Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Jun 23.
Published in final edited form as: J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2009;24(3):192–197. doi: 10.1097/JCN.0b013e31819b54de

Longitudinal Cognitive Performance in Older Adults With Cardiovascular Disease: Evidence for Improvement in Heart Failure

Kelly M Stanek 1, John Gunstad 2, Robert H Paul 3, Athena Poppas 4, Angela L Jefferson 5, Lawrence H Sweet 6, Karin F Hoth 7, Andreana P Haley 8, Daniel E Forman 9, Ronald A Cohen 10
PMCID: PMC2700621  NIHMSID: NIHMS120487  PMID: 19390336

Abstract

Background

Cardiovascular disease (CVD) and particularly heart failure (HF) have been associated with cognitive impairment in cross-sectional studies, but it is unclear how cognitive impairment progresses over time in older adults with these conditions.

Objective

The aim of this study was to prospectively examine cognitive function in patients with HF versus other forms of CVD.

Method

Seventy-five older adults (aged 53–84 years) with CVD underwent Doppler echocardiogram to evaluate cardiac status and 2 administrations of the Dementia Rating Scale (DRS), a test of global cognitive functioning, 12 months apart.

Results

Although DRS performance did not statistically differ between groups at either administration, a significant between-group difference in the rate of cognitive change emerged (λ = 0.87; F = 10.50; P = .002; ω2 = 0.11). Follow-up analyses revealed that patients with HF improved significantly on global DRS performance, whereas patients with other forms of CVD remained stable. More specifically, patients with HF showed improvement on subscales of attention, initiation/perseveration, and conceptualization. Exploratory analyses indicated that higher diastolic blood pressure at baseline was associated with improved DRS performance in patients with HF (r = 0.38; P = .02).

Conclusions

Patients with HF exhibited modest cognitive improvements during 12 months, particularly in attention and executive functioning. Higher diastolic blood pressure at baseline was associated with improvement. These results suggest that cognitive impairment in patients with HF may be modifiable and that improved blood pressure control may be an important contributor to improved function. Further prospective studies are needed to replicate results and determine underlying mechanisms.

Keywords: cardiovascular disease, cognitive impairment, heart failure


Cognitive impairment is a significant and frequent disability in older individuals with cardiovascular disease (CVD). Deficits in multiple cognitive domains have been demonstrated in patients with various types of CVD, including hypertension, atherosclerosis, and myocardial infarction.1 Cognitive impairment is particularly prevalent in patients with heart failure (HF).25 As many as 80% of patients with HF experience some form of cognitive impairment,4 and there is evidence that cognitive deficits in HF are greater than those found in other types of CVD.3 Deficits in attention, executive function, memory, and psychomotor speed are common in patients with HF.47 These impairments likely contribute to a reduced capacity to understand and comply with treatment regimens, worse prognosis, and poor quality of life in patients with HF.4 Cognitive impairment in HF is associated with increased mortality,8 and the degree of impairment is associated with the severity of HF symptoms.3

A growing number of pathophysiological factors have been shown to influence the degree of cognitive impairment in HF, including structural brain pathology, reduced cerebral blood flow, and dysfunction of the autonomic nervous system (ANS).9,10 Patients with HF show many brain abnormalities on neuroimaging, including cerebral atrophy and infarcts,9,11,12 reduced grey matter volume,13 white matter hyperintensities,14,15 and alterations in cerebral metabolism.16,17 Such abnormalities have been strongly related to cognitive dysfunction in persons with CVD.18 Patients with HF also exhibit reduced cerebral perfusion compared with healthy control subjects. One study demonstrated that patients with HF had 31% lower cerebral blood flow than healthy controls.19 Others have found reduced perfusion more specifically in frontal, temporal, and parietal lobes.15,20 Cerebral hypoperfusion contributes to cognitive dysfunction and may be caused by multiple factors.9 There is also substantial evidence for ANS dysfunction in HF.2124 Given that ANS activity, including heart rate and blood pressure, has been linked to cognitive test performance in other populations,2527 ANS dysfunction may also contribute to cognitive impairment in HF.

