Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Brain Behav Immun. 2008 Dec 25;23(3):389–395. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2008.12.005

Increased IgG4 levels in children with autism disorder

Amanda Enstrom 1,5, Paula Krakowiak 2,5, Charity Onore 1,5, Isaac N Pessah 3,5, Irva Hertz-Picciotto 2,5, Robin L Hansen 3,5, Judy A Van de Water 1,5, Paul Ashwood 1,5,*
PMCID: PMC2696343  NIHMSID: NIHMS102881  PMID: 19136055

Abstract

Accumulating evidence indicates that immune dysfunction is associated with autism disorders in a significant subset of children. Previous reports have shown abnormal immunoglobulin (Ig) levels, including an increased presence of autoreactive antibodies in the circulation of individuals with autism. As IgG is the predominant antibody isotype in circulation, we expected that an altered immune response could result in an abnormal IgG subclass profile in children with autism. We examined circulating plasma levels of IgG1, IgG2, IgG3, and IgG4 in 241 children from the CHARGE (Childhood Autism Risks from Genetics and the Environment) study, a large epidemiologic case-control investigation, including 114 children who meet full criteria for autism disorder (AU), 96 typically developing control children (TD) from a randomly selected sample of the general population, and 31 children with developmental delays (DD). We report significantly increased levels of the IgG4 subclass in children with AU compared with TD control children (p=0.016) and compared with DD controls (p=0.041). These results may suggest an underlying immunological abnormality in AU subjects resulting in elevated IgG4 production. Further investigation is necessary to elucidate the relationship between immunological findings and behavioral impairments in autism.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorders are a group of relatively common pervasive neurodevelopmental disorders affecting between 2-6 children per 1000 (Chakrabarti and Fombonne, 2005; Fombonne, 2005; Yeargin-Allsopp et al., 2003). Autism presents early in childhood and is characterized by deficits in verbal and nonverbal communication, restricted and repetitive stereotypical patterns of behavior, and impairments in social interactions. The etiology of autism is unknown with both environmental and genetic factors considered to play key roles. Multiple genetic studies of children and adults with autism have indicated various candidate genes, many of them related to immune system function, including C4 null allele and MHC haplotypes B44-SC30-DR4, HLA-DR β1, DR4, and DR13 (Daniels et al., 1995; Ferrante et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2006; Torres et al., 2006; Warren et al., 1995). Other genes that have important roles in regulating the developing immune system, such as MET, PTEN and RELN have also been implicated in autism.(Beilmann et al., 1997; Butler et al., 2005; Garbett et al., 2008; Pardo and Eberhart, 2007; Skaar et al., 2005). Environmental triggers, such as thalidomide, valproic acid, and prenatal and early postnatal infections have also been associated with autism (Ashwood et al., 2006; Fortier et al., 2007; Meyer et al., 2006; Miyazaki et al., 2005; Narita et al., 2002; Niehus and Lord, 2006; Rodier et al., 1997; Rosen et al., 2007; Shi et al., 2003; Vojdani et al., 2003; Williams et al., 2001). Evidence includes increased rates of infection in neonates who were later diagnosed with autism, (Rosen et al., 2007), and links with congenital rubella, cytomegalovirus and other viral infections (Chess, 1971; Stubbs et al., 1984). In addition, several researchers have described various immune abnormalities, including increased levels of inflammatory mediators and the presence of autoimmune phenomena (Ashwood et al., 2006; Cohly and Panja, 2005; Korvatska et al., 2002). Taken together, these findings suggest that immunological imbalances or alterations during critical neurodevelopmental junctures could be important in a significant subset of children with autism.

Descriptions of aberrant immune function in individuals with autism have been reported for nearly 40 years. Among these findings there has been a long standing focus on autoimmune processes in individuals with autism and/or their primary and secondary family members compared with the general population (Ashwood and Van de Water, 2004; Cohly and Panja, 2005; Comi et al., 1999; Money et al., 1971). Reports of self-reactive antibodies, especially to brain and CNS proteins (Cabanlit et al., 2007; Connolly et al., 2006; Connolly et al., 1999; Kozlovskaia et al., 2000; Silva et al., 2004; Singh and Rivas, 2004; Singh et al., 1993; Todd et al., 1988; Vojdani et al., 2002; Vojdani et al., 2004; Wills et al., 2007), have been one of the most robust and consistently demonstrated immunological findings in autism. Furthermore, depending on the study, an estimated 25-70% of children with autism have antibodies that are reactive to neuronal proteins (Connolly et al., 1999; Singh et al., 1997). Their presence may indicate that a potential breakdown in the regulatory network which controls the immune response has occurred.

Immunoglobulin (Ig) G is the most prevalent antibody isotype in human circulation and consists of four subclasses. Each subclass of IgG has different biological properties. IgG1 and IgG3 are predominately responsible for protection against re-infection based on their ability to activate complement, which induces platelet binding and clearance of infectious agents from the body. In contrast, IgG2 and IgG4 do not bind complement. IgG4 is univalent like IgE and has been implicated in allergy development. IgG4 is also thought to act as a blocking antibody, produced in response to chronic exposure as a means of immune regulation (Aalberse and Schuurman, 2002).

The clinical implications of skewed antibody subclasses are unclear. Croonenberghs et al. demonstrated that increased serum IgG2 and IgG4 concentrations were present in children with autism and were associated with certain behavioral outcomes (Croonenberghs et al., 2002). We compared IgG subclass levels among three groups of children; those who met the cut-offs for autistic disorder (AU), those with typical development and those with developmentally delay. We also evaluated whether IgG subclass levels were associated with several clinical standardized measures of behavior and child neurodevelopment in both AU and control children.

