. Pediatrics 199392787–790. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Inder T E, Clement R S, Austin N C.et al High iron status in very low birthweight infants is associated with increased risk of ROP. J Pediatr 1997131541–544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ornstein M, Ohlsson A, Edmonds J.et al Neonatal follow‐up of very low birthweight/extremely low birthweight infants to school age: a critical overview. Acta Paediatr Scand 199180741–748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Botting N, Powls A, Cooke R.et al Cognitive and educational outcome of very‐low‐birthweight children in early adolescence. Dev Child Neurol 199840652–660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.McCormick M, Brooks‐Gun J, Workman‐Daniels K.et al The health and development status of very low‐birth‐weight children at school age. JAMA 19922672204–2208. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Saigal S, Rosenbaum P, Stoskopf B.et al Comprehensive assessment of the health status of extremely low birthweight children at eight years of age: comparison with a reference group. J Pediatr 1994125411–425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Powls A, Botting N, Cooke R.et al Hand preference in teenage very low birthweight children. Dev Med Child Neurol 199638594–602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Jongmans M, Mercuri E, Vries L D.et al Minor neurological signs and perceptual‐motor difficulties in prematurely born children. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 199776F9–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Roth S, Wyatt J, Baudin J.et al Neurodevelopmental status at 1 year predicts neuropsychiatric outcome at 15–15 years of age in very preterm infants. Early Hum Dev 20016581–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Murphy B P, Inder T E, Huppi P S.et al Impaired cerebral cortical gray matter growth after treatment with dexamethasone for neonatal chronic lung disease. Pediatrics 2001107217–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Cooke R. Factors affecting survival and outcome at 3 years in extremely preterm infants. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 199471F28–F31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.McCormick D M, Gortmaker S, Sobol A very low birthweight children: behaviour problems and school difficulty in a national sample. J Pediatr 1990117687–693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Vries L D, Eken P, Groenendaal F.et al Correlation between the degree of periventricular leukomalacia diagnosed using cranial ultrasound and MRI later in infancy in children with cerebral palsy. Neuropaediatrics 199324263–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Paneth N, Rudelli R, Kazam E.et al Brain damage in the preterm infant. Clinics in Developmental Medicine Vol. 131. London: MacKeith Press, 1994
  • 44.Guit G, Bor M V, Ouden L D.et al Prediction of neurodevelopmental outcome in the preterm infant: MR staged myelination compared with cranial ultrasound. Pediatr Radiol 1990175107–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Pinto‐Martin J, Riolo S, Cnaan A.et al Cranial ultrasound prediction of disabling and non‐disabling cerebral palsy at age two in a low birthweight population. Pediatrics 199595249–254. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hungerford J, Stewart A, Hope P. Ocular sequelae of preterm birth and their relation to ultrasound evidence of cerebral damage. Br J Ophthalmol 198670463–468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Scher M, Dobson V, Carpenter N.et al Visual and neurological outcome of infants with periventricular leukomalacia. Dev Med Child Neurol 198931353–365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Weisglas‐Kuperus N, Heersema D, Baerts W.et al Visual functions in relation with neonatal cerebral ultrasound, neurology and cognitive development in very low birthweight children. Neuropaediatrics 199324149–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Eken P, Niuwenhuizen O, Graaf Y.et al Relation between neonatal cerebral ultrasound abnormalities and cerebral visual impairment. Dev Med Child Neurol 1994363–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Pike M, Holmstrom G, Vries L D.et al Patterns of visual impairment associated with lesions of the preterm infant brain. Dev Med Child Neurol 199436849–862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Leviton A, Gilles F. Ventriculomegaly, delayed myelination, white matter hypoplasia, and periventricular leucomalacia. How are they related? Pediatr Neurol 199615127–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Inder T E, Huppi P S, Zientara G P.et al Early detection of periventricular leucomalacia by diffusion‐weighted magnetic resonance imaging techniques. J Pediatr 1999134631–634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Volpe J J. Neurobiology of periventricular leucomalacia. Pediatr Res 200150553–562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Levene M, Dowling S, Graham M.et al Impaired motor function (clumsiness) in five‐year‐old children: correlation with neonatal ultrasound scan. Arch Dis Child 199267687–690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Volpe J J. Brain injury in the preterm infant: overview of clinical aspects, neuropathology, and pathogenesis. Semin Pediatr Neurol 19985135–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Maalouf E f, Duggan P J, Counsell S J. Comparison of findings on cranial ultrasound and magnetic resonance imaging in preterm infants. Pediatrics 2001107719–727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Huppi P S, Amato M. Advanced magnetic resonance imaging techniques in perinatal brain injury. Biol Neonate 2001807–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Du Plessis A J, Volpe J J. Perinatal brain injury in the preterm and term newborn. Curr Opin Neurol 200215151–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Volpe J J. Overview: normal and abnormal brain development. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 200061–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Inder T E, Volpe J J. Mechanisms of perinatal brain injury. Semin Neonatal 200053–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Archives of Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES