Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Aug 13.
Published in final edited form as: Science. 2009 Feb 13;323(5916):951–953. doi: 10.1126/science.1167025

Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry

Meredith L Rowe 1,*, Susan Goldin-Meadow 1
PMCID: PMC2692106  NIHMSID: NIHMS89518  PMID: 19213922

Abstract

Children from low SES families, on average, arrive at school with smaller vocabularies than children from high SES families. In an effort to identify precursors to, and possible remedies for, this inequality, we videotaped 50 children from families ranging in SES interacting with parents at 14 months, and assessed their vocabulary skills at 54 months. We found that children from high SES families frequently used gesture to communicate at 14 months, a relation that was explained by parent gesture use (with speech controlled). In turn, the fact that children from high SES families have large vocabularies at 54 months was explained by children’s gesture use at 14 months. Differences in early gesture thus help explain the disparities in vocabulary children bring with them to school.

Keywords: SES, Vocabulary, Gesture, Language development


It has long been recognized that children from high SES (socioeconomic-status) families have, on average, larger vocabularies than children from low SES families (1). This SES gap in vocabulary size begins in the toddler years (2), widens until age four, and then remains relatively constant throughout the school years (3). Vocabulary is a key predictor of school success (4), and is a primary reason why low SES children enter school at greater risk for failure than their high SES peers (5). Early childhood is thus a critical educational period, as SES differences in language skills first emerge during these years (3, 6).

What is it about a family’s SES that leads to disparities in child vocabulary? Previous research suggests that the way parents talk to their children explains some of the relation between SES and child vocabulary (1, 79). On average, parents from higher SES groups talk more, use more diverse vocabulary, and use more complex syntax with their children than parents from lower SES groups, and these differences relate to child vocabulary development (2, 78, 1013).

Here we investigate another aspect of parent-child communication in relation to SES—parent and child gesture use. We know that children gesture to communicate before they use speech (1415). Further, there is a positive relation between parent gesture and child gesture (1619). And early child gesture predicts later child vocabulary, even controlling for early child speech (16, 20). We build on this prior work and ask: Are there SES differences in the way children and their parents use gesture? And, if so, might these differences help explain the robust relation between SES and child vocabulary skill?

To address these questions, we videotaped 14-month-old children from 50 families representing the demographic range of the Chicago area, engaging in their ordinary activities with their primary caregivers at home for 90 minutes. We transcribed all speech and gesture used by parent and child during the interaction to glean measures of spoken vocabulary and gesture use (see Supporting Material for details on sample and coding).

The number of gesture types, defined as the number of different meanings conveyed using gesture, served as our measure of child and parent gesture use (e.g., point at dog=dog). Previous research has found child gesture types to be a better predictor of later child spoken vocabulary size than child gesture frequency (16). At 14 months, children produced an average of 20.6 gesture types (SD=11.9). At child age 14 months, parents produced an average of 39.3 gesture types (SD=25.6).

The number of word types, defined as the number of different intelligible word roots produced by the speaker, served as our measure of spoken vocabulary. At 14 months children used an average of 13 word types during the interaction (SD=13.3). At child age 14 months, parents used an average of 364 word types (SD=132.0).

At child age 14 months, there was a positive relation between spoken word types and gesture types for both children (r=0.61, p<0.001) and parents (r=0.67, p<0.001). Further, parents who produced more gesture types had children who produced more gesture types (r=0.44, p<0.001). However, there was no relation between parent word types and child word types at this early stage of language production, nor was there a relation between parent word types and child gesture types.

On average, parents had 15.8 years of education (SD=2.2) and an average family income level of $60,400 (SD=$31,365). Family income and education were positively related to one another (r=0.44, p=0.001) and were combined into one variable (SES) using Principal Components Analysis (see Supporting Material for more information on SES measures).

SES differences in child and parent gesture use

SES was positively related to child gesture at 14 months (r=0.30, p<0.05) and to parent gesture at child age 14 months (r=0.45, p=0.001). Thus, SES differences are reflected in early parent-child gesture use. However, there was no relation between SES and child word types, although there was a positive relation between SES and parent word types (r=0.44, p=0.001).

Correlation and regression analyses were used to determine whether the positive relation between SES and children’s early gesture use is mediated by parents’ gesture use during interactions with their children. We followed guidelines for evaluating mediation models put forth by Baron and Kenny (21). Specifically, we use statistics to determine whether one variable explains a significant amount of the relation found between two other variables (see Supporting Material for more information on mediation analysis and assumptions).