Methods

Study Design

Children in this project were participants in the Childhood Autism Risk from Genetics and the Environment (CHARGE) study, a large ongoing population-based case-control investigation being conducted at the University of California, Davis (Hertz-Picciotto et al., 2006). To be eligible for the CHARGE study, children must be between the ages of 24 and 60 months, born in California, living with their biological parents who speak English or Spanish, and residing within a defined catchment area. Children are recruited from three groups; children diagnosed with autism, diagnosed with developmental delay, or children sampled from the general population. Cases from the first two groups are identified from the California Department of Developmental Services Regional Center system that co-ordinates services for persons with developmental disabilities. The third group (controls) is sampled from birth certificate files with frequency matching by child's age, gender, and broad geographic area to the projected distribution of these factors in the autism case group. Children are evaluated to confirm or preclude developmental diagnoses by trained staff at the Medical Investigations of Neurodevelopmental Disorders (M.I.N.D.) Institute at the University of California, Davis. Standardized instruments considered to be gold-standard for diagnosing autism were administered, and along with standard assessments of cognitive and adaptive function, are described below. Further details on study design, recruitment, and data and specimen collection protocols are described in greater detail elsewhere (Hertz-Picciotto et al., 2006).

Participants in this project were 241 children recruited between March 2003 and August 2006, including 114 children diagnosed with AU based on gold standard diagnostic assessments, 96 typically developing healthy (TD) controls from the general population and 31 children with developmental delays but not an autism spectrum disorder (DD). Samples were selected based on available volumes of plasma from consecutively recruited participants. Demographic data are detailed in Table 1. Consent was obtained from a parent for each study participant. This study was conducted with the approval of the institutional review boards of the UC Davis and the State of California.

Table 1.

Participant demographic and diagnostic information. Data shown as median (interquartile range).

Autism (n=114) TD (n=96) DD (n=31)
Age, yrs * 3.47 (3.01–4.31) 3.21 (2.77–3.84) 3.57 (2.97–4.52)
Sex, n (%) male * 114 (92.1%) 72 (75.0%) 19 (61.3%)
Race/ethnicity
 White 60 (52.6%) 47 (49.0%) 16 (51.6%)
 Hispanic 32 (28.1%) 34 (35.4%) 8 (25.8%)
 Mixed or other ** 22 (19.3%) 15 (15.6%) 7 (22.6%)
Mother's education level *
 High school or less 15 (13.3%) 21 (21.9%) 4 (12.9%)
 Some college/vocational 55 (48.7%) 26 (27.1%) 16 (51.6%)
 Bachelor degree or higher 43 (38.0%) 49 (51.0%) 11 (35.5%)
Mother's birthplace
 U.S. 89 (78.1%) 78 (81.3%) 26 (83.9%)
 Mexico 6 (5.3%) 6 (6.2%) 2 (6.4%)
 Other country 19 (16.7%) 12 (12.5%) 3 (9.7%)
Delivery payer
 Government program 20 (17.7%) 11 (11.5%) 6 (19.3%)
 Private insurance 93 (82.3%) 85 (88.5%) 25 (80.7%)
*

P<0.05 likelihood ratio Chi-square test (child's sex, mother's education); P<0.05 one-way ANOVA (child's age)

**

Other race categories include Black, Asian, and Pacific Islander or Native Hawaiian

Diagnosis and Behavioral Assessments

An autism disorder diagnosis was confirmed by qualified practitioners who have achieved research-reliability using the two gold-standard instruments: the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedules (ADOS) (Lord et al., 2000; Lord et al., 2003) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) (Le Couteur et al., 2003; Lord et al., 1997). Diagnostic algorithms have been published for both tests for autism and for autism spectrum disorders. The ADOS utilizes clinician-directed semi-structured play to assess social, communication, and imaginative play areas, which include stereotypic behaviors and restricted interests that are typical of autism. Four different modules are available depending on the expressive language skills of the child. Based on age and skill level, 92 AU participants were administered ADOS module 1, designed for the least verbal children. Only results from ADOS module 1 were used for correlative analysis of IgG isotypes with behavior as there was insufficient power for analysis using ADOS module 2. The ADI-R is an interview-based assessment administered to the primary caregiver of the child and measures qualitative impairments in reciprocal social interaction, communication, and repetitive and stereotyped behaviors. The ADOS and ADI-R administered together provide a reliable assessment of AU in young children, including those with mental retardation (de Bildt et al., 2004). To satisfy the definition of AU used in the CHARGE study, participants needed to score at or above levels meeting established criteria for autism diagnosis on the social interaction, communication, and stereotyped behaviors domains of the ADI-R prior to 36 months and meet the social + communication cut-off for AU on the ADOS module 1 or 2 (Hertz-Picciotto et al., 2006; Lord et al., 2003).

Parents of all participants, regardless of the child's diagnosis, completed the self-administered Aberrant Behavior Checklist (ABC), which is designed to assess inappropriate and maladaptive behaviors in children (Aman and Singh, 1994). This instrument assesses the frequency of behaviors related to the following domains: irritability, lethargy, stereotypic behaviors, hyperactivity, and inappropriate speech. Elevated scores on the ABC indicate increasing severity of impairment in the behavioral domains assessed. The ABC was completed for the majority of the subjects with only 19 uncompleted assessments. To confirm non-ASD status, the primary caregivers of children entering the study from the DD or TD group were additionally administered the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). The SCQ, which is an adaptation of the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) (Rutter et al., 2003) is a brief caregiver-completed screen for autism spectrum disorders that focuses on communication and social skill development across the child's lifetime, as well as in the most recent three months. Children with an ASD will score above 14 on the SCQ, with good agreement with the ADI-R, whereas children without an ASD will score less than 15 (Rutter et al., 2003). In the CHARGE study children with scores 15 or greater are assessed for autism with ADI-R and ADOS and are removed from the TD or DD control comparison groups (Hertz-Picciotto et al., 2006).

During a clinic visit each child's cognitive function was assessed using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) (Mullen, 1995) and adaptive function using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS) to assess adaptive function (Sparrow and Cicchetti, 1985) and are based on previously established reference values. The MSEL is a standardized test administered to children between 3 and 60 months of age and requires the child to complete requested activities in the presence of the examiner (e.g. block sorting). The MSEL addresses the areas of visual reception, fine motor skills, receptive language, and expressive language. The VABS is a standardized parental interview to assess adaptive function and can be utilized across age ranges. Areas of VABS assessment include communication (receptive, expressive, written), daily living skills (personal, domestic, community), socialization (interpersonal, coping, play), and motor skills (fine and gross). The composite scores from MSEL and VABS were used to designate delayed (DD) and typically developing (TD) groups. Delayed development was defined as having a score of 70 or less on MSEL and/or a score of 69 or less on VABS. Children with a score of 71 or above on MSEL and a score of 70 or above on VABS (i.e. within 2 standard deviations from the mean) were considered as typically developing. Only typically developing children who were unrelated to any other group were included in the TD group. Only one AU subject did not complete the MSEL during the clinic visit. Decreasing standardized scores on the MSEL and VABS assessments indicate a lower level of functional ability.

IgG Isotyping

Peripheral blood was collected in acid-citrate-dextrose Vacutainer tubes (BD Biosciences; San Jose, CA) and centrifuged for 10 min at 2300 rpm. Plasma was harvested and stored at -80°C until samples were assayed. Plasma was analyzed by Luminex using Beadlyte human IgG subclass beads (Upstate; Lake Placid, NY). The assay kit allowed for threshold of detection limits of as little as 3 ng/ml of IgG1, 16 ng/ml IgG2, 0.2 ng/ml IgG3, and 0.1 ng/ml IgG4. Analyte-specific antibody conjugated beads were incubated with 25 μl of 1× biotinylated detection antibody solution and 50μl of either standard, or 50 μl of plasma diluted to 1:8000 in human isotyping serum diluent (supplied in kit) in the dark for 1 hour at room temperature on an orbital shaker (500-600 rpm). After wells were washed and aspirated by vacuum manifold aspiration, 25 μl of strepavidin-conjugated R-phycoerythrin was added to each well and incubated in the dark for 1 hour at room temperature on an orbital shaker (500-600 rpm). R-phycoerythrin fluorescence was measured on a Bio-Plex 200 System (Bio-Rad Laboratories Inc.; Hercules, CA) to determine concentration.

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics were computed for selected sociodemographic variables across diagnostic groups. P-values were calculated using likelihood ratio Chi-square test or one-way ANOVAs. Covariates of interest included the following demographic and socioeconomic factors: child's age at blood draw, child's sex, child's race/ethnicity, maternal education level, mother's place of birth (US vs. elsewhere), and delivery payer (private insurance vs. government program). In analyses involving questionnaire and assessment score data, diagnostic group was also evaluated as a covariate. Bivariate linear regression analyses involving each covariate (i.e. child's age, sex, race/ethnicity, maternal education, birthplace, delivery payer, and diagnostic group) and outcome of interest (i.e. IgG isotypes, ABC scores, assessment scores) were performed to determine associations with each outcome and to assess potential confounding by these variables. Covariates were considered to be associated with an outcome if the p-value was ≤0.20 and were fitted into weighted multiple linear regression models for further evaluation.

Analysis Weights

Analysis weights were constructed to correct for under-represented subjects in the CHARGE study with regard to key demographic and socioeconomic status (SES) factors including mother's education, birthplace, child's race and ethnicity, and delivery payer. First, to calculate these weights, the probability of participation in the study needed to be defined. Pools of potential CHARGE Study participants were determined for each of the three study groups (AU, DD, and TD) using Regional Center data and birth files. Next, these pools were combined and probabilities for participation, contingent on subject group and a set of sociodemographic variables from the birth files, were obtained. (A variable was created to distinguish participants from non-participants). To evaluate the sociodemographic factors, each variable was individually fitted into a logistic regression model adjusted for subject group, where the outcome was participant (in study) versus non-participant (not in study). Finally, a logistic regression model with SES, demographic, and subject group variables, as well as interaction terms (involving subject group and selected sociodemographic variables), was fitted using forward selection to calculate predicted probabilities of participation. The analysis weight for each subject was defined as the inverse of the probability of participation for that subject based on the sociodemographic characteristics and subject group.

Data Analyses

Tolerance and eigenvalues were used to evaluate collinearity in all multiple linear regression models. In primary analyses, natural log-transformed IgG isotypes (outcome) were compared by group (predictor) in weighted multiple linear regression models adjusted for child's age and gender. Secondary analyses examined the association between IgG isotype levels (predictor) and behavioral assessment scores using weighted multiple linear regression models adjusted for diagnostic group, as well as child's age, gender, mother's birthplace, and/or delivery payer. Analyses were not adjusted for multiple comparisons. Findings with P-value <0.05 were considered significant. All analyses were carried out using SAS version 9.1 (SAS Inc.; Cary, NC) and Prism 5 Software (GraphPad Software; San Diego, CA).

Results

Comparison of Study Groups

The median ages of AU participants was 3.5 years (interquartile range [IQR]: 3.0-4.3), for TD participants 3.2 years (IQR: 2.8-3.8) and for DD participants 3.6 years (IQR: 3.0-4.5) (p=0.01, AU vs TD; Table 1). In the AU group there were 92% males compared with 75% males in the TD group (AU vs TD; p=0.001) and 61% males in the DD group (p=0.0001, AU vs DD). Approximately, one-half of participants in each group were Caucasian. Over 50% of mothers of TD participants attained a maximum education level of at least a Bachelor's degree compared with 38% mothers of AU participants (p=0.005). Although more mothers of TD participants used private insurance to pay for delivery compared to mothers of AU participants (89% versus 82%) this was not statistically significant.

Increased IgG4 subclass is associated with AU diagnosis

There were significantly increased levels of the IgG4 subclass in children with AU (3.2 (1.0-8.0) mg/dl median (interquartile range)) compared with TD controls (1.7 (0.5-3.5) mg/dl, p=0.016, Figure 1, Table 2). Furthermore, there was a statistical difference in IgG4 levels in AU children compared with DD controls (1.8 (0.7-5.0) mg/dl, p=0.041, Figure 1, Table 2). No statistical difference in IgG4 levels was observed between DD and TD children. IgG2 subclass levels were marginally raised in children with AU (8.3 (6.6-12.2) mg/dl compared with TD controls (7.4 (5.6-10.8) mg/dl), but did not reach statistical significance (p=0.051, Table 2). IgG2 levels were not different between DD (9.5 (7.2-14.6) mg/dl) compared with TD. IgG1 and IgG3 subclasses were not statistically different between any of the study groups (Table 2).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Plasma IgG4 levels (ng/ml) in autism. IgG4 levels were significantly increased in children with autism (AU) compared with typically developing (TD) controls (p=0.016) and children with other developmental disabilities (DD) (p=0.041). The bars represents the median plus interquartile range for IgG4 levels in each group.

Table 2.

IgG isotype values (mg/dl) shown as median (interquartile ranges). IgG4 is significantly increased in autism (AU) subjects compared with typically developing (TD) and developmental delayed (DD) controls.

AU (n=114) TD (n=96) DD (n=31)
IgG1 101.1 (82.1–128.0) 104.8 (85.5–129.4) 112.4 (96.6–135.4)
IgG2 8.3 (6.6–12.2) 7.4 (5.6–10.8) 9.5 (7.2–14.6)
IgG3 2.8 (2.0–4.1) 2.9 (2.3–3.9) 3.1 (2.0–5.4)
IgG4 3.2 (1.0–8.0)a, b 1.7 (0.5–3.5) 1.8 (0.7–5.0)
a

P<0.05, AU vs TD,

b

AU vs DD

To address whether IgG subclass levels were associated with developmental and behavioral measures as previously suggested (Croonenberghs et al., 2002), we fitted weighted multiple linear regression models to examine the association between IgG4 subclass levels with data from the following behavioral, cognitive and adaptive assessments: ABC, MSEL, and VABS (Table 3) for all participants. Although IgG4 was positively associated with stereotypical behavior (t=2.65, p=0.01) and with lethargy (t=4.21, p=0.001) using the ABC behavioral assessment in weighted analysis that was unadjusted for any covariates, when each regression model was adjusted for diagnostic group, child's age, gender, mother's birthplace and delivery payer the associations between IgG4 and stereotypical behavior (t=2.26, p=0.13) and with lethargy (t=2.55, p=0.11) approached but failed to reach statistical significance. No significant associations were observed between any IgG isotypes levels and other ABC subscales, or in measures using MSEL or VABS assessments in unadjusted or adjusted analyses.

Table 3.

Participant behavioral and assessment scores. Data shown as median (interquartile range).

AU (n=114) TD (n=96) DD (n=31)
Aberrant Behavior Checklist
Irritability 13.0 (9.0–19.0) 1.0 (0.0–4.0) 7.5 (3.0–18.0)
Lethargy 12.0 (8.0–18.0) 0.0 (0.0–0.0) 3.5 (1.0–9.0)
Stereotypy 6.0 (4.0– 9.0) 0.0 (0.0–0.0) 0.0 (0.0–4.0)
Hyperactivity 18.0 (14.0–26.0) 1.5 (0.0–5.0) 10.0 (4.0–23.0)
Inappropriate Speech 2.0 (0.0– 4.0) 0.0 (0.0–1.0) 0.0 (0.0–2.0)
Total Score 56.0 (39.0–73.0) 3.5 (0.0–10.0) 30.0 (13.0–46.0)
Mullen Scales of Early Learning
Visual Reception 20.0 (20.0–29.0) 58.0 (49.5–64.5) 20.0 (20.0–30.0)
Fine Motor 20.0 (20.0–27.5) 52.0 (45.0–59.0) 20.0 (20.0–21.0)
Receptive Language 20.0 (20.0–25.0) 51.0 (46.0–61.0) 20.0 (20.0–26.0)
Expressive Language 20.0 (20.0–23.0) 53.0 (43.0–62.0) 20.0 (20.0–27.0)
Composite Score 50.0 (49.0–58.0) 106.0 (95.0–121.5) 49.0 (49.0–56.0)
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales
Communication 60.0 (54.0–66.0) 107.5 (98.0–116.0) 62.0 (56.0–70.0)
Socialization 63.0 (58.0–69.0) 102.0 (93.0–113.0) 64.0 (57.0–76.0)
Daily Living 63.0 (57.0–72.0) 105.5 (99.0–114.5) 68.0 (59.0–79.0)
Motor Skills 72.0 (62.0–86.0) 111.0 (103.5–120.0) 58.0 (54.0–68.0)
Composite Score 60.0 (54.0–68.0) 111.0 (99.0–120.0) 61.0 (52.0–66.0)

Further analyses were conducted within the group of children with AU only to determine whether IgG subclasses were associated with severity of autistic symptoms using autism-specific assessments. Among AU children assessed using the ADOS module 1, higher IgG4 was associated with increased severity of social interaction impairments as measured by the ADOS module 1 (t=2.13, p=0.036) in models that were weighted and adjusted for child's age and gender. Increased severity of communication impairments in AU participants as measured by ADOS module 1 were not statistically significantly associated with IgG4 levels in weighted linear regression models adjusted for child's age and gender (t=1.64, p=0.093). There were no associations between IgG1, IgG2 or IgG3 levels and ADOS measurements. In AU participants there was no significant associations were noted for any IgG subclasses and reciprocal social interaction, verbal communication, non-verbal communication and stereotyped behaviors using ADI-R assessments following weighted analysis and adjustment for age and gender. There were no associations of any IgG subclass levels with assessments scores on the MSEL, with VABS or ABC in the AU only participants.