Several aspects of HF that likely contribute to cognitive impairment can be ameliorated with medical stabilization and treatment (ie, improved cardiac output [CO] and cerebral perfusion, regulation of blood pressure and heart rate), raising the possibility that the cognitive impairment observed in this population might be partly reversible. Although such changes in cardiac function are likely in patients with HF under medical care and are likely to correspond to improvements in cognitive function, few studies have examined longitudinal neurocognitive outcome in HF.

The present study examined the possibility that cognitive function may be modifiable by assessing cognitive function over a 12-month period in persons with varying severity of CVD, including HF. Evidence for global cognitive improvement was expected in patients with CVD under medical care, with such improvement indicating the possibility that the cognitive dysfunction observed in this population might be modifiable. Exploratory analyses were conducted to identify possible mechanisms for cognitive changes.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from local cardiology clinics as part of a larger longitudinal investigation of the neuropsychological consequences of CVD. Individuals with a history of myocardial infarction, cardiac surgery, HF, coronary artery disease, and/or hypertension who were proficient in the English language were eligible to participate. Individuals with current signs of dementia indicated by a score below 24 on the Mini-Mental Status Examination28 and/or a history of significant neurological (eg, stroke, Alzheimer disease) or psychiatric (eg, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder) problems were excluded to avoid confounding explanations for cognitive impairment. A total of 75 participants from the larger project had complete data and were eligible for current analyses. Participants had an average (SD) age of 70 (7.7) years and education of 14.8 (2.6) years, and 37% were female.

Participants were categorized into the HF group if they met any of the following: (1) previous diagnosis by a cardiologist indicated a New York Heart Association (NYHA) rating of class II or III (no patient in the sample had NYHA class IV), (2) review of medical records by the research team cardiologist indicated an NYHA rating of class II or III, or (3) current CO was less than 4 L/min on echocardiogram. Use of this procedure resulted in 40 individuals categorized into the HF group, and the remainder of the sample remained in the non-HF group (n = 35). Although groups differed in CO (t = 5.90; P < .001), they did not differ on any other demographic or medical characteristics. See Table 1.

TABLE 1.

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of 75 Older Adults With Cardiovascular Disease

Heart Failure (n = 40), Mean (SD) No Heart Failure (n = 35), Mean (SD)
Demographic characteristics
 Age, y 69.08 (8.74) 71.3 (6.11)
 Education, y 14.89 (2.41) 14.71 (2.79)
 Female, % 45.0 28.6
Medical characteristics
 CABG/bypass surgery, % 32.5 54.2
 Diabetes, % 15.0 24.2
 Hypertension, % 80.0 74.3
 Average systolic blood pressure, mm/Hg 127.78 (18.10) 129.76 (20.04)
 Average diastolic blood pressure, mm/Hg 67.65 (10.05) 64.99 (7.38)
 Cardiac output, L/min 3.94 (0.79) 4.94 (0.63)
 Ejection fraction 57.38 (10.65) 59.67 (11.63)
 Stroke volume 53.68 (21.89) 54.95 (26.76)
 Fractional shortening 31.31 (9.02) 32.86 (7.44)
 Left ventricular mass 290.27 (96.49) 321.99 (107.53)
 End systolic volume 42.96 (27.01) 37.12 (22.83)
 End diastolic volume 96.43 (42.53) 92.81 (41.31)

Cognitive Measurement

The Dementia Rating Scale (DRS)29 served as an index of global cognitive functioning. The DRS total score is a composite index of performance on 5 sub-scales, namely, (1) attention, which measures working memory and the ability to attend to and execute commands; (2) initiation/perseveration, a measure of verbal fluency and the ability to perform verbal and motor actions; (3) construction, which measures visual-spatial functioning with copy tasks; (4) conceptualization, which assesses abstract concept formation and verbal and visual reasoning; and (5) memory, which provides an index for orientation, short-term and delayed verbal recall, and verbal and visual recognition.29 Thus, the DRS provides a broad assessment of major cognitive domains including attention, executive function, memory, language, and visuospatial ability. The DRS consists of 36 items among the 5 subscales, with a maximum score of 144.

The DRS is a valid and reliable instrument that is sensitive to cognitive impairment in older adults30,31 and has previously been used to assess cognitive impairment in older patients with HF.32 The DRS total score has a reported test-retest correlation of 0.97, and subscales range from 0.61 (attention) to 0.94 (conceptualization).33 The DRS has also demonstrated good internal consistency across samples.29 For instance, the DRS total score had a Cronbach’s α of .84 in a sample of patients with mixed dementia and mild cognitive impairment30 and reported DRS subscale α values range from .75 (memory) to .95 (attention, conceptualization).34 Although a DRS total score cut point of 123 has traditionally been used to indicate dementia, van Gorp and colleagues35 determined that a cut point of 134 provided maximal discriminatory power, with a sensitivity rate of 93% and a specificity rate of 100%, within a sample of 22 patients with Alzheimer disease, 19 patients with vascular dementia, and 12 normal control subjects.

Cardiac Evaluation

A complete, transthoracic echocardiogram with 2-dimensional apical views was obtained from each participant according to standards put forth by the American Society of Echocardiography. These data were used to derive CO, the amount of blood in liters per minute that is pumped from the heart to perfuse the systemic circulation. Stroke volume was calculated as the mean velocity of blood flow leaving the left ventricle recording with Doppler echocardiography times the area of left ventricular outflow tract measured from the 2-dimensional echo image (CO = (TVI × CSA) × HR, where TVI is time velocity integral, CSA is cross-sectional area, and HR is heart rate). Although this method reflects a noninvasive procedure for obtaining CO, previous research has shown that data generated from such noninvasive procedures strongly correlate with Doppler-based CO.36

Blood pressure was measured using an automated blood pressure cuff throughout this cardiac examination. More specifically, a standard inflatable blood pressure occlusion cuff was placed around the upper portion of the participant’s left arm. Systolic blood pressure and diastolic blood pressure were measured with an automatic, noninvasive monitor, the Pressmate 8800 (Colin Medical Instruments Corp, San Antonio, Texas). Blood pressure measurements were collected from participants while resting in a fasted state in a quiet, darkened room at 10-minute intervals for 2 hours.

Procedure

The local institutional review board approved all methods, and all individuals provided written informed consent before participating in this study. Eligibility criteria were evaluated through participant medical history, which was obtained by self-report and corroborated by medical record review as possible. At baseline, cardiac evaluation and DRS administration occurred on separate occasions. Dementia Rating Scale administration was repeated approximately 12 months later.

The DRS was administered and scored according to standard instructions by a trained research assistant under the supervision of a clinical neuropsychologist. This testing was performed in a quiet, dedicated research office. All echocardiograms were conducted by certified hospital-based technicians, with most (>90%) conducted by the same individual. This technician was closely monitored by a certified cardiologist. Technicians were blinded to cognitive test performance.

Analyses

A longitudinal mixed-model design was used to answer the research questions. Preliminary analyses included independent t tests (or χ2 tests for dichotomous dependent variables) to examine between-group differences on relevant medical and demographic characteristics and identify control variables. No between-group differences were statistically significant, and no control variables were identified.

Main analyses included independent t tests to examine group differences in total DRS performance at baseline and follow-up and repeated-measures analysis of variance to examine differential change in total DRS performance from baseline to 12 months. Paired t tests were then used to examine change in DRS performance within each group. To clarify the nature of any change in global cognitive function, repeated-measures multivariate analysis of variance and Holm’s corrected posttests were used to examine change within DRS subscales.

Finally, exploratory correlation analyses were used to identify potential predictors of change in global DRS performance. Within-group change scores were calculated by subtracting baseline from 12-month DRS performance and correlated with theoretically relevant demographic and medical characteristics.

Results

Change in DRS Performance

T tests revealed no significant between-group mean differences in DRS total score at baseline (t = 1.72; P = .09) or at 12-month follow-up (t = −1.68; P = .10). However, repeated-measures analysis of variance showed groups differed in performance over time (λ = 0.87; F = 10.50; P = .002; ω= 0.11). Follow-up tests indicated that patients with HF improved their DRS total score at 12 months (t = −3.60; P = .001; d = 0.57), whereas patients with other forms of CVD remained stable (t = 1.30; P = .20).

Within the HF group, multivariate analysis of variance showed a significant change in DRS subscale performances (λ= 0.51; F = 6.67; P < .001), with Holm’s corrected posttests revealing improvements in attention (F = 11.83; P = .01; d = 0.59), initiation/perseveration (F = 7.32; P = .01; d = 0.64), and conceptualization (F = 21.01; P = .006; d = 0.49) subscales from baseline to 12-month follow-up. No changes emerged for construction (F = 3.44; P = .07) or memory (F = 0.14; P = .71) subscales. Dementia Rating Scale descriptive statistics are reported in Table 2.

TABLE 2.

Baseline and 12-Month Dementia Rating Scale Performance Across Groups

Heart Failure (n = 40)
No Heart Failure (n = 35)
Mean (SD) Range Mean (SD) Range
Baseline
Dementia rating scale total 136.98 (3.93) 127–144 138.69 (4.71) 122–144
 Attention 35.65 (1.35) 32–37 35.97 (1.42) 31–37
 Initiation/perseveration 35.78 (2.03) 29–37 36.11 (1.71) 30–37
 Construction 5.80 (0.52) 4–6 5.63 (1.03) 1–6
 Conceptualization 36.48 (2.09) 31–39 36.00 (2.61) 29–39
 Memory 23.50 (2.28) 16–25 24.26 (1.29) 19–25
12-Month
Dementia rating scale total 139.35 (4.64) 127–144 137.54 (4.67) 124–144
 Attention 36.45 (0.88) 33–37 36.03 (1.15) 33–37
 Initiation/perseveration 36.48 (1.20) 32–37 35.91 (2.38) 26–37
 Construction 5.58 (0.78) 3–6 5.14 (1.92) 2–6
 Conceptualization 37.50 (2.14) 28–39 36.74 (2.43) 28–39
 Memory 23.35 (2.45) 16–25 23.80 (1.66) 20–25

Predictors of Change in Global DRS Score

A significant correlation was found between change in DRS total score and average diastolic pressure at baseline (r = 0.38; P = .02) within the HF group, indicating that higher diastolic blood pressure at baseline was associated with greater improvement in DRS performance. No other significant correlations emerged, although higher systolic blood pressure at baseline approached significance (r = 0.29; P = .07). See Table 3.

TABLE 3.

Bivariate Correlation Between Change in Dementia Rating Scale (DRS) Total Score and Predictor Variables in 40 Older Adults With Heart Failure

Variable DRS Change P
Age 0.02 0.90
Education 0.14 0.40
Sex −0.05 0.78
History of CABG/bypass surgery −0.04 0.85
History of diabetes 0.06 0.82
History of hypertension −0.03 0.70
Average systolic pressure 0.29 0.07
Average diastolic pressure 0.38a 0.02
Cardiac output 0.17 0.29

Abbreviation: CABG, coronary artery bypass graft.

Pearson r coefficients are reported for continuous criterion variables, whereas point biserial r coefficients are reported for sex and medical history variables.

a

Denotes 2-tailed significance at the .05 level.

Discussion

Patients with HF in the current study showed modest improvement in cognitive test performance over a 12-month period. Follow-up analyses indicated significant improvement on tasks involving attention and executive functioning—cognitive abilities that are commonly impaired in persons with HF.47 These findings suggest that cognitive dysfunction may be modifiable in persons with HF and warrant brief discussion.

Little is known about the pattern of cognitive function in older adults with HF over time. Heart failure is risk factor for stroke,37 and more recent work demonstrates that HF nearly doubles the risk of incident dementia in older adults.38 Such findings would suggest that the cognitive impairment observed in cross-sectional studies of HF47 progresses over time in many individuals. However, results from the current study indicate that cognitive decline in patients with HF is not universal over an intermediate-term follow-up and that attention and executive function abilities may actually improve in some persons. Interestingly, a recent pilot study provided preliminary evidence that a structured exercise program may improve cognitive function in HF, consistent with findings in patients with other forms of CVD.39,40 Structured exercise is known to improve numerous pathophysiological processes in patients with HF,4144 which may ultimately improve cerebral blood flow, a known mechanism for cognitive dysfunction in this population.15,19,20 Additional studies are needed to determine the mechanisms for cognitive improvement in patients with HF and whether they ultimately reduce the risk for subsequent stroke or dementia. Such studies should focus on mechanisms that include frontal lobe functions, as improvements in attention and executive function implicate changes in this brain region.45

Clinical Pearl

  • ▪ Cognitive impairment occurs in up to 80% of heart failure patients.

  • ▪ Impairments in attention, executive function, memory, and psychomotor speed are common, but do not necessarily reflect vascular dementia or Alzheimer’s.

  • ▪ Results of the present study suggest that cognitive decline is not universal and cognitive dysfunction may be modifiable in heart failure.

Exploratory analyses found that higher diastolic blood pressure at baseline was associated with improved DRS test performance at 12-month follow-up. Such findings suggest that elevated diastolic blood pressure in persons with HF may be an important factor for cognitive impairment in this population and improved blood pressure control may be associated with improved cognitive function. If replicated, such findings are consistent with the reduced dementia risk for patients with HF who are prescribed antihypertensive medications.37 Such findings would also suggest that elevated blood pressure, an independent risk factor for adverse neurocognitive outcome, might be especially harmful in patients with HF.4648

The current findings are limited in several ways and require clarification. A primary limitation is found in the sampling procedure. Participants with HF for the current study were selected from a larger, parent study and represented only those individuals who completed both baseline and 12-month cognitive assessment. This sample is likely disproportionately composed of patients with HF with good outcome. Larger studies using intent-to-treat analyses and a wider range of HF severity will better characterize “typical” cognitive changes in this population. Similarly, although the current study was adequately powered to identify cognitive changes in patients with HF, the sample size precludes determination of mechanisms of change. Additional studies that determine the relative contribution of structural brain changes, cerebral blood flow patterns, and other factors to cognitive function will provide greater insight into the underlying mechanisms for impairment and most appropriate treatment strategies.11,15 Also, exclusion criteria for this study precluded persons with a Mini-Mental Status Examination score less than 24 from participating, despite the fact that many patients with HF may show this level of severe cognitive impairment. This criterion was important to minimize possible presence of Alzheimer disease in participants, but may have influenced study findings.

In summary, results from the current study indicate that cognitive decline is not universal in persons with HF and that the observed cognitive impairment may be modifiable. Such findings provide important information for both patients with HF and clinicians, as it reminds that cognitive dysfunction in this population does not necessarily reflect vascular dementia or Alzheimer disease. Prospective studies examining the mechanisms for improved test performance may provide key insight into both behavioral and pharmacological targets for intervention and ultimately reduce the substantial burden associated with cognitive impairment in HF.

Contributor Information

Kelly M. Stanek, Graduate Student, Department of Psychology, Kent State University, Ohio.

John Gunstad, Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, Kent State University, Ohio.

Robert H. Paul, Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Brown Medical School, Providence, Rhode Island.

Athena Poppas, Associate Professor, Department of Cardiology, Rhode Island Medical Center, Providence.

Angela L. Jefferson, Alzheimer’s Disease Center; Associate Professor, Department of Neurology, Boston University School of Medicine, Massachusetts.

Lawrence H. Sweet, Assistant Professor, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Brown Medical School, Providence, Rhode Island.

Karin F. Hoth, Assistant Professor, Department of Medicine, National Jewish Health and Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado, Denver.

Andreana P. Haley, Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Texas, Austin.

Daniel E. Forman, Assistant Professor, Division of Cardiology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts; Geriatric Research, Education, and Clinical Care, Veterans Affairs Boston Healthcare System, Massachusetts.

Ronald A. Cohen, Professor, Department of Cardiology, Rhode Island Medical Center, Providence.

References

  • 1.Waldstein SR, Elias MF, editors. Neuropsychology of Cardiovascular Disease. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Zuccala G, Onder G, Pedone C, et al. Cognitive dysfunction as a major determinant of disability in patients with heart failure: results from a multicentre survey. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001;70:109–112. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.70.1.109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Trojano L, Antonelli Incalzi R, Acanfora D, Picone C, Mecocci P, Rengo F. Cognitive impairment: a key feature of congestive heart failure in the elderly. J Neurol. 2003;250:1456–1463. doi: 10.1007/s00415-003-0249-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bennet SJ, Sauvé MJ. Cognitive deficits in patients with heart failure: a review of the literature. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2003;18:219–242. doi: 10.1097/00005082-200307000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Vogels RLC, Weinstein HC, Schroeder-Tanka JM. Cognitive impairment in heart failure: a systematic review of literature. Eur J Heart Fail. 2006;5(1):33–34. doi: 10.1016/j.ejheart.2006.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Vogels RL, Oosterman JM, van Harten B, et al. Profile of cognitive impairment in chronic heart failure. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55(11):1764–1770. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01395.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lavery L, Vander Bilt J, Chang CC, Saxton JA, Ganguli M. The association between congestive heart failure and cognitive performance in a primary care population of elderly adults: the Steel Valley Seniors Survey. Int Psychogeriatr. 2007;2:215–225. doi: 10.1017/S1041610206003449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Zuccala G, Pedone C, Cesari M, et al. The effects of cognitive impairment on mortality among hospitalized patients with heart failure. Am J Med. 2003;115:97–103. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9343(03)00264-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bennett S, Sauve M, Shaw R. A conceptual model of cognitive deficits in chronic heart failure. J Nurs Scholarsh. 2005;37:222–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1547-5069.2005.00039.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Keary T, Gunstad J, Poppas A, et al. Blood pressure variability and dementia rating scale performance in older adults with cardiovascular disease. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2007;20(1):73–77. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e3180335f9f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Schmidt R, Fazekas F, Offenbacher H, et al. Brain magnetic resonance imaging neuropsychological evaluation of patients with idiopathic dilated cardiomyopathy. Stroke. 1991;22:195–199. doi: 10.1161/01.str.22.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Siachos T, Vanbakel A, Feldman D, et al. Silent strokes in patients with heart failure. J Card Fail. 2005;11:485–489. doi: 10.1016/j.cardfail.2005.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Woo M, Macey P, Fonarow G, et al. Regional brain gray matter loss in heart failure. J Appl Physiol. 2003;95:677–684. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00101.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Almeida J, Alves T, Wajngarten M, et al. Late-life depression, heart failure and frontal white matter hyperintensity: a structural magnetic resonance imaging study. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2005;38:431–436. doi: 10.1590/s0100-879x2005000300014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Alves T, Rays J, Fraguas R, et al. Localized cerebral blood flow reductions in patients with heart failure: a study using Tc-HMPAO SPECT. J Neuroimaging. 2005;15:150–156. doi: 10.1177/1051228404272880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lee C, Lee J, Kim J, et al. Cerebral metabolic abnormalities in congestive heart failure detected by proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. J Am Coll Cardiol. 1999;33:1196–1202. doi: 10.1016/s0735-1097(98)00701-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lee W, Lee J, Lim T, et al. Prognostic significance of cerebral metabolic abnormalities in patients with congestive heart failure. Circulation. 2001;102:2784–2787. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.103.23.2784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Paul R, Gunstad J, Poppas A, et al. Neuroimaging and cardiac correlates of cognitive function among patients with cardiac disease. Cerebrovasc Dis. 2005;20:129–133. doi: 10.1159/000086803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Gruhn N, Larsen F, Boesgaard S, et al. Cerebral blood flow in patients with chronic heart failure before and after heart transplantation. Stroke. 2001;2:2530–2533. doi: 10.1161/hs1101.098360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Burra P, Senzolo M, Pizzolato G, et al. Frontal cerebral blood flow is impaired in patients with heart transplantation. Transpl Int. 2002;15:459–462. doi: 10.1007/s00147-002-0448-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Cohn J, Levine T, Olivari M, et al. Plasma norepinephrine as a guide to prognosis in patients with chronic congestive heart failure. N Engl J Med. 1984;311:819–823. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198409273111303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Dimopoulos S, Anastasiou-Nana M, Sakellariou D, et al. Effects of exercise rehabilitation program on heart rate recovery in patients with chronic heart failure. Eur J Cardiovasc Prev Rehabil. 2006;13:67–73. doi: 10.1097/01.hjr.0000198449.20775.7c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Extramatch Collaborative. . Exercise training meta-analysis in patients with chronic heart failure. BMJ. 2004;328:189–190. doi: 10.1136/bmj.37938.645220.EE. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Garet M, Degache F, Pichot V, et al. Relationship between daily physical activity and ANS activity in patients with CHF. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2005;37:1257–1263. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000174881.68546.ec. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Furchtgott E, Subemeyer J. Heart rate and skin conductance during cognitive processes as a function of age. J Gerontol. 1979;34:183–190. doi: 10.1093/geronj/34.2.183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kennedy D, Scholey A. Glucose administration, heart rate, and cognitive performance: effects of increasing mental effort. Psychopharmacology. 2004;149:63–71. doi: 10.1007/s002139900335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Wright C, Kunz-Ebrecht S, Iliffe S, et al. Physiological correlates of cognitive functioning in an elderly population. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30:826–838. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state:” a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Mattis S. Dementia Rating Scale (DRS) Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Smith G, Ivnik R, Malec J, Kokmen E, Tangalos E, Peterson R. Psychometric properties of the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale. Assessment. 1994;1:123–132. doi: 10.1177/1073191194001002002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Green RC, Woodard JL, Green J. Validity of the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale for detection of cognitive impairment in the elderly. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1995;7(3):357–360. doi: 10.1176/jnp.7.3.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gunstad J, Poppas A, Smeal S, et al. Relation of brain natriuretic peptide levels to cognitive dysfunction in adults >55 years of age with cardiovascular disease. Am J Card. 2006;98:538–540. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2006.02.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Coblentz JM, Mattis S, Zingesser LH, Kasoff SS, Wisniewski HM, Katzman R. Presenile dementia: clinical aspects and evaluation of cerebrospinal fluid dynamics. Arch Neurol. 1973;29:299–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1973.00490290039003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Vitaliano PP, Breen AR, Russo J, Albert M, Vitiello MV, Prinz PN. The clinical utility of the Dementia Rating Scale for assessing Alzheimer patients. J Chronic Dis. 1984;37:743–753. doi: 10.1016/0021-9681(84)90043-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.van Gorp WG, Marcotte TD, Sultzer D, Hinkin C, Mahler M, Cummings JL. Screening for dementia: comparison of three commonly used instruments. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1999;21(1):29–38. doi: 10.1076/jcen.21.1.29.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Moulinier L, Venet T, Schiller NB, Kurtz TW, Morris RC, Jr, Sebastian A. Measurement of aortic blood flow by Doppler echocardiography: day to day variability in normal subjects and applicability in clinical research. J Am Coll Cardiol. 1991;17:1326. doi: 10.1016/s0735-1097(10)80143-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Witt BJ, Brown RD, Jr, Jacobsen SJ, et al. Ischemic stroke after heart failure: a community-based study. Am Heart J. 2006;152(1):102–109. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2005.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Qiu C, Winblad B, Marengoni A, Klarin I, Fastbom J, Fratiglioni L. Heart failure and risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease: a population-based cohort study. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(9):1003–1008. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.9.1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Tanne D, Friemark D, Porch A, et al. Cognitive functions in severe congestive heart failure before and after an exercise training program. Int J Cardiol. 2005;103:145–149. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2004.08.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Gunstad J, Macgregor KL, Paul RH, et al. Cardiac rehabilitation improves cognitive performance in older adults with cardiovascular disease. J Cardiopulm Rehabil. 2005;25(3):173–176. doi: 10.1097/00008483-200505000-00009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Jonsdottir S, Andersen K, Sigurosson A, et al. The effect of physical training in chronic heart failure. Eur J Heart Fail. 2006;9:97–101. doi: 10.1016/j.ejheart.2005.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Linke A, Erbs S, Hambrecht R. Exercise and the coronary circulation-alterations and adaptation in coronary artery disease. Prog Cardiovasc Dis. 2006;48:270–284. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2005.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.van Tol B, Huijsmans R, Kroon D, et al. Effects of exercise training on cardiac performance, exercise capacity, and quality of life in patients with heart failure: a meta-analysis. Eur J Heart Fail. 2006;8(8):841–850. doi: 10.1016/j.ejheart.2006.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Fang Z, Marwick T. Mechanisms of exercise training in patients with heart failure. Am Heart J. 2003;145:904–911. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8703(02)94822-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Bonelli RM, Cummings JL. Frontal-subcortical circuitry and behavior. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2007;9(2):141–151. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2007.9.2/rbonelli. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Shah RC, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Arvanitakis Z, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Relation of blood pressure to risk of incident Alzheimer’s disease and change in global cognitive function in older persons. Neuroepidemiology. 2006;26(1):30–36. doi: 10.1159/000089235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Qiu C, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. The age-dependent relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2005;4(8):487–499. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70141-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Owan TO, Hodge DO, Herges RM, Jacobsen SJ, Roger VL, Redfield MM. Trends in prevalence and outcome of heart failure with preserved ejection fraction. N Engl J Med. 2006;355(3):251–259. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa052256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES