Skip to main content
British Journal of Pharmacology logoLink to British Journal of Pharmacology
. 2009 Jan 19;156(2):388–396. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2008.00008.x

The effect of ‘two hit’ neonatal and young-adult stress on dopaminergic modulation of prepulse inhibition and dopamine receptor density

Kwok Ho Christopher Choy 1, Yvonne P de Visser 1, Maarten van den Buuse 1
PMCID: PMC2697842  PMID: 19154431

Abstract

Background and purpose:

A combination of early neurodevelopmental insult(s) and young-adult stress exposure may be involved in the development of schizophrenia. We studied prepulse inhibition (PPI) regulation in rats after an early stress, maternal deprivation, combined with a later stress, simulated by chronic corticosterone treatment, and also determined whether changes in brain dopamine receptor density were involved.

Experimental approach:

Rats were subjected to either 24 h maternal deprivation on postnatal day 9, corticosterone treatment from 8 to 10 weeks of age, or both. At 12 weeks of age, the rats were injected with 0.1, 0.3 or 1.0 mg·kg−1 of apomorphine or 0.5 or 2.5 mg·kg−1 of amphetamine and PPI was determined using automated startle boxes. Dopamine D1 and D2 receptor levels were assessed in the nucleus accumbens and caudate nucleus using receptor autoradiography.

Key results:

Young-adult treatment with corticosterone resulted in attenuated disruption of PPI by apomorphine and amphetamine. In some rats, maternal deprivation resulted in reduced baseline PPI which added to the effect of corticosterone treatment. There was no down-regulation of dopamine D1 or D2 receptors.

Conclusions and implications:

These results confirm and extend our finding of an inhibitory interaction of developmental stress on dopaminergic regulation of PPI. No corresponding changes in dopamine receptor density were observed in brain regions with a major involvement in PPI regulation, suggesting long-lasting desensitization of dopamine receptor signalling or indirect changes in PPI regulation.

Keywords: dopamine, prepulse inhibition, rat, schizophrenia, stress

Introduction

With its early onset of symptoms in the late teens or early twenties, schizophrenia is commonly referred as a developmental disorder. The developmental hypothesis of schizophrenia suggests that disturbances which occurred early in development, such as malnutrition, viral infection, obstetric complications or genetic deficits, predispose to the illness (Bayer et al., 1999; Murray and Fearon, 1999; Cannon et al., 2003). More recently, it has been suggested that these early neurodevelopmental insults are not sufficient to cause schizophrenia and, instead, a combination of early neurodevelopmental insult(s) and young-adult stress exposure may be involved in the illness (Pantelis et al., 2003).

Several animal models have been proposed to better understand the developmental aspects of schizophrenia (van den Buuse et al., 2003). However, most of the animal models include a single developmental insult, for example neonatal hippocampal or prefrontal cortex lesions, neonatal hypoxia or isolation rearing (Geyer et al., 1993; Lipska et al., 1995; Brake et al., 2000; Rehn et al., 2004). Therefore, we recently established an animal model, comprising of a combination of an early stress, induced by neonatal maternal deprivation (MD), and a later stress, simulated by young-adult corticosterone (CORT) treatment (Garner et al., 2007; Choy and van den Buuse, 2008; Choy et al., 2008) These animals showed selective impairments of learning and memory and reduced brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene expression, changes which were not observed in animals after either of the stress episodes (Choy et al., 2008).

In this model, we also assessed dopaminergic regulation of prepulse inhibition (PPI), a measure of sensorimotor gating, which is disrupted in patients with schizophrenia (Braff and Geyer 1990; Braff et al., 1995). Dopaminergic pathways in regions such as striatum (caudate putamen) and nucleus accumbens play an important role in PPI regulation via activation of both dopamine D1 and D2 receptors (Swerdlow et al., 1986; 1991; Wan and Swerdlow, 1993; Wan et al., 1994). Stress and developmental insults may result in long-term changes in dopamine receptor responsiveness via alterations in the activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (Walker and Diforio, 1997). In animals treated with neonatal MD and young-adult CORT, treatment with either the dopamine receptor agonist, apomorphine, or the dopamine releaser, amphetamine, failed to cause the disruption of PPI seen in controls (Choy and van den Buuse, 2008). Because we previously only used a single dose of apomorphine and amphetamine, the aim of the present study was to replicate and extend our findings by testing multiple doses of both drugs in animals treated with the two developmental insults. Similar dose-response studies have also been done on other neurodevelopmental models (Lipska et al., 1995; Swerdlow et al., 2000). In addition, the density of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in the nucleus accumbens and caudate putamen was assessed, in order to investigate whether the functional dopaminergic desensitization was correlated with dopamine receptor down-regulation, as observed in other studies (Al-Amin et al., 2000; Tenn et al., 2003).

Methods

Animals

All animal experiments and procedures were approved by the University of Melbourne Animal Experimentation Committee.

Wistar outbred rats (Animal Resources Centre, Canning Vale, WA, Australia) were housed in plastic cages (28 cm h × 30 cm w × 43 cm l) at 21°C on a 12 h light/dark cycle (light on 6:00). Rats received ad libitum access to food and water. The rats' cages were cleaned twice weekly excluding the pregnant mothers during the last week of gestation, or mothers and pups prior to weaning. Litters found at 9:00 were classified as postnatal day 1 (pnd 1) and those found at 17:00 as pnd 0. All litters were sexed and culled to a maximum of 8 within 24 h after birth, with healthy male pups as the primary choice. Pups were weaned at pnd 21.

Body weights were obtained at the time of weaning (3 weeks of age), when Chol or CORT pellets were implanted (young adult, 8 weeks of age), 3 days after implantation (week 8 + 3 days), 2 weeks after implantation at the time of removal of the pellets (10 weeks of age) and 1–3 days after the last behavioural test (adult, 14 weeks of age).

MD and CORT pellet implants

Maternal deprivation and CORT pellet implants were carried out as previously described (Choy and van den Buuse, 2008). In brief, the litters were separated from their mothers for 24 h on pnd 9, and the same animals were implanted with a 100 mg CORT pellet from week 8 to week 10. As in our previous studies, no mortality was found among pups. Behavioural testing commenced at week 12, i.e. a minimum of 2 weeks after removal of the CORT pellets. Together with the controls of MD (non-deprived, ND) and CORT (cholesterol pellet, Chol), there were four experimental groups after the MD/ND and Chol/CORT treatments in each cohort: ND Chol (controls), ND CORT (CORT treated), MD Chol (maternally deprived) and MD CORT (maternally deprived and CORT treated). Two cohorts of animals comprising 39–40 pups per cohort from 14 to 16 litters were used in this study, with 1–2 pups from each litter allocated to different pre-treatment (ND or MD), treatment (Chol or CORT), and either apomorphine (n = 10 each group) or amphetamine (n = 9–10 each group) PPI behavioural groups.

PPI of acoustic startle

Prepulse inhibition experiments were performed using automated startle boxes (SR-LAB, San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA, USA) with Plexiglas cylinders (9 cm diameter). A piezoelectric accelerometer unit attached underneath the platform of the Plexiglas cylinders detected the startle responses of the animals.

The session started with a 5 min habituation period and included three startle blocks, with the first and the last consisting of ten 115 dB startle pulse-alone trials. The second block was part of the main PPI protocol and consisted of another two blocks of ten 115 dB startle pulse-alone trials. There were 50 prepulse trials, during which a startle pulse was preceded by a prepulse of 2, 4, 8, 12 or 16 dB above the 70 dB background (10 of each), and 10 ‘no stimulus’ trials. Each startle pulse was of 40 ms duration, while the prepulse was 20 ms and there was 100 ms between the prepulse and the pulse. Percentage PPI was calculated with the formula 100 × [(pulse-alone trials − prepulse trials)/pulse-alone trials].

Saline (baseline), three different doses (0.1, 0.3 and 1.0 mg·kg−1) of apomorphine (Sigma) or two different doses (0.5 and 2.5 mg·kg−1) of amphetamine (Sigma) were subcutaneously (s.c.) injected 10 min before the session start. Using a pseudo-randomized, within-animal protocol, all rats were treated with all doses of either apomorphine or amphetamine, with 3–4 days of washout between individual sessions. PPI testing thus lasted for about 2 weeks. From these animals, eight per group were randomly chosen for dopamine D1 and D2 receptor autoradiography.

Dopamine D1 and D2 receptor autoradiography

Animals were culled by decapitation 3 days after the last behavioural tests and brains were rapidly dissected out and snap frozen on powdered dry ice. Fourteen µm coronal sections from the anterior part of the brain including the nucleus accumbens and caudate nucleus were cut on a cryostat and mounted on gel-coated slides where three sections were used for total binding (TB) as a triplicate and another three for non-specific binding (NSB).

The sections were pre-incubated in buffer containing 50 mmol·L−1 Tris-HCl, 120 mmol·L−1 NaCl, 5 mmol·L−1 KCl, 2 mmol·L−1 CaCl2 and 1 mmol·L−1 MgCl2 (pH 7.4) for 30 min. For dopamine D1 receptor binding we used 30 min incubation with 1 nmol·L−1 of [3H]SCH23390 (Perkin Elmer) for TB and additional 0.5 µmol·L−1 cis-flupenthixol (Sigma) for NSB (Figure 1, left panel). This concentration of ligand was similar to that in other studies (Filloux et al., 1987; McCabe et al., 1987) and is approximately twice the KD of dopamine D1 receptors in these conditions (Billard et al., 1984). For dopamine D2 receptor binding, we used 1 nmol·L−1 of [3H]YM09151-2 (nemonapride, Perkin Elmer), 0.1 µmol·L−1 of pindolol (Sigma) and 0.5 µmol·L−1 of 1,3-di-o-tolyguanidine (Sigma) for TB and additional 10 µmol·L−1 of (-)-sulpiride (Sigma) for NSB (Figure 1, right panel). The reported KD value for dopamine D2 receptors is between 0.1 and 0.6 nmol·L−1 (Cox and Waszczak, 1991; Yokoyama et al., 1994; Unis et al., 1998) and our ligand concentration, which has also been used by others (Landwehrmeyer et al., 1993), therefore approximated to between 1.7× and 10× the KD.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Representative example of dopamine D1 receptor (left column) and D2 receptor (right column) autoradiography of the rat forebrain. Top panel shows the total binding (TB) of [3H]SCH23390 (left, D1) or [3H]YM091512 (right, D2) and the bottom panel shows the corresponding non-specific binding (NS). CC, cingulate cortex; CN, caudate nucleus; NAc, nucleus accumbens core; NAs, nucleus accumbens shell.

Both radiolabelled slides and [3H] microscales were apposed to a BAS-TR2025 phosphoimaging plate (Fuji Imaging Plates, Berthold, Australia) for 6 days at room temperature. On the scanned image, relevant brain areas were identified and outlined on the computer screen using the dopamine D1 and D2 receptor density pattern (see Figure 1) and a brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 1986). Data from the left and right sides of the brain were averaged as we did not observe any significant left to right differences for either D1 or D2 receptor density (data not shown). The photo-stimulated luminescence of the analysed image was converted to dpm·mg−1 tissue equivalents using a standard curve obtained by linear regression of the values derived from the tritium scales exposed to the same phosphor-imaging plate (AIS image analysis software). Then dpm·mg−1 values were converted to fmol·mg−1 taking into account the ligand's specific activity (in Ci mmol−1), a decay factor (a constant) and a dpm to Ci conversion constant (2.22 × 1012 dpm·Ci−1). The density of D1 and D2 receptors is thus presented as fmol per milligram (fmol·mg−1) estimated tissue equivalent by subtracting the NSB density value from the TB value (Figure 1) in the caudate putamen, nucleus accumbens core, nucleus accumbens shell and cingulate cortex.

Receptor nomenclature conforms to British Journal of Pharmacology's Guide to Receptors and Channels (Alexander et al., 2008).

Data analysis

All data were analysed with anova with repeated measures when necessary, using the statistical software package Systat version 9.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Where P < 0.05, differences were considered to be statistically significant. In this 2 × 2 design, the between-group factors were the pre-treatment (two levels: ND or MD) and the treatment (two levels: Chol or CORT implants). Within-group factors were drug dose (apomorphine: saline, 0.1, 0.3 and 1.0 mg·kg−1 and amphetamine: saline, 0.5 and 2.5 mg·kg−1) and prepulse intensity (72, 74, 78, 82 and 86 dB). In the first experiment on apomorphine effects, a possible influence of MD-induced changes in baseline PPI was excluded by selecting a subgroup (n = 5) of animals from each treatment group on the basis of baseline PPI and repeating the anova. Dopamine D1 and D2 autoradiography data of individual brain regions were analysed with two-way ANOVA.

Results

Effect of MD and CORT on body weight

Body weight was significantly lower in maternally deprived animals at weaning at 3 weeks of age [main effect F(1,77) = 24.3, P < 0.001]. This overall effect of MD lasted to at least 3 days after the pellet administration [week 8: F(1,77) = 6.2, P = 0.015, week 8 + 3 days: F(1,75) = 5.0, P = 0.029] and diminished thereafter (e.g. week 10, P > 0.05). A significant CORT effect on body weight was observed 3 days after pellet implantation [F(1,75) = 4.9, P = 0.030] and, similarly, the CORT effect diminished at week 10 (P > 0.05). There were no significant effects of MD or CORT on body weight after week 10 (Table 1). Growth rate in gram per day (g·day−1) showed a significant reduction from week 8 to week 10 [F(1,75) = 19.2, P = 0.002] caused by CORT treatment; however, there were no other differences or interactions among the four groups in other time periods (Table 1).

Table 1.

Body weights of rats subjected to either maternal deprivation (MD) or young-adult corticosterone (CORT) treatment or both

Age ND MD
Week 3 57.6 ± 1.0 49.5 ± 1.3*
Week 8 321.3 ± 4.1 304.2 ± 5.5*
ND Chol ND CORT MD Chol MD CORT
Week 8 + 3 days 332.3 ± 5.6 314.1 ± 6.9* 314.0 ± 8.2 300.0 ± 8.1*
Change from week 8 (g·day−1) 2.60 ± 0.48 −1.33 ± 0.62* 3.05 ± 0.24 −1.20 ± 0.47*,**
Week 10 392.8 ± 8.6 373.4 ± 9.7 373.7 ± 10.5 357.8 ± 11.6*
Change from week 8 (g·day−1) 4.88 ± 0.25 3.95 ± 0.34* 4.92 ± 0.27 3.87 ± 0.36*,**
Week 14 492.5 ± 8.7 465.5 ± 7.9* 468.2 ± 10.2 460.8 ± 12.1*

Controls were non-deprived (ND) or received a cholesterol (Chol) control pellet. Data are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) and were obtained from all animals used in the PPI tests (both apomorphine and amphetamine groups). The number of animals in these combined groups was ND Chol = 20, ND CORT = 20, MD Chol = 19 and MD CORT = 20.

*

P < 0.05 for difference with control (ND Chol),

**

P < 0.05 for difference between Chol- and CORT-treated maternally deprived animals (i.e. MD Chol vs. MD CORT).

Experiment 1: effect of apomorphine on PPI and startle

After saline treatment, there was no significant main effect of CORT treatment on PPI in this cohort and a trend for a main effect of MD [F(1,36) = 3.5, P = 0.068] (Figure 2). The effect of MD was significant at PP4 [F(1,36) = 5.3, P = 0.027] and PP8 [F(1,36) = 5.5, P = 0.025]. This confirms our previous observations of a small reduction of baseline PPI after MD (Garner et al., 2007; Choy and van den Buuse, 2008), which in this cohort is dependent on the prepulse intensity. In contrast to PPI, there were no effects of MD or CORT treatment on baseline startle (Table 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

The effect of different doses of apomorphine (0.1, 0.3 and 1.0 mg·kg−1) on prepulse inhibition of startle (PPI) in rats after maternal deprivation (MD) and chronic corticosterone (CORT) treatment. MD consisted of a 24 h separation of the pups from their mothers on postnatal day 9, whereas controls were non-deprived (ND). CORT treatment consisted of subcutaneous implantation of a 100 mg CORT pellet between 8 and 10 weeks of age, whereas controls received a cholesterol (Chol) implant. Behavioural experiments were done between 12 and 14 weeks of age. Left panels show PPI expressed per prepulse intensity (PP) level, whereas right panels show PPI data expressed as average across all prepulse intensities. Data are mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) for 10 rats per group. *P < 0.05 for difference with saline (Sal) treatment. For details of statistical analysis, see text.

Table 2.

Average startle responses of rats after acute treatment of either saline or different doses of apomorphine or amphetamine

ND Chol ND CORT MD Chol MD CORT
Apomorphine cohort
Saline 603 ± 81 725 ± 103 786 ± 88 707 ± 111
0.1 mg·kg−1 380 ± 54* 403 ± 46* 543 ± 53* 589 ± 95
0.3 mg·kg−1 586 ± 69 566 ± 80 597 ± 71* 687 ± 168
1.0 mg·kg−1 856 ± 129 1013 ± 163 914 ± 115 870 ± 161
Amphetamine cohort
Saline 936 ± 123 837 ± 163 967 ± 96 875 ± 79
0.5 mg·kg−1 818 ± 106 756 ± 68 827 ± 126 932 ± 89
2.5 mg·kg−1 805 ± 125 755 ± 67 906 ± 122 1003 ± 128

The rats were subjected to either maternal deprivation (MD) or young-adult corticosterone (CORT) treatment or both. Controls were non-deprived (ND) or received a cholesterol (Chol) control pellet. Data are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). The number of animals in the apomorphine cohort was n = 10 in each group. The number of animals in the amphetamine cohort was n = 10 each, except MD Chol where n = 9. Differences between the groups were analysed with anova.