Discussion

The major finding of our study was that IgG4 levels were increased in children with autistic disorder compared with typically developing control children and compared with developmentally delayed children unaffected by autism. Our data supports a previous report that showed IgG4 levels were increased in autism (Croonenberghs et al., 2002). This previous study also hinted that increased IgG4 levels were associated with increasing severity of aberrant behaviors in autism. In the current investigation there was some evidence to suggest that IgG4 levels may be associated with autism-specific social interaction impairments among the children with autism based on measurements using the gold-standard ADOS assessment. However, as no other behavioral measures obtained from separate behavioral assessments, including reciprocal interactions on the ADI-R, found any statistical associations between IgG4 levels and behaviors, these data are difficult to interpret and should be treated with caution until further future investigations are performed. In addition, although our statistical analyses addressed potential confounding factors from socioeconomic status, by weighting and adjusting the analysis, these factors are only proxy measures for socioeconomic status and further large-scale studies need to be designed that fully account for socioeconomic and demographic factors.

There are several limitations to this current study. First, while we did not include any participant who showed visible signs of illness or who had a fever, our use of a single cross-sectionally obtained blood sample would not capture temporal fluctuations in the IgG isotype levels based on health status or environmental factors such as vaccinations. Secondly, limiting the ADOS analysis to module 1 was necessary as the algorithm scores between modules 1 and 2 are not interchangeable, but this clearly limited the sample to the more severely language impaired children, who, within the age group studied are usually the more cognitively impaired. Other measures of communication impairments should be examined in further studies that have sufficient power for analysis within groups of children comparable with regard to level of cognitive development, to determine whether the findings on IgG4 and communication skills are replicated. These studies must be conducted within case groups, as diagnostic group may be a confounding factor when correlating IgG levels with behavioral outcomes. Thirdly, in the autism group used for this study, there was a slightly higher than expected prevalence in boys relative to girls, as compared with the Other race categories include Black, Asian, that there is considerable variability in the male:female ratio (reviewed by Fombonne, 2005) which range from 1.3 to 15.7. In addition, the recent CDC survey that obtained much larger case series in six states showed a two-fold variation for sex ratio (Rice et al., 2007). A higher male:female ratio is often found with milder cases, and in a sample size of 114 cases, deviation from the widely acknowledged 4:1 ratio in autism can likely be expected. In the case of TD controls, for the larger CHARGE study, we set the gender ratio to frequency-match (to AU) in advance of the study using the 4:1 figure based on literature reports. However, within any subsample of CHARGE, there will be some slight variations in the gender ratio due to random variability.

The salient finding of this study is the clear demonstration of an increase in IgG4 levels among children with autism compared with TD and DD controls. This result is present after adjusting for socioeconomic and demographic factors and adds to the growing body of research suggesting immune alterations accompany the behaviorally defined disorder of autism. Evidence related to specific behavioral domains is weak, but in any case, all associations reported here need to be replicated in larger future studies and especially in prospective studies that examine children prior to the development of autism's core features.

Although not statistically significant, levels of IgG1 and IgG3 appear lower in AU compared with the TD controls. We recently found a statistically significant decrease in total IgG levels in children with AU compared with controls as assessed by ELISA (Heuer et al., 2008). These studies and the current study differ in the analytes that were measured. For example, here we used a Luminex based technique to measure IgG subclasses and do not directly measure total IgG, whereas the other study measured total IgG but not individual IgG subclasses. The different buffering systems, specificity of antibodies, antibody sensitivities, different assay platforms and different product manufacturers used make it hard to directly compare between these two studies; however, the slight decrease in IgG1 (the major IgG subclass in plasma, especially in pediatric cases) in this study would suggest that the same trend would be observed i.e. that total IgG levels are reduced in AU compared with controls. Moreover, the results from these two papers would suggest that in AU there is a possible shift in IgG constituents, such that there is an increased level of IgG4 on a reduced background of total IgG. This finding is consistent with a previous report of increased IgG4 determined by nephlometry in children with autism compared to unaffected siblings (Trajkovski et al., 2004). Of interest, associations between behavior and IgG levels were noted in both this study and the investigation of total IgG reported by Heuer and colleagues (Heuer et al., 2008). Future studies in a larger cohort in which total IgG and its individual subclasses are measured using the same technique would clarify these issues.

IgG4 is a relatively unique subclass of IgG. IgG4 unlike IgG1, IgG2, and IgG3 does not bind strongly to any of the antibody receptors (CD16, CD32) found on human leukocytes (Aalberse and Schuurman, 2002; van der Zee et al., 1986). IgG4 binds to the receptor CD64 (FcγRI) on monocytes and macrophages with ten times less affinity than either IgG1 or IgG3. Moreover, the circulating structure of IgG4 is functionally monovalent and differs from IgG1, IgG2, or IgG3, which contain two binding sites (Aalberse et al., 1983; van der Zee et al., 1986). These features drastically alter the biological function of the IgG4 antibody, shifting its function to that of a blocking or inhibiting antibody rather than one of protection through the more conventional routes, such as complement fixation (van der Neut Kolfschoten et al., 2007). Increased IgG4 is found in patients with atopy as well as prolonged chronic antigen stimulation (Aalberse et al., 1983; Shakib, 1986). Under prolonged antigen exposure, IgG4 allergen-specific antibodies increase eventually overtaking IgG1-specific titers (Aalberse et al., 1983). These IgG4 antibodies directly interfere with IgG1 antibody function (Aalberse and Vermeulen, 2001; van der Zee et al., 1986), possibly serving a protective role by decreasing IgG1-mediated inflammation and tissue damage.

