Abstract
Drosophila circadian rhythms are controlled by a neural circuit containing ∼150 clock neurons. Although much is known about mechanisms of autonomous cellular oscillation, the connection between cellular oscillation and functional outputs that control physiological and behavioral rhythms is poorly understood. To address this issue, we performed whole-cell patch-clamp recordings on lateral ventral clock neurons (LNvs), including large (lLNvs) and small LNvs (sLNvs), in situ in adult fly whole-brain explants. We found two distinct sizes of action potentials (APs) in >50% of lLNvs that fire APs spontaneously, and determined that large APs originate in the ipsilateral optic lobe and small APs in the contralateral. lLNv resting membrane potential (RMP), spontaneous AP firing rate, and membrane resistance are cyclically regulated as a function of time of day in 12 h light/dark conditions (LD). lLNv RMP becomes more hyperpolarized as time progresses from dawn to dusk with a concomitant decrease in spontaneous AP firing rate and membrane resistance. From dusk to dawn, lLNv RMP becomes more depolarized, with spontaneous AP firing rate and membrane resistance remaining stable. In contrast, circadian defective per0 null mutant lLNv membrane excitability is nearly constant in LD. Over 24 h in constant darkness (DD), wild-type lLNv membrane excitability is not cyclically regulated, although RMP gradually becomes slightly more depolarized. sLNv RMP is most depolarized around lights-on, with substantial variability centered around lights-off in LD. Our results indicate that LNv membrane excitability encodes time of day via a circadian clock-dependent mechanism, and likely plays a critical role in regulating Drosophila circadian behavior.
Keywords: circadian rhythms, Drosophila, membrane excitabiliy, time of day, clock neuron, cellular oscillation
Introduction
Drosophila circadian rhythms are governed by ∼150 autonomously oscillating clock neurons, which include a number of anatomically and functionally distinct cell groups (Helfrich-Forster, 2003; Shafer et al., 2006; Nitabach and Taghert, 2008). Transcriptional feedback loops essential for cellular oscillation of clock neurons have been studied extensively and are well understood (for review, see Harmer et al., 2001; Hardin, 2005; Rosato et al., 2006). Cellular oscillation also relies on depolarization-dependent events at the clock neuron plasma membrane (Nitabach et al., 2002, 2005; Wu et al., 2008). However, the connection between cellular oscillation and clock neuron outputs that regulate Drosophila behavioral and physiological rhythms is still mostly unclear. The neuropeptide pigment dispersing factor (PDF)-expressing lateral ventral subset (LNv)of clock neurons has been proved to contain the pacemaker of the circadian control circuit in constant darkness (DD), and regulated release of PDF from small subset of LNv (sLNv) nerve terminals plays a critical role in synchronizing downstream clock neurons (Renn et al., 1999; Helfrich-Forster et al., 2000; Park et al., 2000; Peng et al., 2003; Stoleru et al., 2004; Nitabach et al., 2006). The molecular and cellular mechanisms of regulated release of PDF by LNvs remain unclear.
Clock neurons of the mammalian suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) exhibit circadian time-dependent electrical properties, with a higher spontaneous AP firing rate and more depolarized resting membrane potential (RMP) during the day than at night (Green and Gillette, 1982; Groos and Hendriks, 1982; de Jeu et al., 1998; Kuhlman and McMahon, 2004). Clock-driven rhythms in various ionic conductances play key roles in circadian regulation of RMP and spontaneous AP frequency in mammalian clock neurons (de Jeu et al., 1998; Pennartz et al., 2002; Kuhlman and McMahon, 2004; Itri et al., 2005; Pitts et al., 2006). However, very little is known about the regulation of membrane excitability of Drosophila clock neurons.
Park and Griffith (2006) reported the first whole-cell recordings from the large subset of LNvs (lLNvs) in whole-brain explants, and compared lLNv RMP and membrane resistance at two times of day in light/dark conditions (LD) and DD. Although they failed to detect spontaneous APs in their recordings, raising the possibility that their recording conditions could be different from in vivo physiological conditions, they found that time of day and lighting condition affected lLNv RMP and membrane resistance. This study is consistent with recent work of the Sheeba et al. (2008), who showed rhythmic changes in lLNv RMP and AP firing rate assayed at four different time points after 2 weeks in constant darkness. We have developed an optimized recording protocol that reveals spontaneous activity and likely more closely replicates physiological conditions, and used this protocol to investigate the modulation of LNv membrane properties continuously across the 24 h day. Applying this approach to wild-type (WT) and circadian-defective mutant flies, we show that lLNv clock neurons encode time of day via circadian clock-dependent oscillatory modulation of membrane excitability. We also apply this approach to the very difficult-to-record-from sLNvs, demonstrating that their RMP is most depolarized around lights-on in LD.
Materials and Methods
Fly stocks.
Flies were maintained at 25°C in a 12 h light/dark cycle with ∼2000 lux in the light phase. Female pdf-GAL4/CyO;UAS-RFP/TM6C,Sb flies that express red fluorescent protein (RFP) solely in LNV clock neurons were collected for electrophysiological recordings. Virgin female y per0 w flies were crossed with male pdf-GAL4/CyO;UAS-RFP/TM6C,Sb flies for collecting male y per0 w;pdf-GAL4/+;UAS-RFP/+ flies for electrophysiological experiments.