*

P < 0.05 for difference with saline within group.

Apomorphine treatment caused a dose-dependent reduction of PPI (Figure 2). Both 0.3 and 1.0 mg·kg−1 of apomorphine disrupted PPI in ND Chol and ND CORT animals, whereas only the highest dose induced a significant effect in the MD Chol group and no significant effect of apomorphine was observed in the MD CORT group (Figure 2). There was a significant overall dose-dependent effect of apomorphine treatment [F(3,108) = 22.0, P < 0.001] and a significant pre-treatment (MD) by treatment (CORT) interaction [F(1,36) = 5.0, P = 0.032] (Figure 2).

To exclude any influence of changes in baseline PPI (see above and Figure 2), further analysis was done on data from a balanced selection of animals (n = 5) from each group (Table 3). In these selected animals, after saline treatment there were no differences in average baseline PPI (Table 3) nor was there an interaction with prepulse level (data not shown). Other than the expected main effect of apomorphine treatment on PPI in these selected rats [F(3,48) = 14.0, P < 0.001], there were no other main effects but there was an interaction of CORT treatment and apomorphine dose which was near significance [F(3,48) = 2.8, P = 0.051]. This was more clear if only the 0.3 mg·kg−1 dose was compared with saline treatment [apomorphine dose × CORT interaction F(1,16) = 4.7, P = 0.045]. Post hoc analysis revealed that this dose caused significant disruption of PPI in both Chol-treated groups [effect of apomorphine F(1,8) = 32.7, P < 0.001, no additional effect of MD] but not in CORT-treated groups. This result suggests that CORT treatment, but not MD, affects the action of apomorphine on PPI if the rat groups are balanced for baseline.

Table 3.

Effect of apomorphine (0.1–1.0 mg·kg−1) on prepulse inhibition (PPI) of rat subgroups selected from the first cohort of animals and balanced for baseline

ND Chol ND CORT MD Chol MD CORT
Saline 40.8 ± 2.1 39.7 ± 4.6 40.7 ± 5.4 39.9 ± 3.8
0.1 mg·kg−1 27.2 ± 5.8 36.1 ± 5.6 47.5 ± 4.6 37.8 ± 5.7
0.3 mg·kg−1 14.2 ± 6.1* 30.6 ± 2.2 22.8 ± 5.6* 40.5 ± 7.1
1.0 mg·kg−1 18.0 ± 4.5* 27.4 ± 4.2 24.1 ± 2.3* 35.8 ± 3.1

The rats were subjected to either maternal deprivation (MD) or young-adult corticosterone (CORT) treatment or both. Controls were non-deprived (ND) or received a cholesterol (Chol) control pellet. Data are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) of n = 5 per group. Differences between the groups were analysed with anova (see text for details).

*

P < 0.05 for difference with saline in the same group (paired t-test).

For startle amplitude, analysis of data from all rats in this cohort (Table 2) revealed a main effect of apomorphine treatment [F(3,108) = 22.7, P < 0.001] but no statistical interactions of MD and CORT treatment, as seen for PPI (Table 2).

Experiment 2: effect of amphetamine on PPI and startle

Similar to the apomorphine cohort, there was no significant main effect of either MD or CORT treatment on PPI or startle in saline-treated rats in this cohort (Figure 3, Table 2). In addition to the expected main effect of prepulse level, there was no interaction between prepulse level and any other factor in this cohort.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

The effect of different doses of amphetamine (0.5 and 2.5 mg·kg−1) on prepulse inhibition of startle (PPI) in rats after maternal deprivation (MD) and chronic corticosterone (CORT) treatment. MD consisted of a 24 h separation of the pups from their mothers on postnatal day 9, whereas controls were non-deprived (ND). CORT treatment consisted of subcutaneous implantation of a 100 mg CORT pellet between 8 and 10 weeks of age, whereas controls received a cholesterol (Chol) implant. Behavioural experiments were done between 12 and 14 weeks of age. Left panels show PPI expressed per prepulse intensity (PP) level, whereas right panels show PPI data expressed as average across all prepulse intensities. Data are mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) for 9–10 rats per group. *P < 0.05 for difference with saline (Sal) treatment. For details of statistical analysis, see text.

In contrast to the effect in the apomorphine cohort (above), analysis of the effect of amphetamine on PPI revealed no statistical interaction of MD by CORT treatment (Figure 3). Instead, in addition to the expected main effect of amphetamine dose [F(2,70) = 28.0, P < 0.001], there was an amphetamine dose by CORT treatment interaction [F(2,70) = 4.2, P = 0.019] and a prepulse by amphetamine dose by CORT interaction [F(8,280) = 2.5, P = 0.011]. The dose by CORT interaction was due to significant PPI disruption caused by 0.5 mg·kg−1 amphetamine in Chol-treated animals [ND Chol: F(1,9) = 8.0, P = 0.020 and MD Chol: F(1,8) = 10.5, P = 0.012] being absent in CORT-treated animals. In contrast, significant amphetamine-induced PPI disruption was observed at the dose of 2.5 mg·kg−1 in all groups: ND Chol [F(1,9) = 12.4, P = 0.007], ND CORT [F(1,9) = 13.7, P = 0.005] and MD Chol [F(1,8) = 18.3, P = 0.003], although only at borderline significance in the MD CORT group [F(1,9) = 5.3, P = 0.047]. With respect to the prepulse by amphetamine dose by CORT interaction, further analysis of individual prepulse intensities revealed that at PP2, in addition to a main effect of amphetamine dose [F(2,70) = 6.1, P = 0.004], there was also a dose × CORT interaction [F(2,70) = 5.6, P = 0.005] reflecting that amphetamine reduced PPI at this prepulse intensity only in the Chol-treated groups, but not after CORT treatment (Figure 3, left panel). There were no interactions between amphetamine dose and MD or CORT at any of the other prepulse intensities. Statistical analysis of startle data revealed neither main effects nor interactions (Table 2).