The three scatterplots presented in the top of Figure 1 show that the necessary conditions for mediation were met. Panel (i) presents the significant relation between the predictor variable (SES) and the outcome variable (child gesture). Panel (ii) displays the significant relation between the predictor variable (SES) and the potential mediator variable (parent gesture). Panel (iii) displays the significant relation between the mediator variable (parent gesture) and the outcome variable (child gesture). The final necessary condition for mediation is that the significant relation between the predictor variable (SES) and the outcome variable (child gesture) must be reduced when the mediator variable (parent gesture) is included in the model. This effect is shown in the bottom portion of Figure 1 (iv): The relation between SES and child gesture (controlling for children’s word types at 14 months) is no longer significant when parent gesture is included in the model—the parameter estimate for the SES effect reduces to 1.85 (from 3.45). Bootstrapping procedures to test the significance of this indirect effect (i.e., the product of the coefficients comprising the mediated effect) (2223) gave a 95% confidence interval corrected for bias of 0.26−3.52, an interval that does not contain zero and thus indicates that the mediation effect is significant.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Scatter plots showing relations between (i) SES and Child Gesture at 14 months (top left), (ii) SES and Parent Gesture at child age 14 months (top middle), (iii) Parent Gesture and Child Gesture (top right), and (iv) analysis showing that Parent Gesture mediates the relation between SES and Child Gesture, controlling for Child Speech at 14 months (bottom), N=50. SES and Child Speech, taken together, explain 45% of the variation in Child Gesture; adding Parent Gesture explains 52%.

Importantly, we ran an additional model including parent word types at child age 14 months. In this model, parent gesture and child word types remained significant predictors of child gesture, but neither SES nor parent word types were significant predictors. Thus, the relation between parent gesture and child gesture holds even with parent talk controlled.

Gesture helps explain the relation between SES and child vocabulary

We now ask whether the relation between SES and later child vocabulary skill can be explained by children’s early gesture use. Child vocabulary skill was assessed at 54 months using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) (24). The average normed PPVT score for the sample was 109.8 (SD=18.1). The scatterplots in Figure 2 show that the first 3 conditions of mediation are met. (i) SES relates to children’s PPVT scores, r=0.55, p<0.001; (ii) SES relates to child gesture, r=0.30, p<0.05; and (iii) child gesture relates to PPVT scores, r=0.47, p=0.001. The bottom portion of Figure 2 shows the results of the mediation analysis (iv). The relation between SES and PPVT (controlling for children’s word types at 14 months) reduces in magnitude when child gesture is included in the model—the parameter estimate for the SES effect reduces to 8.02 (from 9.90). Bootstrapping procedures gave a 95% confidence interval corrected for bias of 0.44–4.51, an interval that does not contain zero and thus indicates that the mediation effect is significant. Thus, the effect of SES on child vocabulary at 54 months is mediated, in part, by children’s gesture use at 14 months.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Scatter plots showing relations between (i) SES and Child Vocabulary Skill (PPVT) at 54 months (top left), (ii) SES and Child Gesture at 14 months (top middle), and (iii) Child Gesture and Child Vocabulary Skill (top right), and (iv) analysis showing that Child Gesture mediates the relation between SES and Child Vocabulary Skill, controlling for Child Speech at 14 months (bottom), N=50. SES and Child Speech, taken together, explain 33% of the variation in Child Vocabulary Skill; adding Child Gesture explains 40%.

Overall, our findings are consistent with the following developmental history. When interacting with their children, parents from higher SES groups use gesture to communicate a broader range of meanings than parents from lower SES groups. By 14 months, children from these higher SES families are using gesture to communicate more meanings than children from lower SES families. Thus, as early as 14 months of age, children in different SES groups may be socialized to communicate more or fewer meanings via gesture. These early differences in gesture, in turn, help explain the large disparities in vocabulary size that characterize children of different SES groups at school entry.

It is striking that, in the initial stages of language learning when SES differences in children’s spoken vocabulary are not yet evident, we see SES differences in child gesture use. Children typically do not begin gesturing until around 10 months (1415). Thus, SES differences are evident a mere four months (and possibly even sooner) after the onset of child gesture production.

Why do we see SES differences in children’s early gestures? Although correlation does not imply causation or the direction of effects, our results implicate parent gesture as a plausible mechanism. Even before they produce their own gestures, children comprehend the gestures that others produce (2526). Since children from high SES backgrounds are exposed to a broader range of meanings in gesture than children from lower SES backgrounds (as shown here), they themselves are likely to produce more meanings in gesture, which then promotes the development of more vocabulary words in speech (27). This scenario parallels findings in speech—children from high SES backgrounds are exposed to more diverse vocabulary than children from lower SES backgrounds and, in turn, produce more vocabulary words of their own (2, 12). Thus, SES appears to relate to child vocabulary in two ways—through parent speech and through parent and child gesture.

We have shown here that variation in children’s early gesture use, independent of early spoken vocabulary, explains a portion of the robust relation between SES and later vocabulary skill. However, the specific nature of the relation between early child gesture and later child vocabulary is not addressed by this study. Child gesture could play an indirect role in word learning by eliciting timely speech from parents; for example, in response to her child’s point at the doll, mother might say, “yes, that’s a doll,” thus providing a word for the object that is the focus of the child’s attention. Indeed, Goldin-Meadow and colleagues (28) found that when mothers “translated” their child’s gestures into words in just this way, those words tended to become part of the child’s spoken vocabulary several months later. Gesture could also play a more direct role in word learning by giving children an opportunity to practice producing particular meanings by hand, at a time when those meanings are difficult to produce by mouth (27).

