Abstract
Visual search for a target object among distractors often takes longer when more distractors are present. To understand the neural basis of this capacity limitation, we recorded activity from visually responsive neurons in the frontal eye field (FEF) of macaque monkeys searching for a target among distractors defined by form (randomly oriented T or L). To test the hypothesis that the delay of response time with increasing number of distractors originates in the delay of attention allocation by FEF neurons, we manipulated the number of distractors presented with the search target. When monkeys were presented with more distractors, visual target selection was delayed and neuronal activity was reduced in proportion to longer response time. These findings indicate that the time taken by FEF neurons to select the target contributes to the variation in visual search efficiency.
INTRODUCTION
Because only some of all the information entering the visual system is relevant for acting in a given environment, attention must be allocated to selectively process one among many alternatives, especially when those alternatives are difficult to distinguish. This has been investigated using visual search for a target stimulus in an array of distractor stimuli (Bergen and Julesz 1983; Carrasco and Yeshurun 1998; Duncan and Humphreys 1989; Palmer 1994; Treisman and Gelade 1980; Wolfe 2007). The observation that visual search response time (RT) often increases with the number of distractors has been central in theories of attention (Bundesen 1990; Bundesen et al. 2005; Desimone and Duncan 1995; Duncan and Humphreys 1989; Treisman and Gelade 1980; Wolfe 2007). Previous work has shown that brain areas in the macaque visuomotor system have distinct classes of neurons involved in different stages of visual search. Visual and visuomovement neurons in the frontal eye field (FEF) (Schall and Hanes 1993; Thompson et al. 1996), lateral intraparietal area (LIP) (Bisley and Goldberg 2003; Ipata et al. 2006; Thomas and Paré 2007), and superior colliculus (SC) (McPeek and Keller 2002) participate in the visual selection of target objects by increasing activity when the target is inside the neuron's receptive field (RF) relative to when a distractor is in the RF. Other neurons in these areas participate in preparing eye movement responses (Bruce and Goldberg 1985; Hanes and Schall 1996; Segraves and Goldberg 1987; Sommer and Wurtz 1998; Sparks et al. 1976; Wurtz and Goldberg 1971). Neurophysiological recordings from nonhuman primates can distinguish between competing accounts of the locus of increases in RT with increased set size because the activity of different types of neurons reveals the timing of different stages of processing (Sato et al. 2001; Sternberg 2001; Woodman et al. 2008).
To determine the neural locus of processing delays with increasing search set sizes, we measured the activity of visually responsive neurons in FEF during a demanding visual search task. The activity of these neurons discriminates between target and distractor, thus measuring the time of target selection, which corresponds to attention allocation (Bichot and Schall 1999; Juan et al. 2004; Murthy et al. 2001; Schall 2004; Thompson et al. 1997, 2005). We trained two monkeys to search for a target stimulus among one, three, or seven distractor stimuli (set sizes 2, 4, and 8) in a T/L search display (Fig. 1A). Monkeys fixated a spot at the center of the display and received a liquid reward for producing a single saccadic eye movement to the target (in Fig. 1A, the L). Across experimental sessions, the monkeys alternated between searching for Ts or Ls among randomly rotated distractors. This visual search task results in longer RT with larger set size in human observers (Bergen and Julesz 1983; Carrasco and Yeshurun 1998; Duncan and Humphreys 1989; Palmer 1994; Treisman and Gelade 1980; Wolfe 2007). We found that neurons selected search targets later and discharged fewer spikes when presented with more distractors.
METHODS
Behavioral task and recording
Activity of FEF neurons was recorded in both hemispheres of two male macaques (Macaca radiata) performing memory-guided saccade and visual search tasks. Recordings were acquired from the rostral bank of the arcuate sulcus using tungsten microelectrodes (FHC). In the search task, monkeys searched for a target (T or L) among distractors (L or T) (Woodman et al. 2007). Distractors could be homogeneous (e.g., upright Ls) or heterogeneous (e.g., Ls oriented differently). Each trial began with monkeys fixating a central spot for about 600 ms. A target was then presented at one of eight isoeccentric locations equally spaced around the fixation spot (Fig. 1A). The other seven locations contained seven distractor stimuli, three distractors, or one distractor. During each recording session, the target was a T or an L rotated by 0, 90, 180, or 270°. Monkeys were given a liquid reward for making a saccade to the target location and fixating it for 1,000 ms.