Dopamine D1 and D2 receptor autoradiography

Dopamine D1 and D2 receptor binding was highest in the caudate nucleus, followed by slightly lower levels in the nucleus accumbens core and shell. Dopamine D1 binding was low in the cingulate cortex, whereas D2 receptor binding was close to the level of NSB in this region (Table 4).

Table 4.

Density of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in rats subjected to either maternal deprivation (MD) or young-adult corticosterone (CORT) treatment or both

ND Chol ND CORT MD Chol MD CORT
D1 receptor
Accumbens core 60.1 ± 5.7 59.5 ± 2.5 72.3 ± 5.3 65.5 ± 2.5
Accumbens shell 65.1 ± 6.4 65.1 ± 2.7 80.6 ± 6.3 70.7 ± 3.3
Caudate 67.1 ± 3.8 69.4 ± 3.1 74.0 ± 3.1 73.5 ± 1.5
Cingulate cortex 13.3 ± 0.6 14.0 ± 1.2 13.8 ± 0.5 14.9 ± 0.7
D2 receptor
Accumbens core 59.0 ± 4.0 51.0 ± 2.3 57.1 ± 2.0 64.0 ± 4.2
Accumbens shell 57.5 ± 3.7 50.7 ± 2.9 57.9 ± 1.9 64.8 ± 5.3
Caudate 82.0 ± 4.2 69.3 ± 4.1 76.1 ± 5.0 86.5 ± 5.4
Cingulate cortex 4.5 ± 1.1 4.7 ± 2.6 2.9 ± 0.5 2.4 ± 0.8

Data are mean ± SEM of eight rats per group and expressed as fmol·mg−1 estimated tissue equivalent (ETE). Controls were non-deprived (ND) or received a cholesterol (Chol) control pellet. [3H]SCH23390 (D1 receptor) or [3H]YM091512 (D2 receptor) binding density was assessed by autoradiography.

For D1 receptor levels, there were no main effects or interactions observed in the caudate nucleus. Instead, a significant, although weak, effect of MD was observed in the nucleus accumbens core [F(1,28) = 4.5, P = 0.042] and shell [F(1,28) = 4.4, P = 0.044]. Further analysis revealed no differences between the ND Chol and other groups in any of the brain regions (Table 4). For D2 receptor levels, there was a MD by CORT interaction observed in the caudate nucleus [F(1,28) = 6.1, P = 0.020] and nucleus accumbens core [F(1,28) = 5.1, P = 0.032]. This statistical interaction reflected the tendency for a CORT-induced decrease of D2 receptor density in ND animals compared with a tendency for a CORT-induced increase of D2 receptor density in maternally deprived animals in these regions (Table 4).

Discussion

The present study confirmed and extended our previous observations that MD and CORT treatment induced differential changes in the PPI responses to dopaminergic stimulation by apomorphine and amphetamine (Choy and van den Buuse, 2008). Apomorphine is a direct dopamine receptor agonist, with limited selectivity for dopamine D2 over D1 receptors. As a dopamine releaser, amphetamine indirectly activates dopamine receptors, with no selectivity between them. As expected, the control animals showed dose-dependent disruption of PPI in response to treatment with either of the dopaminergic psychotomimetics. The main finding of our study was that in animals previously treated as young adults with CORT, both the apomorphine- and amphetamine-induced disruption of PPI was attenuated. It also appeared that MD may influence the results through changes in baseline PPI. This could explain some of our previous findings where the greatest inhibition of PPI disruption was observed in rats which had been previously treated with a combination of MD and young-adult CORT (Choy and van den Buuse, 2008). However, if the rat groups were balanced for baseline PPI, as we did in the current study in the apomorphine cohort, or if baseline PPI was not significantly different, as in the amphetamine cohort, the additional effect of MD on drug-induced PPI disruption disappeared. Importantly, the altered responses to dopaminergic drugs were not associated with CORT-induced dopamine D1 or D2 receptor down-regulation in forebrain areas with a major involvement in PPI regulation.

The effect of CORT treatment on the action of apomorphine and amphetamine in the absence of major changes in receptor density suggests long-lasting effects on dopamine receptor signalling or dopamine release involved in PPI. The additional effect of MD through changes in baseline PPI could also involve effects on dopaminergic activity; however, further experiments are needed to confirm this. In any case the effect of MD on baseline PPI in the current experiments (in the first cohort) and our previous studies (Garner et al., 2007; Choy and van den Buuse, 2008) was far smaller than that seen in previous studies using the same protocol (Ellenbroek et al., 1998). The reason for these differences is unclear, but could be related to the use of different Wistar substrains or details of the MD and/or PPI procedure. Interestingly, a previous study focusing on baseline PPI, found that the disruption caused by either MD or post-weaning isolation was absent in animals which had undergone both stress episodes (Ellenbroek and Cools, 2002). This confirms that two stress episodes along the neurodevelopmental trajectory can interact and lead to an apparently reduced disruption of PPI.

The observation that there were only minor changes of dopamine receptor levels in the nucleus accumbens and caudate nucleus is in line with some previous studies, although overall the literature on the effects of MD or CORT treatment on dopamine receptor densities has been inconclusive. For example, MD increased dopamine D1 receptor levels in one study (Brake et al., 2004), although other studies showed no change (Henry et al., 1995; Ploj et al., 2003). Inconclusive results were also reported for dopamine receptors after chronic stress, as simulated in the present study with chronic CORT treatment. For example, unavoidable stresses increased D1 receptor binding density and decreased D2 binding in the nucleus accumbens (Scheggi et al., 2002). In contrast, another study showed that chronic restraint stress caused a decrease of D1 and no change of D2 receptor density in the nucleus accumbens (Giardino et al., 1998), whereas in another study, chronic immobilization stress was reported to cause increased D2 receptor density (Lucas et al., 2007). Prenatal stress or stress during pregnancy caused a significant increase of D2 receptor expression at an adult age (Henry et al., 1995; Berger et al., 2002) although the neonatal MD in our experiment did not. Instead of increasing D2 receptor density, in our study the maternally deprived animals showed slightly higher D1 receptor expression. However, in these animals there were no major changes of the amphetamine and apomorphine effect on PPI, suggesting that the minor D1 receptor up-regulation has little functional consequence. In this context it is of interest to note that the role of dopamine D1 receptors in PPI regulation may be largely focused in the prelimbic frontal cortex (Peng et al., 1990; de Jong and van den Buuse, 2006), where no difference in densities was observed in the present experimental groups.

Because our receptor binding studies did not reveal receptor down-regulation which could explain the apparent CORT-induced desensitization to the effect of apomorphine and amphetamine on PPI, mechanisms ‘behind’ the receptor, that is receptor signalling, are likely to be involved. Other studies have also shown a lack of receptor density or mRNA changes despite alterations in drug responsiveness caused by developmental disruptions (Al-Amin et al., 2001). The identity of the dopamine receptor signalling mechanisms remains to be elucidated. Previously, the effects of chronic treatment with the dopamine D2/D3 receptor agonist, quinpirole, were studied (Culm et al., 2004). These authors showed that the disruption of PPI by acute quinpirole treatment was absent after a 4-week daily treatment with the same drug. This relative insensitivity to quinpirole was associated with increased phosphorylation of cyclic AMP response element binding protein (CREB) which is a second messenger component downstream of dopamine receptors (Culm et al., 2004). This finding could indicate a possible molecular mechanism relevant for our CORT pre-treatment effect, whereby enhanced dopaminergic tone, by chronic quinpirole treatment in the Culm et al. (2004) study or intrinsic in our animals, induces reduced sensitivity to acute dopaminergic stimulation [quinpirole in Culm et al. (2004) or apomorphine/amphetamine in our studies] by enhanced CREB phosphorylation. Another possibility to explain the present results is BDNF, as we have recently found a decrease of BDNF expression in animals that were subjected to both MD and young-adult CORT treatment (Choy et al., 2008). BDNF expression is regulated at the level of CREB phosphorylation (Miyata et al., 2001), and the decrease of BDNF expression could be a reflection of changes in the levels of CREB phosphorylation, which additionally contributes to attenuation of dopaminergic responsiveness in PPI regulation. However, at this point, the association between functional dopaminergic desensitization, CREB phosphorylation and BDNF expression remains speculative and further experiments are needed to explore these links.

Another animal model with relevance to the present data is the apomorphine-unsusceptible (APO-UNSUS) Wistar rat developed by Cools et al. (1990). These animals and their controls, the apomorphine susceptible (APO-SUS) rats, were selectively bred by these researchers according to their responsiveness to a 1.5 mg·kg−1 apomorphine dose assessed by their gnawing scores (Cools et al., 1990). Similar to the present results, these animals displayed a lower D2 receptor-associated PPI response (Rots et al., 1996; van der Elst et al., 2006). These authors furthermore observed lower corticotrophin-releasing hormone mRNA levels in the paraventricular nucleus, lower baseline plasma adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) levels, and lower and shorter plasma ACTH and CORT secretion responses (Rots et al., 1995; de Kloet et al., 1996). The APO-UNSUS also exhibited a smaller supply of dopamine, which was determined by measuring the levels of factors limiting dopamine synthesis, such as tyrosine hydroxylase (van der Elst et al., 2005). Because APO-UNSUS rats appear similar to our CORT-treated animals in terms of low apomorphine responsiveness, it is possible that our animals also exhibit these changes in components of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis. On the other hand, APO-UNSUS exhibited lower dopamine D1 and D2 receptor expression (Rots et al., 1996; van der Elst et al., 2006), which is not in line with our findings of a lack of receptor down-regulation. Furthermore, while the apomorphine responsiveness in APO-UNSUS and APO-SUS rats was linked to changes of mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptor binding and expression in the hippocampus (de Kloet et al., 1996; de Bruin et al., 2001), we have conducted preliminary in situ hybridization measurements of glucocorticoid receptor expression and found it to be not significantly altered in rats after either MD, CORT treatment, or both (unpublished results).

In conclusion, early developmental insults have been implicated in the development of schizophrenia and it has been proposed that two or more developmental insults are required. Our experimental model, combining early stress by MD with later, young-adult stress in the form of CORT treatment, has unmasked significant effects of these neurodevelopmental interventions on dopaminergic regulation of PPI. Particularly young-adult CORT treatment resulted in reduced effects of apomorphine and amphetamine on PPI. In some animals, MD induces a reduction of baseline PPI which added to the effect of CORT. These findings could help to find behavioural and neurochemical mechanisms involved in the effect of stress on schizophrenia development, and also be a potential model for therapeutic approaches.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia. At the time of this study, the Mental Health Research Institute was a Stanley Research Centre, supported by the Stanley Medical Research Institute. Kwok Ho Christopher Choy was supported by a PhD scholarship from the Australian Rotary Health Research Fund.