Brain dissection.
After eclosion (3–7 d), flies were anesthetized with CO2 and then immersed in 70% ethanol for ∼30 s before being transferred to a dissection chamber filled with external recording solution. External solution consisted of (in mm) 101 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1 CaCl2, 4 MgCl2, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 5 glucose, and 20.7 NaHCO3, pH 7.2, with an osmolarity of 250 mmol/kg. After head cuticle, eyes, proboscis, and trachea were removed with fine forceps, the brain was dissected out and placed on the floor of the recording chamber, ventral side up. A mammalian brain slice “harp” holder with a 0.5 mm interval between each nylon fiber was placed on top of the fly brain to secure it during recording.
Electrophysiology.
Fly brains were continuously perfused with external solution bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2 at room temperature. LNvs were visualized by RFP fluorescence using an Olympus fixed-stage upright microscope (BX50WI); sLNVs and lLNVs were distinguished by their sizes and anatomical locations. The immediate area surrounding the intended lLNV or sLNv was enzymatically digested with focal application of protease XIV (2 mg/ml; Sigma). Whole-cell recordings were performed using borosilicate standard wall capillary glass pipettes (Sutter Instrument) and data were acquired with Axopatch 200B amplifier, Digidata 1200 A/D hardware, and pClamp 8.0 software (Molecular Devices). Recording pipettes are filled with internal solution consisting of (in mm) 102 potassium gluconate, 17 NaCl, 0.085 CaCl2, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.5 Na-GTP, 0.94 EGTA, and 8.5 HEPES, pH 7.2, with an osmolarity of 235 mmol/kg. Gigaohm seals were achieved before recording in cell-attached configuration in voltage-clamp mode, followed by break-in to whole-cell configuration while in voltage-clamp mode. To confirm maintenance of a good seal and absence of damage to the cell, a 40 mV hyperpolarizing pulse was imposed on each cell while in whole-cell voltage-clamp mode from a holding potential of −80 mV. Only if the resulting inward leak current was less than −50 pA was that cell used for subsequent current-clamp measurements of RMP, AP firing rate, and membrane resistance. RMP was determined after stabilization of the membrane potential after the transition from voltage-clamp to current-clamp mode, and for cells with oscillating membrane potential was defined at the trough of the oscillation. AP firing rate was computed over the 5 min period after the transition from voltage-clamp to current-clamp configuration. Membrane resistance was measured by injecting −20 pA current in current-clamp recording mode. The amplitude of sinusoidal oscillation of RMP was defined as the difference between peak and trough. For recordings on flies in the dark phase, all recordings were made within 20 min from the fly being taken out of the incubator to minimize any acute effect of light.
For the gap junction inhibitor experiments, halothane, carbenoxolone (CBX), and 2-octanol were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich. Halothane and CBX were prepared with external solution, and 2-octanol was dissolved in DMSO and diluted with external solution.
Statistical analysis.
Relationships between time of day and parameters of membrane electrical excitability were modeled using linear regression (Origin 6.1; OriginLab) and nonlinear regression analysis (SPSS 14). The null hypothesis we tested in linear regression analysis is that the slope of any fitted regression line is zero. For nonlinear regression analysis, the temporal relationship between lLNv RMP and time of day was fitted to a sinusoidal curve. The null hypothesis for this analysis is that there is no sinusoidal relationship between RMP and time of day. We modeled lLNv RMP as a function of time of day (ZT) in LD: RMP = a × sin(b + c × ZT) + d.
The period was calculated using the formula period (hours) = 2π/c, and the peak and trough of the sinusoid curve were defined as peak (hours) = (π/2 − b)/c and trough (hours) = (3π/2 − b)/c, respectively. Best fits to the data were generated using the Sequential Quadratic Programming iterative fitting algorithm, with initial parameters of a = −0.1, b = −0.1, c = −0.1, and d = −0.1. These initial parameter values represent a very shallow sinusoid with a very long period. To test the validity of fits to the real data, we fit randomly reshuffled data sets generated by reshuffling the RMP associated with each time point measured. We combined both linear regression analysis and nonlinear regression analysis to further validate our sinusoidal model. We analyzed the data between the peak and trough of the fitted sinusoidal curve with linear regression analysis. If the null hypothesis was accepted in linear regression analysis (i.e., RMP distributed like a horizontal line from peak to trough), we then accepted the null hypothesis in nonlinear regression analysis even if the p value in nonlinear regression analysis was <0.05 (i.e., RMP versus time of day did not fit a sinusoidal model). The rationale is that the linear fit of data from the peak to trough of a sinusoidal curve should be significantly different from a horizontal line.
Results
Characterization of membrane excitability of lLNv clock neurons
We performed whole-cell electrophysiological recordings on lLNv clock neurons and characterized RMP, spontaneous AP firing rate, and membrane resistance. To verify that our internal recording solution was reasonably close to physiological, we first recorded spontaneous AP firing in cell-attached configuration after forming a gigaohm seal but before breaking into whole-cell configuration. This allowed us to record APs without dialyzing the cytoplasm with pipette solution. One example of such a recording is shown in Figure 1A. During 3 min in cell-attached configuration, this cell exhibited 31 bursts of APs, with each burst containing two to six APs. After breaking into whole-cell current-clamp configuration, 52 bursts of APs occured in 5 min. The AP firing frequencies and patterns were very similar both before and after breaking into whole-cell configuration, suggesting that our recording solutions permit reasonably physiological spontaneous membrane activity.
Not surprisingly, there is some variability from cell to cell in lLNv membrane activity. As shown in the 10 s trace in Figure 1A, this lLNv exhibits 0.2 Hz ∼10 mV oscillation in RMP, with a burst of two to six APs riding on the depolarized phase. In another cell shown in Figure 1B, RMP oscillates with an ∼5 mV amplitude, and spontaneous APs occur on the depolarized phase of some RMP cycles, but not the majority of them (82%). Some lLNvs exhibit two different sizes of APs (Fig. 1C). We categorized the spontaneous membrane activity during the light phase of LD of 58 lLNvs from >50 different fly brains. Thirty-five lLNvs exhibited RMP oscillation with an average frequency of 0.28 ± 0.18 Hz and an average peak-to-trough amplitude of 7.4 ± 3.5 mV (mean ± SD). Neither the frequency nor amplitude of this oscillation is significantly correlated to time of day (linear regression, p > 0.05). Of these 35 oscillating cells, 26 exhibited spontaneous AP bursts, whereas 9 were silent. There was no correlation between spontaneous AP firing and amplitude of RMP oscillation (p > 0.05). Of the 23 nonoscillating cells, six exhibited repetitive sustained high-frequency bursts, three exhibited tonic high-frequency firing, five fired APs without any obvious pattern, and seven were silent. Of the 38 total cells that fired APs, 16 had APs all of approximately the same magnitude, peaking at ∼45 mV above rest, whereas 22 also had a second population of APs peaking only ∼20 mV above rest. There was no relationship between time of day and the presence or absence of RMP oscillations or multiple AP sizes.
Small action potentials come from the contralateral optic lobe
Because there are two distinct sizes of APs in >50% of lLNvs that fire APs, we investigated the cellular mechanisms of these two different APs. lLNvs extend ramified arborization in the optic lobes of both sides of the brain, and dendritic aborizations of all lLNvs from either side of the fly brain mostly overlap (for review, see Helfrich-Forster, 2002, 2003). This makes it possible that lLNvs are electrically coupled with each other via gap junctions, and small APs arise from large APs in neighboring lLNvs propagating to the recorded cell through gap junctions. Also, each lLNv contains dendritic arbors in each of the two optic lobes (Park and Griffith, 2006). APs generated in the contralateral optic lobe travel a significantly longer distance (300–500 μm) than those generated in the ipsilateral optic lobe (<50 μm) to reach the soma, where signals are recorded. Therefore, another possibility is that small APs result from the decay of large APs generated in the contralateral optic lobe as they travel passively along the posterior optic tract (POT) to reach the lLNv soma.
To test the electrical coupling hypothesis, we examined whether pharmacological gap junction inhibitors could specifically reduce the magnitude of small APs. Three different gap junction inhibitors, halothane (0.1%), 2-octanol (2 mm), and carbenoxolone (CBX, 1 mm) (Schneider and Stengl, 2006), were used. All three gap junction inhibitors decreased the frequency of small APs, but had no effect on the amplitude. Gap junction inhibitors showed a complex effect on large APs. Like small APs, the amplitude of large APs was unaltered by gap junction inhibitors; however, the frequency of large APs exhibited mixed responses, as five of eight lLNvs in halothane, one of four in CBX, and one of five in 2-octanol showed an increase, whereas three in halothane, two in CBX, and four in 2-octanol showed a decrease, and one in CBX showed no change. In the example shown in Figure 2, after the application of 0.1% halothane, both large and small AP firing frequencies decreased gradually but were not totally abolished. Importantly, neither large nor small AP amplitude was changed by halothane. Bath application of gap junction blockers is expected to induce a gradual decrease in the amplitude of small APs as the blockade takes effect if they were propagating from coupled cells through gap junctions. Because only the frequency but not the amplitude of small APs decreased after blocking gap junctions, it is unlikely that small APs are generated in coupled cells and propagated through gap junctions to the cell being recorded from.
We then tested the hypothesis that small APs originate in the distal contralateral arbor of the cell being recorded from. As depicted schematically in Figure 3A, we disconnected lLNv soma from their contralateral arbors by severing the POT in the middle of the brain using a scalpel. As a sham-cut control, we made a similar cut, but dorsal to the POT. After cutting the POT, only large APs were found in all lLNvs tested (n = 7), whereas after the sham operation of eight lLNvs, one was silent, two exhibited only large APs, and five showed both large and small APs. We also cut some fly brains into two complete halves along the midline. Of 19 lLNvs recorded from half brains, 5 did not fire any APs, 13 fired only large APs, and 1 fired both large and small APs. Unlike in the whole brain, where the occurrence of small APs appears temporally uncorrelated with large APs, every small AP recorded from the lLNv in the single half brain that exhibits small APs occurred ∼10 ms before a large AP, suggesting these small APs may have a different origin from those recorded in the normal whole brain preparation. The likelihood of occurrence of small APs in lLNvs in cut-POT fly brains and complete half brains is significantly different from that in the control fly brains (χ2 test, both comparisons, p < 0.0001). These results strongly suggest that small APs originate in the contralateral optic lobe arbor of the lLNv recorded from.
Regulation of lLNv membrane excitability in LD
We assessed the dependence of membrane excitability on time of day in LD. We recorded 109 lLNvs at different times in LD, nearly all of them (>100) from different individual fly brains. Figure 4 depicts scatter plots of RMP, spontaneous AP firing rate, and membrane resistance as a function of the time when recordings were performed, with each data point representing an individual lLNV. In LD, RMP exhibits a robust cyclical dependence on time of day, highest just after lights-on, declining over the course of the day (linear regression, r = −0.54; p < 0.0001), and then increasing during the night (linear regression, r = 0.41; p < 0.01). Consistent with this temporal pattern of RMP in the light phase, spontaneous AP firing rate is highest just after lights-on, and becomes lower as the day progresses (linear regression, r = −0.35; p < 0.01). In the light phase, membrane resistance also decreases over time (linear regression, r = −0.49; p < 0.01). In the dark phase, spontaneous AP firing rate and membrane resistance do not change significantly over time (linear regression, p > 0.1).
We also fit the relationship between RMP and time of day to a sinusoidal model. RMP versus time of day best fits the sinusoidal model y = 6.0 × sin (1.75 + 0.29 × x) − 49.7 (nonlinear regression, p < 0.0001). The amplitude of this sinusoid is 6.0 mV and the period is 21.4 h. We also fit multiple randomly reshuffled data sets of RMP versus time of day to a sinusoidal model, and in all cases best fits were to sinusoids with very short noncircadian periods (supplemental Fig. 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Data sets are randomized by randomly reshuffling the measured RMPs for each cell among the times of day of each recording. This absence of significant fit of any randomly reshuffled data sets to a circadian sinusoid strongly supports the validity of the sinusoidal fit to the real data. The peak and trough of the sinusoid of RMP versus time of day are indicated by vertical dashed lines in Figure 4. The decrease in RMP between the peak and trough and the increase between the trough and peak of the sinusoid are both statistically significant by linear regression analysis (p < 0.01). Spontaneous AP firing rate and membrane resistance decrease significantly from the peak to trough of the sinusoid (linear regression, p < 0.05). However, there is no statistically significant change in spontaneous AP firing rate and membrane resistance over time from the trough to peak (p > 0.1). These different methods of statistical analysis all strongly support the conclusion that time of day in LD is encoded by lLNv membrane excitability.
Regulation of per0 circadian mutant lLNv membrane excitability in LD
To test whether rhythmic changes in WT lLNv membrane excitability are simply responses to the LD cycle (i.e., “masking”) or, alternatively, depend on circadian clock function, we assayed membrane excitability of lLNvs in per0 null mutant flies. per0 flies lack a functional circadian clock, exhibit only behavioral masking responses to light transitions in LD, and are arrhythmic in DD (supplemental Fig. 3, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material) (Konopka and Benzer, 1971). We recorded 87 lLNvs from per0 flies in LD, each from one individual fly. The distribution of per0 lLNv RMP either in the light phase, in the dark phase, or over the entire 24 h cycle is not significantly different from a horizontal line (linear regression, p > 0.1). The relationship between per0 lLNv RMP and time of day best fits a sinusoidal model y = −1.4 × sin (3.39–0.26 × x) − 44.8 (p < 0.0001) (Fig. 5A). Whereas the fit to this sinusoid is statistically significant, its amplitude is less than one-fourth that of WT (compare Fig. 4). Randomly reshuffled data sets of per0 lLNv RMP versus time of day do not fit sinusoids with circadian period (supplemental Fig. 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), suggesting that the small-amplitude circadian oscillation observed in the raw data is valid. However, linear regression analysis of RMP from the trough to peak of the sinusoid reveals no significant relationship between RMP and time of day (p > 0.1). Linear regression analysis between lights-on and the trough of the sinusoid reveals that RMP becomes slightly more hyperpolarized during this interval (p < 0.05). Correspondingly, per0 lLNv membrane resistance decreases gradually over this interval (p < 0.05), but spontaneous AP firing rate is not significantly changed (p > 0.05). The RMP, spontaneous AP firing rate, and membrane resistance of per0 lLNvs do not change significantly after this 7 h interval starting at lights-on, or when analyzed for the entire light phase, dark phase, or complete 24 h cycle (linear regression, p > 0.1). Therefore, these brief, time-dependent changes in per0 lLNv membrane excitability right after lights-on in LD must be masking effects, because per0 flies have no circadian timekeeper. We therefore conclude that the much larger and more robust time-dependent membrane excitability changes observed in WT lLNvs in LD almost wholly arise out of circadian clock function.
WT lLNv membrane excitability in DD
Rhythmic transcriptional feedback ceases in lLNvs, but not other clock neurons, by the first day after transition from LD to DD (Yang and Sehgal, 2001; Shafer et al., 2002). Thus, if robust clock-dependent oscillation of lLNv membrane excitability in LD is cell autonomous (i.e., driven by cellular oscillation of the lLNvs themselves), then the prediction is that lLNv membrane excitability will not oscillate in DD. To test this prediction, we measured lLNv membrane excitability in DD. As shown in Figure 6, lLNv RMP becomes slightly more depolarized over time in DD (linear regression, slope, 0.14; p < 0.01), but there is no hint of any circadian rhythm, as revealed by the inability to fit these data to a sinusoid with circadian period. Spontaneous AP firing rate and membrane resistance exhibit no significant changes over 24 h in DD. This absence of rhythmic changes in lLNv membrane excitability in DD, when transcriptional feedback oscillation ceases in lLNvs but continues in other clock neurons, supports the conclusion that the rhythmic changes occurring in LD are driven cell-autonomously by the circadian clock of lLNvs themselves.
Regulation of sLNv membrane excitability in LD
Although the specific function of lLNv versus sLNv clock neurons remains unclear, it has been suggested that the sLNvs are particularly important pacemakers of locomotor activity (Grima et al., 2004). Although the size and location of sLNvs make it much more difficult to obtain stable recordings, we obtained whole-cell recordings of 103 sLNvs in LD, each from one individual fly. As shown in Figure 7A, the RMP of some sLNvs exhibit 3∼10 mV oscillation, like lLNvs. Few sLNvs (18 of 103) fire spontaneous APs, and small synaptic transmission-like transients with an amplitude of 2–5 mV are found in 47% of sLNvs. The relationship of sLNv RMP to time of day in LD is plotted in Figure 7B. sLNv RMP is highest around lights-on. From ZT0 to ZT6, sLNv RMP exhibited a significant decrease (linear regression, p < 0.0001), and from ZT18 to ZT24, sLNv RMP showed a significant increase (linear regression, p = 0.018). However, from ZT6 to ZT18, the distribution of sLNv RMP had no significant nonzero slope (linear regression, p > 0.05) and was more variable. The peak of sLNv depolarization at lights-on is consistent with the temporal pattern of lLNv RMP.
Discussion
In this study, we investigated the temporal regulation of membrane excitability of PDF-expressing LNv clock neurons. We characterized RMP and AP firing patterns in lLNvs, and demonstrate that lLNv membrane excitability oscillates as a function of time of day in LD. We showed furthermore that this oscillation requires a functional circadian oscillator, as it is abolished in per0 flies. This requirement could be cell autonomous to the lLNvs, as suggested by the fact that the oscillation is abolished in DD, a condition in which lLNv, but not other clock neuron, transcriptional oscillation ceases. We found that sLNv RMP is most depolarized near lights-on.
lLNv spontaneous activity is somewhat variable, with 33% of cells silent in the light phase and 38% in the dark in LD. That, overall, 35% of lLNvs are silent in LD is slightly higher but still comparable with mammalian clock neurons in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) (Walsh et al., 1992; Kim and Dudek, 1993). Despite the 35% of silent lLNvs, the average AP firing frequency and the percentage of lLNvs that fire APs show a circadian temporal pattern similar to that in mammalian SCN, which is consistent with linear regression analysis of the scatter plot of individual cells (Fig. 4B). Interestingly, in contrast to the cyclic changes in lLNv RMP, membrane resistance and AP firing rate decrease from dawn to dusk in the day but stay low during the night in LD (Fig. 4B,C). The biophysical basis for this uncoupling of RMP from AP firing rate and membrane resistance at night is not clear, but presumably reflects differential modulation of particular ion channel subtypes. This issue bears further investigation in the future using physiological, pharmacological, and genetic techniques.
lLNvs exhibit two sizes of APs, with the small APs approximately half the amplitude of the large APs (Fig. 1C). Gap junction inhibitors reduce the frequency of small APs and alter the normal firing rate of large APs, suggesting that gap junctions are involved in modulating membrane excitability of lLNvs. This is consistent with observations of cockroach lateral neurons (Schneider and Stengl, 2006), where gap junction blockers also influence spontaneous AP firing rate, and clock neurons in mammalian SCN (Colwell, 2000; Shinohara et al., 2000; Long et al., 2005; Rash et al., 2007). However, none of the three gap junction blockers had any effect on the amplitude of either large or small APs, thus making it highly unlikely that APs generated in coupled cells are propagating passively to the site of recording at the soma through gap junctions. That cutting the POT removes small APs strongly supports that small APs originate in the contralateral optic lobes. This raises the interesting possibility that the ipsilateral and contralateral arbors of individual lLNvs can process information independently.