Activity from each neuron was recorded during a memory-guided saccade task to distinguish visual- from movement-related activity (Bruce and Goldberg 1985; Hikosaka and Wurtz 1983). The target was flashed alone for 80 ms. Monkeys were required to maintain fixation for 500–1,000 ms after the target onset. When the fixation spot disappeared, the monkey was rewarded for a saccade to the remembered location of the target.
Our data set consists of 57 neurons from monkey Q and 26 neurons from monkey S. Spikes were sorted on-line and off-line using principal components analysis and template matching (Plexon, Dallas, TX).
Monkeys were surgically implanted with a head post, a subconjunctival eye coil, and recording chambers. Surgery was conducted under aseptic conditions with animals under isoflurane anesthesia. Antibiotics and analgesics were administered postoperatively. All surgical and experimental procedures were in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and approved by the Vanderbilt Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Data analysis
To measure the firing rate of each neuron, we used a spike density function, convolving each spike with a kernel resembling a postsynaptic potential (Thompson et al. 1996).
We used a memory-guided saccade task to classify neurons. Visual neurons had significantly greater activity in the 100 ms after the target flash than in the 100 ms before the target flash. Movement neurons had greater responses in the 100 ms leading up to the saccade than in the 100 ms before the target flash. Visuomovement neurons had greater responses in the 100 ms after the target flash and in the 100 ms leading up to the saccade than in the 100 ms before the target flash. Neurons analyzed in this study were visual and visuomovement and had significant above-baseline activity during the memory delay.
To measure the time of target selection we used millisecond-by-millisecond Wilcoxon rank-sum tests. Selection time is defined as the time at which the distribution of activity when the search target was inside a neuron's RF was significantly greater than the distribution of activity when the target was opposite the RF for ten consecutive milliseconds with P < 0.01. This “neuron–antineuron” approach presumes that a population of neurons in the brain representing the location of the target competes with a population of neurons representing the location of distractors opposite the target. Measuring selection time with a receiver operating characteristic analysis (Thompson et al. 1996) yielded similar results. There was no difference in target selection time between visual and visuomovement neurons.
All statistical tests were done with Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons. All analyses were done with R (http://www.r-project.org/).
RESULTS
Behavior
Both monkeys exhibited a consistent and strong effect of set size on RT (Fig. 1B). Across sessions RT was significantly longer for set size 8 compared with set size 4 and for set size 4 compared with set size 2 and, within sessions, RT increased significantly with set size in 100% of sessions (Table 1). The slopes of RT by set size corresponded to an increase of 22 ms per additional distractor for monkey Q and 10 ms per additional distractor for monkey S. Percentage error (moving the eyes to a distractor) was low for both monkeys and increased with set size only for monkey S (Fig. 1B; Wilcoxon signed-rank test, P < 0.001). These results indicate that the monkeys consistently emphasized accuracy over speed.
TABLE 1.
Values are means±SE. Pairwise comparisons are Wilcoxon signed-rank tests with significant differences highlighted:
**for P < 0.001,
*for P < 0.05, and N.S. for not significant. The sample sizes are 57 for monkey Q and 26 for monkey S.
Set-size effects on target selection time
We recorded the activity of 57 visually responsive FEF neurons from monkey Q and 26 from monkey S that selected the target in the visual search task. This selection has been identified with the allocation of attention (Bichot et al. 2001; Schall 2004; Thompson and Bichot 2005; Thompson et al. 2005). Each neuron exhibited visual responses and sustained activity during the delay period of a memory-guided saccade task. Target selection time measures the time at which a neuron's activity on trials with the search target inside its RF exceeded activity on trials with a distractor in its RF (and the target opposite its RF). To determine how variation of target selection time related to variation of RT, we aligned neuronal responses to the time of presentation of the search array and measured target selection time for each set size.
Figure 2 shows the average response of a neuron when the target versus a distractor appeared in the RF for each set size, aligned to the onset of the search array. The neuron selected the target after 160 ms when one distractor was present (Fig. 2A, vertical line), after 177 ms when three distractors were present (Fig. 2B), and after 230 ms when seven distractors were present (Fig. 2C). The delay of the target selection corresponded with the delay of the RT (set size 2 mean RT ± SE, 219 ± 4.2 ms; set size 4, 265 ± 7.8 ms; set size 8, 386 ± 12.3 ms). Median RT for each set size is indicated by the black arrowhead in each panel.
Neurons selected the target later when confronted with larger set sizes (Fig. 3, Table 1). Comparing across neurons, target selection time was significantly later for set size 8 than for set sizes 2 or 4 (Kruskal–Wallis rank-sum test, P < 0.01). Comparing within neurons, target selection time was significantly later for set size 4 compared with set size 2 for monkey S and was significantly later for set size 8 than for set size 4 and for set size 8 than for set size 2 for both monkeys.
We measured the relationship between RT and selection time by computing a linear regression for each set size (Table 1). The regression for monkey Q was significant for set size 8 (solid gray line in Fig. 4C). For monkey S, regression was significant for set size 4 (solid line in Fig. 4B) and set size 8 (solid black line in Fig. 4C). Combined across monkeys, regression was significant for set size 8, with a slope of 0.79 ± 0.16 (P < 0.001) and intercept of 189 ± 31 ms (P < 0.001). That is, for set size 8, a 10 ms increase in selection time corresponded to an approximately 8 ms increase in RT. Thus target selection time of FEF neurons accounts for a significant amount of variance in RT for large set sizes.
Set-size effects on firing rate
We measured peak mean firing rate for each neuron from search array onset until saccade onset when the search target was inside the neuron's RF. For the example neuron in Fig. 2, the dashed horizontal lines indicate peak mean firing rate for set size 2. Peak firing rate decreased from 72 to 64 to 56 spikes/s for set sizes 2, 4, and 8, respectively. Across the population of neurons, peak firing rate decreased with increasing set size (Fig. 5, Table 1). Firing rate was significantly higher for set size 2 than that for set size 4 for monkey Q. Firing rate was significantly higher for set size 2 than that for set size 8 and for set size 4 than that for set size 8 for both monkeys. Thus increasing set size decreases peak firing rate in FEF.
DISCUSSION
We discovered that neurons in FEF that select the location of the target in a visual search array do so later with fewer spikes when presented with progressively more, complex distractors. This result complements earlier observations that the time course of target selection by FEF neurons is related to the visual similarity between targets and distractors in singleton and conjunction search (Bichot et al. 1999, 2001; Sato et al. 2001; Thompson et al. 1996). The target selection process first described in FEF (Schall and Hanes 1993) has now been observed in posterior parietal cortex (Constantinidis and Steinmetz 2001; Ipata et al. 2006; Thomas and Paré 2007) and SC (McPeek and Keller 2002). These results are consistent with the hypothesis that a population of neurons in FEF, LIP, and SC can be identified with the salience map in models of visual search and attention (Bundesen 2005; Itti and Koch 2000; Thompson and Bichot 2005; Wolfe 2007). These neurons represent the extent to which a stimulus in their RF is the focus of attention. The target selection process marks the outcome of visual processing and the covert allocation of attention to the target that precedes overt responses (Schall 2004; Thompson and Bichot 2005). We found that RT varied with selection time as if the saccade could not be produced until the target was located. This occurs because the onset of activation of presaccadic movement neurons in FEF is delayed when visual search is less efficient, yielding longer RT (Woodman et al. 2008). In other words, under the conditions of our experiments, preparation of the eye-movement response does not begin until perceptual processing of the visual array is completed. Note that although target selection time in FEF accounted for a large amount of variation of RT, other sources also contributed to the variation in RT; the major source is the postselection stochastic process of saccade preparation (Woodman et al. 2008).
Theories of visual attention propose that the cause of longer RT with increased set size arises because of the limited capacity of the visual system to simultaneously analyze all elements in a visual search array (Bundesen 1990; Bundesen et al. 2005; Desimone and Duncan 1995; Duncan and Humphreys 1989; Treisman and Gelade 1980; Wolfe 2007). Such theories predict that a neural index of the allocation of attention to the target is delayed when more nontarget stimuli must be processed. The time at which visually responsive neurons in FEF signal the location of the target provides a neurochronometric measure of the conclusion of perceptual analysis (Juan et al. 2004; Murthy et al. 2001; Schall 2004; Thompson et al. 1997, 2005). According to some theories of attention, such as Treisman's Feature Integration Theory (Treisman and Gelade 1980) and Wolfe's Guided Search (Wolfe 2007), target selection maps onto a mechanism directing attention in the visual field. Our measure of selection time indicates when, on average, attention is reliably focused on the target. This time is later with larger set sizes because attention shifts more often to one or more distractors before focusing on the target. According to other theories of attention, such as Bundesen's Theory of Visual Attention (TVA) (Bundesen 1990; Bundesen et al. 2005), attention is deployed to all of the possible targets simultaneously and eventually selects an object when it enters short-term memory. According to TVA, the neurochronometric measure of target selection would mark the time at which perceptual processing is completed and the target representation is transferred into memory. Because FEF activity has been associated with attention and working memory (Funahashi et al. 1989) both of these theoretical explanations of our findings are tenable and distinguishing between them is a goal for future research.
The reduced discharge rate with larger stimulus arrays has been observed previously (Balan et al. 2008) and described in terms of increased uncertainty (Basso and Wurtz 1998), number of alternatives (Lee and Keller 2008), or RF surround suppression (Schall et al. 1995a). This may underlie the capacity limitation of visual search with increasing number of distractors (Desimone and Duncan 1995; Palmer 1994; Treisman and Gelade 1990). Lower discharge rates among the neurons representing the target and distractors result in less dynamic range for discriminating between the alternative representations. Thus more time is taken to achieve a given signal-to-noise ratio (Bichot et al. 2001; Shadlen et al. 1996). This is consistent with accounts of variation of search efficiency in terms of signal detection theory (Palmer et al. 2000). Confronted with stimulus ambiguity and armed with a target template, a top-down influence can bias processing of stimuli that resemble the target (Bundesen 2005; Desimone and Duncan 1995; Sato et al. 2003). The dense reciprocity of connectivity between FEF and extrastriate visual cortex (Schall et al. 1995b) makes it difficult to distinguish the respective contributions of activity in areas like V4 and MT signaling the features of search items (Ogawa and Komatsu 2004) and activation in FEF feeding back to influence extrastriate areas (Hamker 2005; Moore and Armstrong 2003). This basic question may be resolved in future studies that directly compare the timing of signals across areas.
In contrast to what we found in FEF, a recent study of area LIP in macaques performing a different visual search task reported that the time for target selection did not vary with set size (Balan et al. 2008). We believe that these contradictory findings are unlikely to be due to differences between LIP and FEF and instead can be attributed to differences in performance. The effect of set size on RT was larger in our study of FEF than it was in the study of LIP. For example, we obtained a significant increase of RT with set size in 100% of the experimental sessions, compared with roughly 70% of the sessions reported in the LIP study. Also, we obtained a variation of RT relative to the overall mean RT from the largest (8) to the smallest (2) set size of 25–43%; the study of LIP found a ratio from the largest (6) to the smallest (2) set size of around 10%. Finally, the error rates in our experiment did not vary substantially with set size; the error rates in the LIP study increased with set size. Consequently, it seems most plausible that we observed the variation of RT with neural selection time because we obtained a larger variation of RT against which to perform the regression.