If one considers IgG4 as a blocking antibody induced in the presence of chronic antigen exposure, including potential autoantigens or food allergens, this could relate to several of the phenomena described in autism, including increased food allergies and the presence of autoantibodies (Ashwood and Van de Water, 2004; Jyonouchi et al., 2002). The increased IgG4 in ASD children may be indicative of a genetic tendency towards either 1) chronic self-antigen exposure or 2) poor clearance of microbial antigens due to some other immune abnormality. Either situation is equally likely, both increased self-reactive (primarily neuroreactive) antibodies (Cabanlit et al., 2007; Connolly et al., 2006; Connolly et al., 1999; Kozlovskaia et al., 2000; Singh and Rivas, 2004; Singh et al., 1997; Singh et al., 1993; Todd et al., 1988; Wills et al., 2007), and other altered immunological findings (Ashwood and Van de Water, 2004) have been reported in autism. Indeed, the association of increased IgG4 with severity of behavioral and cognitive impairment may be related to another primary factor, such as tissue damage releasing sequestered neuronal antigens or underlying chronic inflammation. An area for further research is to determine if neuronal antibodies reported in other studies (Cabanlit et al., 2007; Connolly et al., 2006; Connolly et al., 1999; Kozlovskaia et al., 2000; Singh and Rivas, 2004; Singh et al., 1997; Singh et al., 1993; Todd et al., 1988; Wills et al., 2007) are of the IgG4 subclass, suggesting they may be blocking antibodies.

The implications of the association between higher IgG4 levels and AU are unclear, however, one could speculate that a dysregulation in immune function may skew the immune response towards an elevated IgG4 antibody titer. Furthermore, potential gene-environment interactions, in which chronic antigen exposure in individuals who have a genetic predisposition towards immune dysregulation could thus, lead to increased IgG4 concentrations. The link between such immune dysregulation and changes in neurodevelopment and, subsequently behavior, are still unknown. This study provides further specificity to the growing evidence for a role of the immune system in the development and exacerbation of behavioral defined autism disorders, and for potentially critical interactions of the nervous and immune systems in such disorders.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by NIEHS 1 P01 ES11269-01; U.S. EPA STAR program grant R829388, Cure Autism Now, the Ted Lindsay Foundation, Peter Emch Foundation and a generous gift from the Johnson Family. We are very grateful for the dedication of our study participants and their families. We would like to acknowledge the technical support of the UC Davis M.I.N.D. clinicians and staff.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Aalberse RC, Schuurman J. IgG4 breaking the rules. Immunology. 2002;105:9–19. doi: 10.1046/j.0019-2805.2001.01341.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aalberse RC, van der Gaag R, van Leeuwen J. Serologic aspects of IgG4 antibodies. I. Prolonged immunization results in an IgG4-restricted response. J Immunol. 1983;130:722–726. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aalberse RC, Vermeulen E. Immune reactivity to mite allergens in nonatopic subjects: immune deviation or immune ignorance. International archives of allergy and immunology. 2001;124:208–209. doi: 10.1159/000053712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aman MG, Singh NN. Aberrant Behavior Checklist - Community. Slosson Educational Publications; East Aurora, NY: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  5. Ashwood P, Van de Water J. A review of autism and the immune response. Clin Dev Immunol. 2004;11:165–174. doi: 10.1080/10446670410001722096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ashwood P, Wills S, Van de Water J. The immune response in autism: a new frontier for autism research. Journal of leukocyte biology. 2006 doi: 10.1189/jlb.1205707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beilmann M, Odenthal M, Jung W, Vande Woude GF, Dienes HP, Schirmacher P. Neoexpression of the c-met/hepatocyte growth factor-scatter factor receptor gene in activated monocytes. Blood. 1997;90:4450–4458. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Butler MG, Dasouki MJ, Zhou XP, Talebizadeh Z, Brown M, Takahashi TN, Miles JH, Wang CH, Stratton R, Pilarski R, Eng C. Subset of individuals with autism spectrum disorders and extreme macrocephaly associated with germline PTEN tumour suppressor gene mutations. J Med Genet. 2005;42:318–321. doi: 10.1136/jmg.2004.024646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cabanlit M, Wills S, Goines P, Ashwood P, Van de Water J. Brain-specific Autoantibodies in the Plasma of Subjects with Autistic Spectrum Disorder. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2007;1107:92–103. doi: 10.1196/annals.1381.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chakrabarti S, Fombonne E. Pervasive developmental disorders in preschool children: confirmation of high prevalence. The American journal of psychiatry. 2005;162:1133–1141. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chess S. Autism in children with congenital rubella. Journal of autism and childhood schizophrenia. 1971;1:33–47. doi: 10.1007/BF01537741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cohly HH, Panja A. Immunological findings in autism. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2005;71:317–341. doi: 10.1016/s0074-7742(05)71013-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Comi AM, Zimmerman AW, Frye VH, Law PA, Peeden JN. Familial clustering of autoimmune disorders and evaluation of medical risk factors in autism. J Child Neurol. 1999;14:388–394. doi: 10.1177/088307389901400608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Connolly AM, Chez M, Streif EM, Keeling RM, Golumbek PT, Kwon JM, Riviello JJ, Robinson RG, Neuman RJ, Deuel RM. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and autoantibodies to neural antigens in sera of children with autistic spectrum disorders, Landau-Kleffner syndrome, and epilepsy. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:354–363. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Connolly AM, Chez MG, Pestronk A, Arnold ST, Mehta S, Deuel RK. Serum autoantibodies to brain in Landau-Kleffner variant, autism, and other neurologic disorders. J Pediatr. 1999;134:607–613. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(99)70248-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Croonenberghs J, Wauters A, Devreese K, Verkerk R, Scharpe S, Bosmans E, Egyed B, Deboutte D, Maes M. Increased serum albumin, gamma globulin, immunoglobulin IgG, and IgG2 and IgG4 in autism. Psychol Med. 2002;32:1457–1463. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702006037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Daniels WW, Warren RP, Odell JD, Maciulis A, Burger RA, Warren WL, Torres AR. Increased frequency of the extended or ancestral haplotype B44-SC30-DR4 in autism. Neuropsychobiology. 1995;32:120–123. doi: 10.1159/000119223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. de Bildt A, Sytema S, Ketelaars C, Kraijer D, Mulder E, Volkmar F, Minderaa R. Interrelationship between Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic (ADOS-G), Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R), and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) classification in children and adolescents with mental retardation. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34:129–137. doi: 10.1023/b:jadd.0000022604.22374.5f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ferrante P, Saresella M, Guerini FR, Marzorati M, Musetti MC, Cazzullo AG. Significant association of HLA A2-DR11 with CD4 naive decrease in autistic children. Biomed Pharmacother. 2003;57:372–374. doi: 10.1016/s0753-3322(03)00099-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fombonne E. Epidemiology of autistic disorder and other pervasive developmental disorders. J Clin Psychiatry. 2005;66 10:3–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fortier ME, Luheshi GN, Boksa P. Effects of prenatal infection on prepulse inhibition in the rat depend on the nature of the infectious agent and the stage of pregnancy. Behavioural brain research. 2007;181:270–277. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.04.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Garbett K, Ebert PJ, Mitchell A, Lintas C, Manzi B, Mirnics K, Persico AM. Immune transcriptome alterations in the temporal cortex of subjects with autism. Neurobiol Dis. 2008;30:303–311. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2008.01.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hertz-Picciotto I, Croen LA, Hansen R, Jones CR, van de Water J, Pessah IN. The CHARGE study: an epidemiologic investigation of genetic and environmental factors contributing to autism. Environ Health Perspect. 2006;114:1119–1125. doi: 10.1289/ehp.8483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Heuer L, Ashwood P, Schauer J, Goines P, Krakowiak P, Hertz-Picciotto I, Hansen R, Croen L, Pessaah IN, Van de Water J. Reduced Levels of Immunoglobulin in Children with Autism Correlates with Behavioral Symptoms. Autism Research Submitted. 2008 doi: 10.1002/aur.42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Jyonouchi H, Sun S, Itokazu N. Innate immunity associated with inflammatory responses and cytokine production against common dietary proteins in patients with autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychobiology. 2002;46:76–84. doi: 10.1159/000065416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Korvatska E, Van de Water J, Anders TF, Gershwin ME. Genetic and immunologic considerations in autism. Neurobiol Dis. 2002;9:107–125. doi: 10.1006/nbdi.2002.0479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kozlovskaia GV, Kliushnik TP, Goriunova AV, Turkova IL, Kalinina MA, Sergienko NS. Nerve growth factor auto-antibodies in children with various forms of mental dysontogenesis and in schizophrenia high risk group. Zh Nevrol Psikhiatr Im S S Korsakova. 2000;100:50–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Le Couteur A, Lord C, Rutter M. Autism Diagnostic Interview - Revised (ADI-R) Western Psychological Services; Los Angeles: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  29. Lee LC, Zachary AA, Leffell MS, Newschaffer CJ, Matteson KJ, Tyler JD, Zimmerman AW. HLA-DR4 in families with autism. Pediatr Neurol. 