Whether or not early gesture plays a direct or indirect role in word learning, it is clear that gesturing partially accounts for the relation between SES and later vocabulary skill. Of course gesture is not the sole factor mediating the robust relation between SES and child vocabulary. Other environmental factors (including parent speech) and child factors are likely to influence child vocabulary as well. Nonetheless, given our findings, it seems fruitful for future research to explore whether parents and children can be encouraged to increase the rate at which they spontaneously gesture when they speak (as has been shown in older children, either by directly telling them to gesture (29) or by modeling gesture for them (30)). If so, the next step is to explore whether increases in gesturing lead to vocabulary gains in early childhood.

Footnotes

This manuscript has been accepted for publication in Science. This version has not undergone final editing. Please refer to the complete version of record at http://www.sciencemag.org/. The manuscript may not be reproduced or used in any manner that does not fall within the fair use provisions of the Copyright Act without the prior, written permission of AAAS

One-sentence summary: Differences in vocabulary that children from high vs. low socioeconomic families bring with them to school can be traced back to how the children gestured at 14 months, which, in turn, can be traced back to how the parents gestured to their children.

References and Notes

  • 1.Hoff E. Developmental Review. 2006;26:55–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hart B, Risley T. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Farkas G, Beron K. Social Science Research. 2004;33:464–497. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Anderson RC, Freebody P. In: Reading comprehension and education. Guthrie JT, editor. Newark, DE: International Reading Association; 1981. pp. 77–117. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Snow CE, Burns S, Griffin P. Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Duncan GJ, Yeung WJ, Brooks-Gunn J, Smith JR. American Sociological Review. 1998. pp. 406–423.
  • 7.Hoff E. Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 147–160. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hoff E. Child Development. 2003;74:1368–1378. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hoff E, Laursen B, Tardiff T. In: Handbook of parenting, Volume II: Ecology & biology of parenting. Bornstein MH, editor. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 161–188. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bornstein MH, Haynes MO, Painter KM. Journal of Child Language. 1998;25:367–393. doi: 10.1017/s0305000998003456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Heath SB. In: Cultural Psychology: Essays on comparative human development. Stigler JW, Shweder RA, Herdt G, editors. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1990. pp. 496–519. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Waterfall HR, Vevea JL, Hedges LV. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43:1062–1083. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Pan BA, Rowe ML, Singer JD, Snow CE. Child Development. 2005;76:763–782. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00876.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Bates E. Language and Context. Orlando: Academic Press; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bates E, Benigni L, Bretherton I, Camaioni L, Volterra V. The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press; 1979. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Rowe ML, Özçaliskan S, Goldin-Meadow S. First Language. 2008;28:182–199. doi: 10.1177/0142723707088310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Rowe ML. First Language. 2000;20:305–330. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Iverson JM, Capirci O, Longobardi E, Caselli MC. Cognitive Development. 1999;9:23–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Namy LL, Acredolo L, Goodwyn S. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 2000;24:63–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Rowe ML, Goldin-Meadow S. Developmental Science. 2009;12:182–187. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00764.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Baron RM, Kenny DA. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1986;51(6):1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.McCartney K, Burchinal MR, Bub KL. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2006;71(3) doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2006.07103001.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Preacher KJ, Hayes A. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 2004;36:717–731. doi: 10.3758/bf03206553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. 3rd edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Butterworth G, Grover L. In: Thought without language. Weiskrantz L, editor. Oxford: Carendon; 1988. pp. 5–24. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Carpenter M, Nagell K, Tomasello M. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1998;63(5) no. 255. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Iverson JM, Goldin-Meadow S. Psychological Science. 2005;16:368–371. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01542.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Goldin-Meadow S, Goodrich W, Sauer E, Iverson J. Developmental Science. 2007;10:778–785. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00636.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Broaders S, Cook SW, Mitchell Z, Goldin-Meadow S. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2007;136:539–550. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cook S, Goldin-Meadow S. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2006;7:211–232. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Supported by NICHD: K99HD055522 to MLR, P01HD40605 to SGM. We thank J.Huttenlocher, S.Levine, W.Meadow, J.Rowe, and three anonymous reviewers for comments on earlier versions; the families for their participation; K.Brasky, L.Chang, E.Croft, K.Duboc, J.Griffin, S.Gripshover, K.Harden, L.King, C.Meanwell, E.Mellum, M.Nikolas, J.Oberholtzer, C.Rousch, L.Rissman, B.Seibel, M.Simone, K.Uttich, J.Wallman for data collection/transcription; E.Sauer for gesture coding; K.Schonwald, J.Voigt for administrative/technical assistance.

RESOURCES