We demonstrated that WT lLNv membrane excitability is modulated as a function of time of day. In particular, in the light phase of LD, lLNv RMP becomes more hyperpolarized from dawn to dusk, as the AP firing rate and lLNv membrane resistance decrease. In the dark phase, lLNv RMP becomes more depolarized from the beginning to the end of the night, whereas the AP firing rate and membrane resistance stay relatively stable (Fig. 4). This continuous cyclical modulation of WT lLNv RMP is consistent with previous coarse time-scale studies demonstrating that lLNv RMP is more depolarized in the early morning and more hyperpolarized in the early night (Park and Griffith, 2006; Sheeba et al., 2008). This temporal modulation is almost completely abolished in per0 flies (Fig. 5), thus demonstrating for the first time its dependence on an intact transcriptional feedback oscillator. Although this is consistent with an autonomous requirement for transcriptional feedback oscillation in the lLNvs themselves, because per0 flies lack transcriptional feedback oscillation in all neurons, this could be a circuit-based phenomenon. To further address this question, we examined the modulation of WT lLNv excitability on the first day in DD conditions (DD-D1), in which transcriptional oscillation in lLNvs, but not in any other clock neurons, is abolished (Yang et al., 1998; Veleri et al., 2003; Grima et al., 2004; Nitabach et al., 2006; Wu et al., 2008). The absence of oscillatory modulation of lLNv excitability in DD-D1 further supports the conclusion that transcription feedback oscillation is required cell-autonomously in the lLNvs, and the transcriptional feedback oscillator directly modulates lLNv membrane excitability at the cellular level. The apparent difference between our demonstration of an absence of dependence of electrical excitability on time of day on DD-D1, and Sheeba et al.'s (2008) demonstration of presence of a dependence of electrical excitability on time of day on DD-D14 actually makes sense in light of what is known about the response of lLNvs to constant darkness. Whereas sLNvs and other clock neurons maintain their cellular rhythmicity immediately after transition from LD to DD conditions, the lLNvs immediately lose their cellular rhythms with transition into DD (Yang and Sehgal, 2001; Shafer et al., 2002), and only regain cellular rhythmicity after several days in DD (Peng et al., 2003; Stoleru et al., 2004). This immediate loss of lLNv cellular rhythmicity followed by resumption several days later perfectly accounts for the fact that we observed no rhythm of lLNv electrical excitability on DD-D1 whereas Sheeba et al. (2008) observed such a rhythm on DD-D14.
Either pdf gene mutation, LNv ablation, or LNv electrical silencing severely disrupts free-running locomotor rhythms (Renn et al., 1999; Nitabach et al., 2002), indicating that PDF-expressing LNvs play a critical role in controlling circadian behavior. Both sLNvs and lLNvs express PDF. However, previous studies suggest that of these two cell groups it is the sLNvs that are most important for controlling free-running rhythms in DD. In particular, restoration of per gene expression in sLNvs, but not lLNvs can rescue free-running circadian rhythmicity in per0 null mutant flies (Grima et al., 2004). Furthermore, as mentioned above, lLNv transcriptional feedback oscillation cease in DD (Yang and Sehgal, 2001; Shafer et al., 2002). The lLNvs have extensive dendritic arborizations in the medulla and project to contralateral lLNvs through the posterior optic tracts, thus enabling enable them to potentially receive light information and synchronize the two hemispheres (Helfrich-Forster and Homberg, 1993; Kaneko and Hall, 2000). The lLNvs also project to the accessory medulla (aMe), where the sLNvs have short dendrites that may be postsynaptic to lLNv projections (Helfrich-Forster et al., 2007). Although the functional role of lLNvs in circadian rhythms is mostly unknown, it is reasonable to speculate based on this anatomy that lLNvs relay environmental light information to the sLNvs via synapses formed in aMe. We thus propose that circadian modulation of lLNv membrane excitability in LD gates the transfer of entraining light information to the sLNvs, which more directly control circadian locomotor rhythms.
Because sLNvs are thought to be pacemakers of the neural circuit controlling circadian rhythms (Park et al., 2000; Yang and Sehgal, 2001; Grima et al., 2004), it is critical to understand how membrane excitability of sLNvs is regulated, and how sLNv membrane excitability is transformed to signals that control downstream neurons, and, ultimately, circadian locomotor activity. We recorded 103 sLNvs in LD and found that sLNv RMP is most depolarized around lights-on, with greater variability around lights-off (Fig. 7B). The two linear regression fits were selected to span the 6 h just before, and just after, lights-on (ZT0, ZT24). sLNv RMP exhibits significant trends of depolarization before lights-on and hyperpolarization after lights-on. It is clear that during the 12 h surrounding lights-off (ZT6–ZT18), there is much more variability in sLNv RMP. This difference makes some sense, as sLNvs are considered to be the “morning” cells that drive the increase in locomotor activity that anticipates lights-on (Grima et al., 2004; Stoleru et al., 2004). This would be consistent with an increase in sLNv electrical excitability in the hours preceding lights-on, and a decrease after lights-on, as we clearly observed. Because the sLNvs are not thought to be important for driving the increase in locomotor activity that anticipates lights-off (ZT12) (Grima et al., 2004; Stoleru et al., 2004), it is perhaps not surprising that there are no consistent trends in sLNV electrical excitability surrounding lights-off (ZT12). The reason sLNvs exhibit little spontaneous activity in this brain explant preparation is not clear, but it may reflect technical issues relating to the extremely small size of their soma. Previous studies indicate that the peak of anti-PDF staining in the sLNv nerve terminals is around lights-on, and the trough is around lights-off (Park et al., 2000; Nitabach et al., 2006). It is thus possible that the trough of PDF accumulation around lights-off represents depletion of the releasable pool of PDF starting in the early day, when sLNvs are most depolarized.
Our study, and that by Sheeba et al. (2008), demonstrate a dependence of lLNV electrical excitability on time of day in LD and after many days in DD, respectively. Our results go beyond those of Sheeba et al. (2008) in a number of important ways, including demonstration that the daily rhythm of lLNV electrical excitability depends on a functional cellular circadian oscillator, demonstration of a daily rhythm of electrical excitability in the sLNV pacemaker subset of clock neurons, and elucidation of the cellular origin of the bimodal distribution of sizes of lLNV APs. Future studies will determine the daily rhythms of electrical excitability of other functionally and anatomically identifiable subsets of fly clock neurons.
Footnotes
This work was supported in part by the Whitehall Foundation and National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grants R01NS055035 and R01NS056443 (M.N.N.). We thank Dr. Reiko Fitzimonds for the gift of equipments. We are grateful to members of the Nitabach Laboratory for their support and help, especially Dr. Ying Wu for her critical comments and discussion.
References
- Colwell CS. Rhythmic coupling among cells in the suprachiasmatic nucleus. J Neurobiol. 2000;43:379–388. doi: 10.1002/1097-4695(20000615)43:4<379::aid-neu6>3.0.co;2-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Jeu M, Hermes M, Pennartz C. Circadian modulation of membrane properties in slices of rat suprachiasmatic nucleus. NeuroReport. 1998;9:3725–3729. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199811160-00028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green DJ, Gillette R. Circadian rhythm of firing rate recorded from single cells in the rat suprachiasmatic brain slice. Brain Res. 1982;245:198–200. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90361-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grima B, Chelot E, Xia R, Rouyer F. Morning and evening peaks of activity rely on different clock neurons of the Drosophila brain. Nature. 2004;431:869–873. doi: 10.1038/nature02935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groos G, Hendriks J. Circadian rhythms in electrical discharge of rat suprachiasmatic neurones recorded in vitro. Neurosci Lett. 1982;34:283–288. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(82)90189-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hardin PE. The circadian timekeeping system of Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2005;15:R714–R722. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harmer SL, Panda S, Kay SA. Molecular bases of circadian rhythms. Annu Rev Cell Dev Biol. 2001;17:215–253. doi: 10.1146/annurev.cellbio.17.1.215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helfrich-Forster C. The circadian system of Drosophila melanogaster and its light input pathways. Zoology (Jena) 2002;105:297–312. doi: 10.1078/0944-2006-00074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helfrich-Forster C. The neuroarchitecture of the circadian clock in the brain of Drosophila melanogaster. Microsc Res Tech. 2003;62:94–102. doi: 10.1002/jemt.10357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helfrich-Forster C, Homberg U. Pigment-dispersing hormone-immunoreactive neurons in the nervous system of wild-type Drosophila melanogaster and of several mutants with altered circadian rhythmicity. J Comp Neurol. 1993;337:177–190. doi: 10.1002/cne.903370202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helfrich-Forster C, Tauber M, Park JH, Muhlig-Versen M, Schneuwly S, Hofbauer A. Ectopic expression of the neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor alters behavioral rhythms in Drosophila melanogaster. J Neurosci. 2000;20:3339–3353. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-09-03339.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helfrich-Forster C, Shafer OT, Wulbeck C, Greishaber E, Rieger D, Taghert P. Development and morphology of the clock-gene-expressing lateral neurons of Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Neurol. 2007;500:47–70. doi: 10.1002/cne.21146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Itri JN, Michel S, Vansteensel MJ, Meijer JH, Colwell CS. Fast delayed rectifier potassium current is required for circadian neural activity. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:650–656. doi: 10.1038/nn1448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaneko M, Hall JC. Neuroanatomy of cells expressing clock genes in Drosophila: transgenic manipulation of the period and timeless genes to mark the perikarya of circadian pacemaker neurons and their projections. J Comp Neurol. 2000;422:66–94. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(20000619)422:1<66::aid-cne5>3.0.co;2-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim YI, Dudek FE. Membrane properties of rat suprachiasmatic nucleus neurons receiving optic nerve input. J Physiol (Lond) 1993;464:229–243. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konopka RJ, Benzer S. Clock mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1971;68:2112–2116. doi: 10.1073/pnas.68.9.2112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhlman SJ, McMahon DG. Rhythmic regulation of membrane potential and potassium current persists in SCN neurons in the absence of environmental input. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;20:1113–1117. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03555.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Long MA, Jutras MJ, Connors BW, Burwell RD. Electrical synapses coordinate activity in the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:61–66. doi: 10.1038/nn1361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nitabach MN, Taghert PH. Organization of the Drosophila circadian control circuit. Curr Biol. 2008;18:R84–R93. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nitabach MN, Blau J, Holmes TC. Electrical silencing of Drosophila pacemaker neurons stops the free-running circadian clock. Cell. 2002;109:485–495. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(02)00737-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nitabach MN, Sheeba V, Vera DA, Blau J, Holmes TC. Membrane electrical excitability is necessary for the free-running larval Drosophila circadian clock. J Neurobiol. 2005;62:1–13. doi: 10.1002/neu.20053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nitabach MN, Wu Y, Sheeba V, Lemon WC, Strumbos J, Zelensky PK, White BH, Holmes TC. Electrical hyperexcitation of lateral ventral pacemaker neurons desynchronizes downstream circadian oscillators in the fly circadian circuit and induces multiple behavioral periods. J Neurosci. 2006;26:479–489. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3915-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park D, Griffith LC. Electrophysiological and anatomical characterization of PDF-positive clock neurons in the intact adult Drosophila brain. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95:3955–3960. doi: 10.1152/jn.00117.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park JH, Helfrich-Forster C, Lee G, Liu L, Rosbash M, Hall JC. Differential regulation of circadian pacemaker output by separate clock genes in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:3608–3613. doi: 10.1073/pnas.070036197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peng Y, Stoleru D, Levine JD, Hall JC, Rosbash M. Drosophila free-running rhythms require intercellular communication. PLoS Biol. 2003;1:e13. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0000013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennartz CM, de Jeu MT, Bos NP, Schaap J, Geurtsen AM. Diurnal modulation of pacemaker potentials and calcium current in the mammalian circadian clock. Nature. 2002;416:286–290. doi: 10.1038/nature728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitts GR, Ohta H, McMahon DG. Daily rhythmicity of large-conductance Ca2+-activated K+ currents in suprachiasmatic nucleus neurons. Brain Res. 2006;1071:54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rash JE, Olson CO, Pouliot WA, Davidson KG, Yasumura T, Furman CS, Royer S, Kamasawa N, Nagy JI, Dudek FE. Connexin36 vs connexin32, “miniature” neuronal gap junctions, and limited electrotonic coupling in rodent suprachiasmatic nucleus. Neuroscience. 2007;149:350–371. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.06.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renn SC, Park JH, Rosbash M, Hall JC, Taghert PH. A pdf neuropeptide gene mutation and ablation of PDF neurons each cause severe abnormalities of behavioral circadian rhythms in Drosophila. Cell. 1999;99:791–802. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81676-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosato E, Tauber E, Kyriacou CP. Molecular genetics of the fruit-fly circadian clock. Eur J Hum Genet. 2006;14:729–738. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider NL, Stengl M. Gap junctions between accessory medulla neurons appear to synchronize circadian clock cells of the cockroach Leucophaea maderae. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95:1996–2002. doi: 10.1152/jn.00835.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shafer OT, Rosbash M, Truman JW. Sequential nuclear accumulation of the clock proteins period and timeless in the pacemaker neurons of Drosophila melanogaster. J Neurosci. 2002;22:5946–5954. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-14-05946.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shafer OT, Helfrich-Forster C, Renn SC, Taghert PH. Reevaluation of Drosophila melanogaster's neuronal circadian pacemakers reveals new neuronal classes. J Comp Neurol. 2006;498:180–193. doi: 10.1002/cne.21021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheeba V, Gu H, Sharma VK, O'Dowd DK, Holmes TC. Circadian- and light-dependent regulation of resting membrane potential and spontaneous action potential firing of Drosophila circadian pacemaker neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99:976–988. doi: 10.1152/jn.00930.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinohara K, Hiruma H, Funabashi T, Kimura F. GABAergic modulation of gap junction communication in slice cultures of the rat suprachiasmatic nucleus. Neuroscience. 2000;96:591–596. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(99)00556-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoleru D, Peng Y, Agosto J, Rosbash M. Coupled oscillators control morning and evening locomotor behaviour of Drosophila. Nature. 2004;431:862–868. doi: 10.1038/nature02926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veleri S, Brandes C, Helfrich-Forster C, Hall JC, Stanewsky R. A self-sustaining, light-entrainable circadian oscillator in the Drosophila brain. Curr Biol. 2003;13:1758–1767. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2003.09.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh IB, van den Berg RJ, Marani E, Rietveld WJ. Spontaneous and stimulated firing in cultured rat suprachiasmatic neurons. Brain Res. 1992;588:120–131. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)91351-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu Y, Cao G, Nitabach MN. Electrical silencing of PDF neurons advances the phase of non-PDF clock neurons in Drosophila. J Biol Rhythms. 2008;23:117–128. doi: 10.1177/0748730407312984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang Z, Sehgal A. Role of molecular oscillations in generating behavioral rhythms in Drosophila. Neuron. 2001;29:453–467. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00218-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang Z, Emerson M, Su HS, Sehgal A. Response of the timeless protein to light correlates with behavioral entrainment and suggests a nonvisual pathway for circadian photoreception. Neuron. 1998;21:215–223. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80528-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]