Glossary

Abbreviations:

ACTH

adrenocorticotropic hormone

APO-UNSUS

apomorphine unsusceptible

APO-SUS

apomorphine-susceptible

BDNF

brain-derived neurotrophic factor

CC

cingulate cortex

CN

caudate nucleus

Chol

cholesterol

CORT

corticosterone

CREB

cyclic-AMP response element binding protein

ETE

estimated tissue equivalent

MD

maternal deprivation

NAc

nucleus accumbens core

Nas

nucleus accumbens shell

ND

non-deprived

NSB

non-specific binding

PPI

prepulse inhibition

TB

total binding

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

  1. Al-Amin HA, Weinberger DR, Lipska BK. Exaggerated MK-801-induced motor hyperactivity in rats with the neonatal lesion of the ventral hippocampus. Behav Pharmacol. 2000;11:269–278. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200006000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Al-Amin HA, Shannon Weickert C, Weinberger DR, Lipska BK. Delayed onset of enhanced MK-801-induced motor hyperactivity after neonatal lesions of the rat ventral hippocampus. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49:528–539. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00968-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alexander SPH, Mathie A, Peters JA. Guide to receptors and channels (GRAC), 3rd edn: 7TM receptors. Br J Pharmacol. 2008;153:S4–S95. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjp.0707746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bayer TA, Falkai P, Maier W. Genetic and non-genetic vulnerability factors in schizophrenia: the basis of the ‘two hit hypothesis’. J Psychiatric Res. 1999;33:543–548. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(99)00039-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Berger MA, Barros VG, Sarchi MI, Tarazi FI, Antonelli MC. Long-term effects of prenatal stress on dopamine and glutamate receptors in adult rat brain. Neurochem Res. 2002;27:1525–1533. doi: 10.1023/a:1021656607278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Billard W, Ruperto V, Crosby G, Iorio LC, Barnett A. Characterization of the binding of 3H-SCH 23390, a selective D-1 receptor antagonist ligand, in rat striatum. Life Sci. 1984;35:1885–1893. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(84)90540-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Braff DL, Geyer MA. Sensorimotor gating and schizophrenia. Human and animal model studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47:181–188. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810140081011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Braff DL, Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA. Gating and habituation deficits in the schizophrenia disorders. Clin Neurosci. 1995;3:131–139. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brake WG, Flores G, Francis D, Meaney MJ, Srivastava LK, Gratton A. Enhanced nucleus accumbens dopamine and plasma corticosterone stress responses in adult rats with neonatal excitotoxic lesions to the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience. 2000;96:687–695. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00002-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brake WG, Zhang TY, Diorio J, Meaney MJ, Gratton A. Influence of early postnatal rearing conditions on mesocorticolimbic dopamine and behavioural responses to psychostimulants and stressors in adult rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;19:1863–1874. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03286.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. de Bruin NM, Ellenbroek BA, van Luijtelaar EL, Cools AR, Stevens KE. Hippocampal and cortical sensory gating in rats: effects of quinpirole microinjections in nucleus accumbens core and shell. Neuroscience. 2001;105:169–180. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(01)00183-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. van den Buuse M, Garner B, Koch M. Neurodevelopmental animal models of schizophrenia: effects on prepulse inhibition. Curr Mol Med. 2003;3:459–471. doi: 10.2174/1566524033479627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cannon TD, van Erp TG, Bearden CE, Loewy R, Thompson P, Toga AW, et al. Early and late neurodevelopmental influences in the prodrome to schizophrenia: contributions of genes, environment, and their interactions. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29:653–669. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Choy KH, van den Buuse M. Attenuated disruption of prepulse inhibition by dopaminergic stimulation after maternal deprivation and adolescent corticosterone treatment in rats. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;18:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2007.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Choy KH, de Visser Y, Nichols NR, van den Buuse M. Combined neonatal stress and young-adult glucocorticoid stimulation in rats reduce BDNF expression in hippocampus: effects on learning and memory. Hippocampus. 2008;18:655–667. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cools AR, Brachten R, Heeren D, Willemen A, Ellenbroek B. Search after neurobiological profile of individual-specific features of Wistar rats. Brain Res Bull. 1990;24:49–69. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(90)90288-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cox RF, Waszczak BL. Autoradiography of dopamine D2 receptors using [3H]YM-09151-2. Eur J Pharmacol. 1991;199:103–106. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(91)90642-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Culm KE, Lugo-Escobar N, Hope BT, Hammer RP., Jr. Repeated quinpirole treatment increases cAMP-dependent protein kinase activity and CREB phosphorylation in nucleus accumbens and reverses quinpirole-induced sensorimotor gating deficits in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1823–1830. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ellenbroek BA, Cools AR. Early maternal deprivation and prepulse inhibition: the role of the postdeprivation environment. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;73:177–184. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00794-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ellenbroek BA, van den Kroonenberg PT, Cools AR. The effects of an early stressful life event on sensorimotor gating in adult rats. Schizophr Res. 1998;30:251–260. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(97)00149-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. van der Elst MC, Roubos EW, Ellenbroek BA, Veening JG, Cools AR. Apomorphine-susceptible rats and apomorphine-unsusceptible rats differ in the tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive network in the nucleus accumbens core and shell. Exp Brain Res. 2005;160:418–423. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2025-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. van der Elst MC, Ellenbroek BA, Cools AR. Cocaine strongly reduces prepulse inhibition in apomorphine-susceptible rats, but not in apomorphine-unsusceptible rats: regulation by dopamine D2 receptors. Behav Brain Res. 2006;175:392–398. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Filloux FM, Wamsley JK, Dawson TM. Presynaptic and postsynaptic D1 dopamine receptors in the nigrostriatal system of the rat brain: a quantitative autoradiographic study using the selective D1 antagonist [3H]SCH 23390. Brain Res. 1987;408:205–209. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90373-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Garner B, Wood SJ, Pantelis C, van den Buuse M. Early maternal deprivation reduces prepulse inhibition and impairs spatial learning ability in adulthood: no further effect of post-pubertal chronic corticosterone treatment. Behav Brain Res. 2007;176:323–332. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Geyer MA, Wilkinson LS, Humby T, Robbins TW. Isolation rearing of rats produces a deficit in prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle similar to that in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1993;34:361–372. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(93)90180-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Giardino L, Zanni M, Pozza M, Bettelli C, Covelli V. Dopamine receptors in the striatum of rats exposed to repeated restraint stress and alprazolam treatment. Eur J Pharmacol. 1998;344:143–147. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(97)01608-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Henry C, Guegant G, Cador M, Arnauld E, Arsaut J, Le Moal M, et al. Prenatal stress in rats facilitates amphetamine-induced sensitization and induces long-lasting changes in dopamine receptors in the nucleus accumbens. Brain Res. 1995;685:179–186. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00430-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. de Jong IEM, van den Buuse M. SCH 23390 in the prefrontal cortex enhances the effect of apomorphine on prepulse inhibition of rats. Neuropharmacology. 2006;51:438–446. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2006.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. de Kloet ER, Rots NY, Cools AR. Brain-corticosteroid hormone dialogue: slow and persistent. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 1996;16:345–356. doi: 10.1007/BF02088100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Landwehrmeyer B, Mengod G, Palacios JM. Differential visualization of dopamine D2 and D3 receptor sites in rat brain. A comparative study using in situ hybridization histochemistry and ligand binding autoradiography. Eur J Neurosci. 1993;5:145–153. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1993.tb00480.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lipska BK, Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Jaskiw GE, Braff DL, Weinberger DR. Neonatal excitotoxic hippocampal damage in rats causes post-pubertal changes in prepulse inhibition of startle and its disruption by apomorphine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995;122:35–43. doi: 10.1007/BF02246439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lucas LR, Wang CJ, McCall TJ, McEwen BS. Effects of immobilization stress on neurochemical markers in the motivational system of the male rat. Brain Res. 2007;1155:108–115. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.04.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. McCabe RT, Hanson GR, Dawson TM, Wamsley JK, Gibb JW. Methamphetamine-induced reduction in D1 and D2 dopamine receptors as evidenced by autoradiography: comparison with tyrosine hydroxylase activity. Neuroscience. 1987;23:253–261. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(87)90287-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Miyata K, Omori N, Uchino H, Yamaguchi T, Isshiki A, Shibasaki F. Involvement of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor/TrkB pathway in neuroprotecive effect of cyclosporin A in forebrain ischemia. Neuroscience. 2001;105:571–578. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(01)00225-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Murray RM, Fearon P. The developmental ‘risk factor’ model of schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. 1999;33:497–499. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(99)00032-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Pantelis C, Yucel M, Wood SJ, McGorry PD, Velakoulis D. Early and late neurodevelopmental disturbances in schizophrenia and their functional consequences. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2003;37:399–406. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2003.01193.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Co-Ordinates. 2nd edn. New York: Academic Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  38. Peng RY, Mansbach RS, Braff DL, Geyer MA. A D2 dopamine receptor agonist disrupts sensorimotor gating in rats. Implications for dopaminergic abnormalities in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1990;3:211–218. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ploj K, Roman E, Nylander I. Long-term effects of maternal separation on ethanol intake and brain opioid and dopamine receptors in male Wistar rats. Neuroscience. 2003;121:787–799. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00499-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Rehn AE, Van Den Buuse M, Copolov D, Briscoe T, Lambert G, Rees S. An animal model of chronic placental insufficiency: relevance to neurodevelopmental disorders including schizophrenia. Neuroscience. 2004;129:381–391. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.07.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rots NY, Cools AR, de Jong J, de Kloet ER. Corticosteroid feedback resistance in rats genetically selected for increased dopamine responsiveness. J Neuroendocrinol. 1995;7:153–161. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.1995.tb00678.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Rots NY, Cools AR, Berod A, Voorn P, Rostene W, de Kloet ER. Rats bred for enhanced apomorphine susceptibility have elevated tyrosine hydroxylase mRNA and dopamine D2-receptor binding sites in nigrostriatal and tuberoinfundibular dopamine systems. Brain Res. 1996;710:189–196. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01379-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Scheggi S, Leggio B, Masi F, Grappi S, Gambarana C, Nanni G, et al. Selective modifications in the nucleus accumbens of dopamine synaptic transmission in rats exposed to chronic stress. J Neurochem. 2002;83:895–903. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2002.01193.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Geyer MA, Koob GF. Central dopamine hyperactivity in rats mimics abnormal acoustic startle response in schizophrenics. Biol Psychiatry. 1986;21:23–33. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(86)90005-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Swerdlow NR, Keith VA, Braff DL, Geyer MA. Effects of spiperone, raclopride, SCH 23390 and clozapine on apomorphine inhibition of sensorimotor gating of the startle response in the rat. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1991;256:530–536. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Swerdlow NR, Taaid N, Halim N, Randolph E, Kim YK, Auerbach P. Hippocampal lesions enhance startle gating-disruptive effects of apomorphine in rats: a parametric assessment. Neuroscience. 2000;96:523–536. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(99)00528-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Tenn CC, Fletcher PJ, Kapur S. Amphetamine-sensitized animals show a sensorimotor gating and neurochemical abnormality similar to that of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2003;64:103–114. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(03)00009-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Unis AS, Roberson MD, Robinette R, Ha J, Dorsa DM. Ontogeny of human brain dopamine receptors. I. Differential expression of [3H]-SCH23390 and [3H]-YM09151-2 specific binding. Dev Brain Res. 1998;106:109–117. doi: 10.1016/s0165-3806(97)00202-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Walker EF, Diforio D. Schizophrenia: a neural diathesis-stress model. Psychol Rev. 1997;104:667–685. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.104.4.667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wan FJ, Swerdlow NR. Intra-accumbens infusion of quinpirole impairs sensorimotor gating of acoustic startle in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1993;113:103–109. doi: 10.1007/BF02244341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wan FJ, Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR. Accumbens D2 modulation of sensorimotor gating in rats: assessing anatomical localization. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1994;49:155–163. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90470-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Yokoyama C, Okamura H, Nakajima T, Taguchi J, Ibata Y. Autoradiographic distribution of [3H]YM-09151-2, a high-affinity and selective antagonist ligand for the dopamine D2 receptor group, in the rat brain and spinal cord. J Comp Neurol. 1994;344:121–136. doi: 10.1002/cne.903440109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from British Journal of Pharmacology are provided here courtesy of The British Pharmacological Society

RESOURCES