In summary, when presented with complex search arrays containing more distractors, FEF neurons, as part of a distributed network, take longer to locate the target because the representations of both target and distractors are weaker. This provides a mechanistic basis for the capacity limitation that is expressed when visual inputs compete for representation.
GRANTS
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants T32-MH-064913, R01-EY-08890, P30-EY-08126, and P30-HD-015052, and Robin and Richard Patton through the E. Bronson Ingram Chair in Neuroscience.
Acknowledgments
We thank G. Logan, T. Palmeri, R. Marois, M. Paré, M. Wallace, and B. Purcell for helpful discussions.
The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. The article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
REFERENCES
- Balan 2008.Balan PF, Oristaglio J, Schneider DM, Gottlieb J. Neuronal correlates of the set-size effect in monkey lateral intraparietal area. PLoS Biol 6: e158, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basso 1998.Basso MA, Wurtz RH. Modulation of neuronal activity in superior colliculus by changes in target probability. J Neurosci 18: 7519–7534, 1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergen 1983.Bergen JR, Julesz B. Parallel versus serial processing in rapid pattern discrimination. Nature 303: 696–698, 1983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bichot 1999.Bichot NP, Schall JD. Effects of similarity and history on neural mechanisms of visual selection. Nat Neurosci 2: 549–554, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bichot 2001.Bichot NP, Thompson KG, Rao SC, Schall JD. Reliability of frontal eye field neurons signaling saccade targets during visual search. J Neurosci 21: 713–725, 2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bisley 2003.Bisley JW, Goldberg ME. Neuronal activity in the lateral intraparietal area and spatial attention. Science 299: 81–86, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruce 1985.Bruce CJ, Goldberg ME. Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. J Neurophysiol 53: 603–635, 1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bundesen 1990.Bundesen C A theory of visual attention. Psychol Rev 97: 523–547, 1990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bundesen 2005.Bundesen C, Habekost T, Kyllingsbæk S. A neural theory of visual attention: bridging cognition and neurophysiology. Psychol Rev 112: 291–328, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrasco 1998.Carrasco M, Yeshurun Y. The contribution of covert attention to the set-size and eccentricity effects in visual search. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24: 673–692, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Constantinidis 2001.Constantinidis C, Steinmetz MA. Neuronal responses in area 7a to multiple-stimulus displays: I. Neurons encode the location of the salient stimulus. Cereb Cortex 11: 581–591, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desimone 1995.Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 18: 193–222, 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan 1989.Duncan J, Humphreys GW. Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychol Rev 96: 433–458, 1989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Funahashi 1989.Funahashi S, Bruce CJ, Goldman-Rakic PS. Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 61: 331–349, 1989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamker 2005.Hamker FH The reentry hypothesis: the putative interaction of the frontal eye field, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, and areas V4, IT for attention and eye movement. Cereb Cortex 15: 431–447, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanes 1996.Hanes DP, Schall JD. Neural control of voluntary movement initiation. Science 274: 427–430, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hikosaka 1983.Hikosaka O, Wurtz RH. Visual and oculomotor functions of monkey substantia nigra pars reticulata. III. Memory-contingent visual and saccade responses. J Neurophysiol 49: 1268–1284, 1983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ipata 2006.Ipata AE, Gee AL, Goldberg ME, Bisley JW. Activity in the lateral intraparietal area predicts the goal and latency of saccades in a free-viewing visual search task. J Neurosci 26: 3656–3661, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Itti 2000.Itti L, Koch C. A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Res 40: 1489–1506, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juan 2004.Juan C-H, Shorter-Jacobi SM, Schall JD. Dissociation of spatial attention and saccade preparation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 15541–15544, 2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee 2008.Lee KM, Keller EL. Neural activity in the frontal eye fields modulated by the number of alternatives in target choice. J Neurosci 28: 2242–2251, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McPeek 2002.McPeek RM, Keller EL. Saccade target selection in the superior colliculus during a visual search task. J Neurophysiol 88: 2019–2034, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore 2003.Moore T, Armstrong KM. Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex. Nature 421: 370–373, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murthy 2001.Murthy A, Thompson KG, Schall JD. Dynamic dissociation of visual selection from saccade programming in frontal eye field. J Neurophysiol 86: 2634–2637, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogawa 2004.Ogawa T, Komatsu H. Target selection in area V4 during a multidimensional visual search task. J Neurosci 24: 6371–6382, 2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer 1994.Palmer J Set-size effects in visual search: the effect of attention is independent of the stimulus for simple tasks. Vision Res 34: 1703–1721, 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer 2000.Palmer J, Verghese P, Pavel M. The psychophysics of visual search. Vision Res 40: 1227–1268, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sato 2001.Sato T, Murthy A, Thompson KG, Schall JD. Search efficiency but not response interference affects visual selection in frontal eye field. Neuron 30: 583–591, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sato 2003.Sato TR, Watanabe K, Thompson KG, Schall JD. Effect of target-distractor similarity on FEF visual selection in the absence of the target. Exp Brain Res 151: 356–363, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schall 2004.Schall JD On the role of the frontal eye field in guiding attention and saccades. Vision Res 44: 1453–1467, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schall 1993.Schall JD, Hanes DP. Neural basis of saccade target selection in frontal eye field during visual search. Nature 366: 467–469, 1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schall 1995a.Schall JD, Hanes DP, Thompson KG, King DJ. Saccade target selection in frontal eye field of macaque. I. Visual and premovement activation. J Neurosci 15: 6905–6918, 1995a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schall 1995b.Schall JD, Morel A, King DJ, Bullier J. Topography of visual cortex connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and segregation of processing streams. J Neurosci 15: 4464–4487, 1995b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segraves 1987.Segraves MA, Goldberg ME. Functional properties of corticotectal neurons in the monkey's frontal eye field. J Neurophysiol 58: 1387–1419, 1987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadlen 1996.Shadlen MN, Britten KH, Newsome WT, Movshon JA. A computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and behavioral responses to visual motion. J Neurosci 16: 1486–1510, 1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sommer 1998.Sommer MA, Wurtz RH. Frontal eye field neurons orthodromically activated from the superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 80: 3331–3335, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparks 1976.Sparks DL, Holland R, Guthrie BL. Size and distribution of movement fields in the monkey superior colliculus. Brain Res 113: 21–34, 1976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg 2001.Sternberg S Separate modifiability, mental modules, and the use of pure and composite measures to reveal them. Acta Psychol 106: 147–246, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas 2007.Thomas NW, Paré M. Temporal processing of saccade targets in parietal cortex area LIP during visual search. J Neurophysiol 97: 942–947, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson 2005.Thompson KG, Bichot NP. A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. Prog Brain Res 147: 251–262, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson 1997.Thompson KG, Bichot NP, Schall JD. Dissociation of visual discrimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye field. J Neurophysiol 77: 1046–1050, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson 2005.Thompson KG, Biscoe KL, Sato TR. Neuronal basis of covert spatial attention in the frontal eye field. J Neurosci 25: 9479–9487, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson 1996.Thompson KG, Hanes DP, Bichot NP, Schall JD. Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. J Neurophysiol 76: 4040–4055, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treisman 1980.Treisman AM, Gelade G. A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognit Psychol 12: 97–136, 1980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe 2007.Wolfe JM Guided search 4.0: current progress with a model of visual search. In: Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems, edited by Gray W. New York: Oxford Univ. Press, 2007.
- Woodman 2007.Woodman GF, Kang M-S, Rossi AF, Schall JD. Nonhuman primate event-related potentials indexing covert shifts of attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci 104: 15111–15116, 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woodman 2008.Woodman GF, Kang M-S, Thompson K, Schall JD. The effect of visual search efficiency on response preparation: neurophysiological evidence for discrete flow. Psychol Sci 19: 128–136, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wurtz 1971.Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME. Superior colliculus cell responses related to eye movements in awake monkeys. Science 171: 82–84, 1971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]