2006;35:303–307. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2006.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lord C, Pickles A, McLennan J, Rutter M, Bregman J, Folstein S, Fombonne E, Leboyer M, Minshew N. Diagnosing autism: analyses of data from the Autism Diagnostic Interview. J Autism Dev Disord. 1997;27:501–517. doi: 10.1023/a:1025873925661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A, Rutter M. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:205–223. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) Western Psychological Services; Los Angeles: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  33. Meyer U, Nyffeler M, Engler A, Urwyler A, Schedlowski M, Knuesel I, Yee BK, Feldon J. The time of prenatal immune challenge determines the specificity of inflammation-mediated brain and behavioral pathology. J Neurosci. 2006;26:4752–4762. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0099-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Miyazaki K, Narita N, Narita M. Maternal administration of thalidomide or valproic acid causes abnormal serotonergic neurons in the offspring: implication for pathogenesis of autism. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2005;23:287–297. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Money J, Bobrow NA, Clarke FC. Autism and autoimmune disease: a family study. Journal of autism and childhood schizophrenia. 1971;1:146–160. doi: 10.1007/BF01537954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mullen EM. Mullen Scales of Early Learning. American Guidance Service, Inc; Circle Pines, MN: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  37. Narita N, Kato M, Tazoe M, Miyazaki K, Narita M, Okado N. Increased monoamine concentration in the brain and blood of fetal thalidomide- and valproic acid-exposed rat: putative animal models for autism. Pediatric research. 2002;52:576–579. doi: 10.1203/00006450-200210000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Niehus R, Lord C. Early medical history of children with autism spectrum disorders. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006;27:S120–127. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200604002-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Pardo CA, Eberhart CG. The neurobiology of autism. Brain Pathol. 2007;17:434–447. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2007.00102.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Rice CE, Baio J, Van Naarden Braun K, Doernberg N, Meaney FJ, Kirby RS. A public health collaboration for the surveillance of autism spectrum disorders. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2007;21:179–190. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2007.00801.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rodier PM, Ingram JL, Tisdale B, Croog VJ. Linking etiologies in humans and animal models: studies of autism. Reprod Toxicol. 1997;11:417–422. doi: 10.1016/s0890-6238(97)80001-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Rosen NJ, Yoshida CK, Croen LA. Infection in the first 2 years of life and autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics. 2007;119:61–69. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Rutter M, Bailey A, Berument SK, Lord C, Pickles A. Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) Western Psychological Services; Los Angeles: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  44. Shakib F. The IgG4 subclass. Monographs in allergy. 1986;19:223–226. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Shi L, Fatemi SH, Sidwell RW, Patterson PH. Maternal influenza infection causes marked behavioral and pharmacological changes in the offspring. J Neurosci. 2003;23:297–302. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-01-00297.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Silva SC, Correia C, Fesel C, Barreto M, Coutinho AM, Marques C, Miguel TS, Ataide A, Bento C, Borges L, Oliveira G, Vicente AM. Autoantibody repertoires to brain tissue in autism nuclear families. Journal of neuroimmunology. 2004;152:176–182. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2004.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Singh VK, Rivas WH. Prevalence of serum antibodies to caudate nucleus in autistic children. Neurosci Lett. 2004;355:53–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Singh VK, Warren R, Averett R, Ghaziuddin M. Circulating autoantibodies to neuronal and glial filament proteins in autism. Pediatr Neurol. 1997;17:88–90. doi: 10.1016/s0887-8994(97)00045-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Singh VK, Warren RP, Odell JD, Warren WL, Cole P. Antibodies to myelin basic protein in children with autistic behavior. Brain, behavior, and immunity. 1993;7:97–103. doi: 10.1006/brbi.1993.1010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Skaar DA, Shao Y, Haines JL, Stenger JE, Jaworski J, Martin ER, DeLong GR, Moore JH, McCauley JL, Sutcliffe JS, Ashley-Koch AE, Cuccaro ML, Folstein SE, Gilbert JR, Pericak-Vance MA. Analysis of the RELN gene as a genetic risk factor for autism. Molecular psychiatry. 2005;10:563–571. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sparrow S, Cicchetti D. Diagnostic uses of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. J Pediatr Psychol. 1985;10:215–225. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/10.2.215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Stubbs EG, Ash E, Williams CP. Autism and congenital cytomegalovirus. J Autism Dev Disord. 1984;14:183–189. doi: 10.1007/BF02409660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Todd RD, Hickok JM, Anderson GM, Cohen DJ. Antibrain antibodies in infantile autism. Biol Psychiatry. 1988;23:644–647. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(88)90012-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Torres AR, Sweeten TL, Cutler A, Bedke BJ, Fillmore M, Stubbs EG, Odell D. The association and linkage of the HLA-A2 class I allele with autism. Human immunology. 2006;67:346–351. doi: 10.1016/j.humimm.2006.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Trajkovski V, Ajdinski L, Spiroski M. Plasma concentration of immunoglobulin classes and subclasses in children with autism in the Republic of Macedonia: retrospective study. Croat Med J. 2004;45:746–749. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. van der Neut Kolfschoten M, Schuurman J, Losen M, Bleeker WK, Martinez-Martinez P, Vermeulen E, den Bleker TH, Wiegman L, Vink T, Aarden LA, De Baets MH, van de Winkel JG, Aalberse RC, Parren PW. Science. Vol. 317. New York, N.Y: 2007. Anti-inflammatory activity of human IgG4 antibodies by dynamic Fab arm exchange; pp. 1554–1557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. van der Zee JS, van Swieten P, Aalberse RC. Serologic aspects of IgG4 antibodies. II. IgG4 antibodies form small, nonprecipitating immune complexes due to functional monovalency. J Immunol. 1986;137:3566–3571. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Vojdani A, Campbell AW, Anyanwu E, Kashanian A, Bock K, Vojdani E. Antibodies to neuron-specific antigens in children with autism: possible cross-reaction with encephalitogenic proteins from milk, Chlamydia pneumoniae and Streptococcus group A. Journal of neuroimmunology. 2002;129:168–177. doi: 10.1016/s0165-5728(02)00180-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Vojdani A, O'Bryan T, Green JA, McCandless J, Woeller KN, Vojdani E, Nourian AA, Cooper EL. Immune response to dietary proteins, gliadin and cerebellar peptides in children with autism. Nutr Neurosci. 2004;7:151–161. doi: 10.1080/10284150400004155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Vojdani A, Pangborn JB, Vojdani E, Cooper EL. Infections, toxic chemicals and dietary peptides binding to lymphocyte receptors and tissue enzymes are major instigators of autoimmunity in autism. Int J Immunopathol Pharmacol. 2003;16:189–199. doi: 10.1177/039463200301600302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Warren RP, Yonk J, Burger RW, Odell D, Warren WL. DR-positive T cells in autism: association with decreased plasma levels of the complement C4B protein. Neuropsychobiology. 1995;31:53–57. doi: 10.1159/000119172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Williams G, King J, Cunningham M, Stephan M, Kerr B, Hersh JH. Fetal valproate syndrome and autism: additional evidence of an association. Developmental medicine and child neurology. 2001;43:202–206. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Wills S, Cabanlit M, Bennett J, Ashwood P, Amaral D, Van de Water J. Autoantibodies in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1107:79–91. doi: 10.1196/annals.1381.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Yeargin-Allsopp M, Rice C, Karapurkar T, Doernberg N, Boyle C, Murphy C. Prevalence of autism in a US metropolitan area. JAMA. 2003;289:49–55. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.1.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES