Abstract
Background
A basic theme in the study of plant–pollinator interactions is that pollinators select not just for single floral traits, but for associations of traits. Responses of pollinators to sets of traits are inherent in the idea of pollinator syndromes. In its most extreme form, selection on a suite of traits can take the form of correlational selection, in which a response to one trait depends on the value of another, thereby favouring floral integration. Despite the importance of selection for combinations of traits in the evolution of flowers, evidence is relatively sparse and relies mostly on observational approaches.
Scope
Here, methods for measuring selection on multivariate suites of floral traits are presented, and the studies to date are reviewed. It is argued that phenotypic manipulations present a powerful, but rarely used, approach to teasing apart the separate and combined effects of particular traits. The approach is illustrated with data from studies of alpine plants in Colorado and New Zealand, and recommendations are made about several features of the design of such experiments.
Conclusions
Phenotypic manipulations of two or more traits in combination provide a direct way of testing for selection of floral trait associations. Such experiments will be particularly valuable if rooted in hypotheses about differences between types of pollinators and tied to a proposed evolutionary history.
Key words: Colour, correlational selection, experiment, floral integration, multivariate selection, phenotypic manipulation, pollination syndrome, pollinator visitation
INTRODUCTION
Flowers exhibit an astonishing variety of colours, shapes, sizes and positioning of reproductive parts. There is a long tradition of attempting to explain this variety in terms of suites of traits that are associated with one another and/or with a particular type of pollinator. These ideas have taken several forms. One of the best known is the notion of pollination syndromes, in which flowers are grouped into categories that are associated with particular pollinator types (Faegri and van der Pijl, 1966). In such a scheme, flowers of a particular style share not one trait, but a suite of traits. For example, in the Baker and Hurd (1968) scheme, the sphingophily (hawkmoth) syndrome is characterized by flowers that are white or pale, with a strong, usually sweet, odour, usually actinomorphic and held horizontal or pendent, with a deep narrow tube to the corolla, and ample and concealed nectar. Such a suite of characters is thought to reflect convergent evolution of unrelated species due to selection imposed by the perceptual abilities of a pollinator and its ability to transfer pollen efficiently, although the evidence for this process and its applicability is controversial (Waser et al., 1996; Ollerton et al., 2009).
Testing the role of selection by pollinators is helped by an understanding of the myriad forms it can take. Even for a single floral trait, selection can be directional, stabilizing or disruptive. In directional selection, fitness increases with more extreme values of a trait, for example the increase in hawkmoth-mediated pollen export for flowers with an especially deep floral tube in orchids of the genus Platanthera (Nilsson, 1988). In stabilizing selection, an intermediate value is favoured, as illustrated by the peak in male reproductive success for plants of the noctuid moth-pollinated Silene latifolia (Caryophyllaceae) that have intermediate calyx diameters (Wright and Meagher, 2004). Disruptive selection is a pattern in which extreme values are favoured, as in the case of pollinator visitation by the combination of hummingbirds and hawkmoths as a function of corolla width in a hybrid zone of Ipomopsis (Polemoniaceae; Campbell, 2003). When selection on a whole suite of floral traits is considered, selection may also take the form of correlational selection, defined as selection on a combination of traits that is not predictable from examining selection on the two single traits, one at a time. For example, in correlational selection for deep, narrow corolla tubes, the effect of tube length on fitness would depend on the width of the tube. In the simplest case, if each of two traits were controlled by a single gene, correlational selection would be equivalent to epistasis for fitness.
These types of selection on floral traits can be quantified, by examining their separate and combined relationship with fitness. A widely used approach for quantitative traits is phenotypic selection analysis (Lande and Arnold, 1983). Consider two traits, as illustrated in Fig. 1 for corolla length and stamen length, and examine the multiple regression of relative fitness (w) on the two traits (X1, X2), each standardized to a mean of zero and variance of 1.
1 |
In this expression,β1 and β2 measure the strength of directional selection on each trait separately (assuming multivariate normality prior to selection, otherwise directional selection is estimated from regression with just the linear terms), γ11 and γ22 measure the strength of stabilizing or disruptive selection on each of those traits, and γ12 measures the strength of correlational selection (Lande and Arnold, 1983; Phillips and Arnold, 1989; Brodie, 1992). Starting with no directional selection (β1, β2 = 0), stabilizing selection (γ11, γ22 < 0) favours an intermediate value for each trait. Adding moderate correlational selection (Fig. 1) produces a ridge of high fitness such that plants with short stamens and short corollas are as fit as those with long stamens and long corollas. In this example of correlational selection, what matters is the match of the two traits. This result (which depends on the value ofγ12) might represent a situation in which the position of the anther relative to the opening of a corolla tube is critical for efficient transfer of pollen (Conner et al., 2009).
Correlational selection is of special interest because it can be an evolutionary cause of genetic and phenotypic correlations between traits. Simulation models have illustrated the evolution of genetic covariance in response to correlational selection as defined above, provided there are also pleiotropic effects of genes on the two characters (Jones et al., 2003). The correlational selection in Fig. 1 would generate a tighter genetic covariance between corolla length and stamen length. Thus correlational selection is one possible cause of phenotypic integration, defined as the pattern of functional, developmental and/or genetic correlations (Pigliucci, 2003). As this definition implies, such a quantitative genetic pattern can also be produced by common developmental regulation of two traits (Armbruster and Schwaegerle, 1996; Bissell and Diggle, 2008). The role of pollinator-mediated selection in producing floral integration has been addressed primarily by comparing the strength of correlations across types of traits or types of plants, and testing the predictions that plants dependent on more specialized pollinators should show tighter trait integration, and plants should show tighter integration of floral traits than they do of floral with vegetative traits (Berg, 1959; Conner and Via, 1993; Armbruster et al., 1999; but see Ordano et al., 2008).
Many of our most fundamental ideas about floral evolution, including the concept of pollination syndromes, posit that particular pollinators favour certain combinations of floral traits, such that we must view selection on flowers as a multivariate process. To date, this idea has been approached largely with observational methods. Here it is argued that experimental methods relying on phenotypic or genetic manipulation would provide a powerful and complementary means of testing this idea.
Despite long-standing interest in the idea of associated floral traits, there are remarkably few attempts to use experimental approaches (Herrera, 2001). Experimental manipulations of traits have proven a powerful approach to demonstrating selection on individual flower traits, dating back at least to Clements and Long (1923). Examples of floral traits that have been phenotypically manipulated include flower colour, nectar reward, nectar spur length, presence of a corolla lip, positions of stigma and anthers, width of the corolla, petal size and orientation of the flower (Waser and Price, 1983; Nilsson, 1988; Cresswell and Galen, 1991; Mitchell, 1993; Wilson, 1995; Campbell et al., 1996; Fulton and Hodges, 1999; Cresswell, 2000; Temeles and Rankin, 2000; Castellanos et al., 2004). Almost all such studies have, however, manipulated only one trait at a time. Here methods for using trait manipulation to study multivariate selection are outlined, results from case studies are presented and future directions for studying floral associations with experimental methods are previewed.
MEASURING MULTIVARIATE SELECTION
Measuring selection of floral trait associations requires more than one trait to be considered. I discuss three approaches, differing in increasing experimental control over trait values. The first approach is purely observational, in that it uses phenotypic selection methods to measure selection on combinations of traits, as well as individual traits. This can be done using standard methods of phenotypic selection analysis (Fig. 1) or by other statistical methods for describing the selection surface (Schluter and Nychka, 1994). A drawback to this approach is that it is essentially correlational and may suggest spurious relationships that are really due to effects of traits not included in the analysis (Mitchell-Olds and Shaw, 1987), or to environmentally induced correlations between floral traits and fitness (Rausher, 1992). A second approach compares the effects of manipulating one trait (e.g. flower colour) across experiments in which plants either do, or do not, vary naturally in a second trait. Finally, to avoid spurious correlations, a third approach is to manipulate two or more traits in factorial combinations.
Method 1: observational methods for estimating phenotypic selection
This method of studying selection on a set of floral traits has been used by far the most often. Whereas many of these studies have employed multiple regression to control statistically for the effect of phenotypically or genetically correlated traits, most have attempted to estimate directional selection only, and few have allowed for the possibility of correlational selection (Kingsolver et al., 2001). Because this form of selection has not been emphasized in prior reviews of pollinator-mediated selection, I briefly review such studies here.
A pioneering example that included interactions between floral traits is Armbruster's (1990) estimated selection surface for pollination as a function of gland area and gland-to-stigma distance in Dalechampia. Three studies of orchids have tested statistically for correlational selection. Using pollinium receipt as a measure of pollination success in Cypripedium acaule, O'Connell and Johnston (1998) detected an interaction between the effects of flower height and labellum length. Maad (2000) found weak correlational selection on a combination of flowering date and plant size in Platanthera bifolia. Also, correlational selection favoured a combination of high flower number and deep nectaries in the orchid Cyclopogon elatus (Benitez-Vieyra et al., 2006). Cresswell and Galen (1991) observed bumble-bees at four classes of naturally varying flowers of Polemonium viscosum. Handling time was affected by an interaction between flower size and nectar reward, whereas number of flowers probed was influenced only by the reward. Gómez (2000) found that white-flowered plants with large displays make more seeds in Lobularia maritima, even though this plant is pollinated by insects thought to be highly generalized. Also, in Erysimum mediohispanicum there were non-additive effects of flower number and flower size on seed production (Gómez et al., 2006). A full understanding of correlational selection requires not just detection of a trait interaction but demonstration of a functional mechanism to explain it. One example is provided by Raphanus raphanistrum (Brassicaceae), in which there is stabilizing selection for the difference between stamen and corolla tube length, which is equivalent to correlational selection on the two traits combined (Conner, 2006; Conner et al., 2009). In this case, an intermediate anther position appears most effective in placing pollen onto sweat bee pollinators.
Illustration of methods using Ipomopsis
To illustrate the shape of a selection surface and compare this observational method with experimental methods for estimating selection on trait combinations, I re-analyse data on hummingbird visitation at flowers of Ipomopsis (Polemoniaceae), previously reported by Campbell et al. (1997). This study examined the rate of visitation to potted plants in experimental arrays that included plants of two species and their natural hybrids. The red-flowered I. aggregata also has relatively wide, short corolla tubes, in comparison with the white-flowered I. tenuituba. The original analysis used multiple regression to estimate directional selection on three traits: colour as measured by maximal optical density between a wavelength of 507 and 530 nm, corolla width and corolla length. I re-analysed these data to test for other forms of selection by adding quadratic terms and two-way products of traits to the multiple regression. No selection on corolla length was detected, so I dropped that trait for simplicity. Nectar reward was not measured, and it is known to influence hummingbird visitation (Mitchell, 1993). For this and other reasons described below, the analysis should be considered an illustration of methods for measuring selection of trait associations, rather than a definitive example.
Fitting eqn (1) revealed an interaction between the effects of flower colour and corolla width (F1,71 = 6·20, P < 0·0151) in addition to directional selection favouring large values for both traits (P < 0·0023 for width and P <0·0105 for colour), but no stabilizing or disruptive selection. The interaction coefficient had a negative sign and thus was in the opposite direction from predicted if hummingbirds are cuing in on the reddest, widest flowers, as might be expected from the hummingbird syndrome. Instead, increasing the width of the flower increased visit rate more than expected for the pale flowers, as compared with the red ones, such that expected visitation is actually highest for a combination of narrow and red flowers (Fig. 2A), in part due to one exceptionally attractive plant. When the data are broken down by type of plant, for hybrid plants with intermediate colours, pollinators responded by preferentially visiting those with wider-tubed flowers, but showed no such response for red-flowered plants of I. aggregata (which were visited heavily overall) or the very pale-coloured flowers of I. tenuituba (Fig. 2A).
One statistical limitation of this approach is that a combination of linear and quadratic terms is not guaranteed to describe selection well, even for a single trait. An alternative way to describe the selection surface is by measuring selection on a combination of traits, for example with projection pursuit regression (Schluter and Nychka, 1994). To my knowledge, this method has rarely been used with floral traits (Maad, 2000). It starts by finding a linear combination of the original traits that explains as much of the variation in the fitness component (here visitation rate) as possible. In this case, the first projection loads only slightly more heavily on standardized corolla width compared with flower colour. Notice that the projection does not go through the direction that explains the greatest variation in the explanatory traits, but a direction that explains variation in pollinator visitation (Fig. 2A). Along this slice through the selection surface, visit rate is predicted to rise steeply. Predicted visit rate can now be fitted with a non-parametric cubic spline regression (Fig. 2B), rather than assuming that the form must be described by a certain number of parameters. In this case, the pursuit projection is almost perfectly linear, suggesting that the selection by hummingbirds is mostly directional, favouring red flowers and wide tubes, with a weak contribution of any correlational selection.
Further analysis of pollinator responses would benefit from experimental manipulation to extend the range of trait values to create critical combinations. In this data set, for example, there are few plants with narrow, red flowers. It would also be valuable to use this method to consider the form of selection generated by the combined visitation of hummingbirds and hawkmoths in this Ipomopsis hybrid zone. Since the two species are not fully reproductively isolated (Campbell, 2003), it is of interest to consider selection on the full range of flowers by both pollinator types combined. Such selection could take the form of correlational selection. For example, if hummingbirds strongly prefer short, wide tubes while hawkmoths prefer long, narrow tubes, then the effect of tube length on overall visitation would depend on the width of the tube. Even if each pollinator were exerting directional selection on the two traits, the combined effect of two pollinators could be the production of two peaks on a selection surface, one corresponding to bird visitation and one to moth visitation, with a fitness minimum in between.
Method 2: manipulation of one trait with natural variation in other traits
A second method of investigating selection of trait associations involves manipulating one trait against a background in which other traits vary to different degrees. This approach goes beyond a simple manipulation of a single trait in that it allows assessment of whether the effect of that trait is context dependent. My colleagues and I have used this approach with experimental arrays of I. aggregata, I. tenuituba and natural hybrids, in this case to analyse the effects of flower colour vs. all other traits that separate the species (Meléndez-Ackerman and Campbell, 1998).
Although rarely included in studies of multivariate selection, flower colour is a good choice of a trait for experimental methods, as it is relatively easily manipulated. Flowers can be painted to match certain reflectance spectra (Meléndez-Ackerman et al., 1997), and control flowers painted their natural colour can serve as controls to rule out the effects of the paint itself. Even so, a recent review by Rausher (2008) found just two cases where flower colour had been phenotypically manipulated: Delphinium nelsonii and Ipomopsis, both studied in sub-alpine meadows in Colorado. In D. nelsonii, the colour contrast provided by a nectar guide reduced handling time and increased visitation by hummingbirds and bumble-bees (Waser and Price, 1983, 1985).
In the Ipomopsis study, three types of arrays were observed: a single-species array with flowers painted different colours, a mixed array with all natural variation intact, and a mixed array with flowers painted the same colour but with other natural variation present (Meléndez-Ackerman and Campbell, 1998). The three types of arrays were used to sort out the role of colour relative to the roles of other floral traits in influencing visitation. A single-species array with flower colour manipulated tests for the role of colour alone. A mixed array with all plant types painted the same colour tests for the role of other floral traits. An unmanipulated array tests for the combined influence of colour and other floral traits that differ between the species. Ideally, arrays would be observed simultaneously to assess the relative effects of each combination without confounding changes in pollinator behaviour over time. In our study, we found by far the largest difference in fitness (seeds set and seeds sired) in the array with all natural variation intact, which, in combination with visitation data, suggested that the hummingbird pollinators responded to a combination of traits and not just colour (Meléndez-Ackerman and Campbell, 1998). The differences in visitation with all traits intact was larger (relative fitnesses of I. aggregata, hybrids and I. tenuituba = 1·00, 0·25 and 0·27, respectively) than predicted by adding the fitness effects of just colour (fitnesses = 1·00, 0·80 and 0·25, respectively) and the remaining traits that separate the species (fitnesses = 0·83, 1·00 and 0·96, respectively). Thus, the hummingbirds responded to traits in a non-additive way, consistent with correlational selection, although with this approach it is not possible to identify which specific traits other than colour were involved.
Floral traits in the New Zealand alpine
Method 2 as described above allows separation of the effects of flower colour from the effects of all other trait differences between two or more types of plants. The use of single flowers or flower heads instead of whole plants also allows one to dissect out effects of floral display when examining responses of flower visitors. My colleagues and I are taking this approach with flowers visited by insects in the New Zealand alpine [D. Campbell, M. Bischoff (U. Otago, New Zealand) and A. Robertson (Massey University, New Zealand), unpubl. res.]. This habitat provides an extreme test case for the role of flower colour, as about 70 % of the species in the flora have white flowers, one of the highest percentages anywhere on Earth (Wardle, 1978), even though many have congeners elsewhere that are brightly coloured (Lloyd, 1985). The preponderance of white has traditionally been attributed to the historical absence of long-tongued social bees (Gibbs, 2006), with the implication that the insects which are present do not have preferences based on colour.
The study site is a set of natural meadows in the Remarkables Mountains on the South Island of New Zealand. We have begun exploring selection on flower colour by providing choices between white and yellow flowers (the second most common colour in the area). The set of experiments allows investigation of whether responses of particular insect pollinators to colour depend on the context of other floral traits. Each experimental array contained 16 cut flowers, of two types, in 1·5 mL tubes filled with water and spaced 10 cm apart. We calculated the proportion of visits made in a foraging bout to one type of flower and compared its mean with the null hypothesis of 0·5 using a one-sample t-test, following arcsin transformation.
At unmanipulated arrays containing eight flowers of each of two species, both colletid bees (Hylaeus sp.) and syrphid flies (Allograpta spp.) showed preferences for certain flower species. We next performed manipulations of colour in single-species arrays of Brachyscome sinclarii and Brachyglottis bellidioides (both Asteraceae). Acrylic paints were used to match the visible spectra of white flowers to those of naturally yellow flowers, and vice versa. These paints also block UV reflectance. Since the experiments involved controls of flowers painted both colours, UV reflectance was absent in both treatments, and responses apply only to signals in the human-visible range. These manipulations resulted in overvisitation of yellow flowers compared with white flowers for both syrphid flies and colletid bees when visiting Brachyglottis bellidioides, but not for syrphid flies visiting B. sinclarii, which is normally white (Fig. 3A).
Both types of insects also showed preferences based on other floral traits, and so it is of interest to ask how the two kinds of traits interact. For Allograpta, responses to other traits depended on flower colour, with the flies switching preference from Brachyscome to Brachyglottis when flowers of both species were painted yellow rather than white (Fig. 3B). Furthermore, they showed a preference for yellow only when foraging on the normally yellow-flowered species, B. bellidioides. The interaction between effects of the two types of traits can also be seen by comparing preferences in the three types of arrays. With all flower differences intact, these syrphid flies made 67 % of visits to Brachyglottis. With only colour varying (Fig. 3A), the average of the two responses (across the two species) was 62 % of visits to the yellow flowers, which is consistent with their preference for Brachyglottis. However, when flowers of the two species were painted the same colour, so that only morphology and rewards differed, they actually preferred on average to visit Brachyscome instead, with the two responses averaging only 0·375 of the visits to Brachyglottis (Fig. 3B). Creation of other trait combinations is still required to understand fully how these insects respond to trait associations.
Method 3: factorial manipulation of traits
The most direct way to test for selection of trait associations is by producing experimental combinations of traits in a factorial design, through either phenotypic or genetic manipulation. This method allows testing directly for (a) responses of pollinators to a suite of multiple traits, as suggested by pollination syndromes, and (b) responses that are not additive, i.e. for the presence of correlational selection. Surprisingly, this method has hardly been used. A few studies have examined the effects on pollination of manipulating several traits in a single flower species one at a time, for example Wilson's (1995) study of pollen removal and deposition in Impatiens pallida; However, it is extremely rare for multiple traits to be altered in a factorial combination. In one exception, Herrera (2001) reduced or completely excised the upper and/or lower corolla lip of Lavandula latifolia. The normal, non-manipulated phenotype had no pollination or fecundity advantage, arguing against the presence of correlational selection, but it is also a case in which no effect of either trait by itself was detected either. To my knowledge, no other studies of selection using phenotypic manipulation of two or more floral traits in factorial combination have been reported. Recently, however, Kessler et al. (2008) used genetic manipulation to block expression of a floral fragrance attractant and a nectar repellant in all combinations in Nicotiana attenuata. Both the repellant and the attractant were required for maximal fitness. Capsule production was very low in all of the transformed plants compared with the wild type, and correlational selection for a combination of scents was supported.
DESIGN OF FUTURE EXPERIMENTS
The basic idea of manipulating traits in combination to assess selection of floral trait associations seems straightforward. However, many features of the experimental design will require careful attention.
Choice of traits
Perhaps the most basic is the choice of traits to manipulate. The paucity of experiments probably reflects in part a sense that progress on a multivariate question can be made more rapidly with observational studies that can easily include many traits simultaneously. However, judicious choice of a relatively small number of traits could be highly informative. For example, factorial manipulations of two or three traits critical to a particular pollinator syndrome could go a long ways towards testing its validity.
How many traits at a time?
A related question is the number of traits to manipulate simultaneously. Imagine three scenarios. In the first, natural flowers receive either extra nectar or none. In the second, this manipulation of nectar is crossed with manipulation of flower size (large vs. small) and with flower colour (coloured vs. white), producing a choice between eight types of flowers. In the third scenario, a choice is offered between white flowers with no extra nectar, white flowers with extra nectar and coloured flowers with extra nectar. A pollinator may respond very differently to the same manipulation of nectar in these scenarios. In both of the first two scenarios there is no correlation between nectar reward and other traits. However, a pollinator might be more likely to respond to nectar in the first scenario because it is simpler to remember locations of rewarding plants when not faced with unfamiliar new morphologies. In contrast, in the third scenario extra nectar has a correlation with flower colour, which may cause pollinators to choose the coloured flowers, but show no greater tendency to visit white flowers with ample nectar as compared with white flowers with no nectar. Thus, pollinator response to one trait may depend on the range of choices presented.
This dependence of pollinator response to the range of choices means that different experiments may be required to study behaviour in the current ecological context (e.g. where trait correlations are already present as in the third scenario) and to study behaviour as it may have occurred under conditions when a new mutation had been introduced but new trait correlations had not yet evolved (as in the second scenario). Choosing the scenario relevant to the evolutionary stage of interest may be particularly important for the study of traits thought to be key innovations, such as the evolution of nectar spurs in Aquilegia (Hodges and Arnold, 1995). In such a case, the order of introduction of a new character may matter, for example pollinators may respond to addition of nectar only in flowers that have deep nectar spurs.
Number of trait values and some limitations to the approach
The difficulty in studying large numbers of trait combinations to characterize a multivariate selection surface fully is perhaps the biggest limitation on the experimental approach. A set of two values per trait is the minimal number required to test for interactions in effects of traits, but it does not give much information on the shape of the surface. However, if the intent is to understand differences between two hybridizing flower species, two values per trait can be extremely informative, as the trait values can be altered to match the species means. As an illustration, consider the two species of Ipomopsis studied in Fig. 2A. The dashed trapezoid connects the four combinations of flower colour and corolla width that correspond to the mean values for I. aggregata and I. tenuituba. An experiment employing these trait values, i.e. painting flowers to match one of the two species and manipulating width to match one of the two species, would be expected to detect correlational selection, as the change in width makes more difference for the pale-coloured category of flowers, changing predicted visitation from 0·59 to 1·32 compared with staying nearly constant (from 1·42 to 1·41) for the darkly coloured category.
Performing manipulations: phenotypic vs. genetic manipulation
Flower traits are in many ways especially amenable to phenotypic manipulation. Unlike animal surgery, sham controls for the operation may be quick and easy, e.g. applying a coat of paint to match the natural colour, inserting a syringe without sugar water, or cutting and refastening a flower to its original shape. It is critical to consider such sham treatments, as experimental manipulations can have unintended effects. As an alternative, molecular genetic tools in principle also offer the option of manipulating traits genetically, as illustrated above with a study of floral scents. Isogenic lines have also been used to study the impact of single genes for flower colour on pollinator visitation at Mimulus (Bradshaw and Schemske, 2003). One important difference between phenotypic and genetic manipulation approaches is that the former alters only the target trait, whereas the latter may alter other traits if there are pleiotropic effects of the gene. If the intent is to examine the evolutionary consequences of introduction of a new gene, the latter method may be the most appropriate. For example, the flavonoids responsible for some flower colours can influence vegetative as well as floral tissue (Rausher, 2008), and have been shown to protect against UV radiation and provide defence against herbivores and/or pathogens (Frey, 2004; Strauss and Whittall, 2006). Thus, the evolution of flower colour may respond to other selective pressures besides those mediated by pollinators, and phenotypic manipulation of just the flower may miss those. On the other hand, for the ecological question of whether a certain type of pollinator responds to flower colour and other traits in the way suggested by the idea of pollinator syndromes, phenotypic manipulation of the flower is a more direct approach. Finally, it would be possible to use artificial flowers to understand some of these mechanisms (Smith et al., 1996), but at the considerable cost of lower realism.
Fitness components
The question of the response variables to include in the study is common to both observational and experimental approaches to the study of selection, so I will not discuss it in detail. Nevertheless, it is important to keep in mind that some aspects of floral integration are thought to arise from pollinator behavioural responses, necessitating inclusion of variables such as visitation rate or handling time (Temeles, 1996). Others are thought to depend on the way in which morphological fit of a pollinator affects the efficiency of pollen transfer, and thereby require tracking pollen removal and/or deposition (Castellanos et al., 2004). Furthermore, if the intent is to show that pollinators lead to selection on combinations of traits, it is necessary to measure both the functional impacts on pollinators or pollen movement and estimates of fitness that include seed production and seeds sired.
Spatial scale
A variety of pollinators have been shown to respond differently to floral traits depending on aspects of spatial variation. Foraging theory suggests that an animal making a choice among simultaneously available resources should exhibit higher selectivity when choosing among rich and poor resources in distinct patches than when choosing among resources mixed within a patch, provided the animals know the locations of rich and poor resources (Mitchell, 1989). This is because the marginal cost of using a poor resource is lower when resources are mixed within a patch. There is evidence, from experiments with feeders, that some hummingbirds behave this way (Mitchell, 1989). Bumble-bees also respond to the spatial patterning of resources. In one study, the approach rate of bumble-bees was higher to nectar-rich plants when foraging in a sparse population of Echium vulgare, but not when foraging in a dense population (Klinkhamer and van der Lugt, 2004).
This dependence of behaviour on spatial scale raises questions on how to design a study of selection on trait associations. Even with just two traits and two character states of each, an experiment could be designed in one of three ways (Fig. 4): (1) with all four flower types randomized in a single array (Fig. 4A); (2) with two arrays representing the two character states of one trait, each involving a choice between the character states of the other trait (Fig. 4B); or (3) choice among four patches of flowers, each of a different type (Fig. 4C). These arrangements may elicit different responses depending, for example, on the spatial memory of the pollinator.
Temporal scale
One of the trickiest problems in studying pollinator responses to floral traits is that many pollinators exhibit rapid learning, as illustrated by bumble-bees (Heinrich et al., 1977) and hawkmoths (Riffell et al., 2008). As a result, their responses to floral trait associations may depend on the frequency with which they have previously experienced them (Cresswell and Galen, 1994). Due to learning, initial responses of pollinators to novel combinations of traits presented in an experiment may not correspond to longer-term behaviour that would lead to evolutionary responses to selection. The potential effect of temporal scale is illustrated by an experiment examining responses of bumble-bees to artificial inflorescences with all four combinations of high vs. low flower number and high vs. zero sugar reward (Makino and Sakai, 2007). During the early phases of the experiment, bees chose displays with high flower number regardless of reward, but switched within 3 h to visiting primarily inflorescences that combined high flower number and reward. Thus, depending on the point in time at which results were taken, the results switched from additive effects of traits to detection of a significant interaction. Further experiments showed that the bees had learned the spatial location of the rewarding inflorescences (Makino and Sakai, 2007).
CONCLUSIONS AND SUGGESTED EXPERIMENTS
Given the long history of discussing pollination in terms of suites of associated traits, it is remarkable that there are so few experimental tests of how pollinators respond to multiple traits and whether they respond in a non-additive way to combinations of floral traits. Here I make a call for experiments testing for such responses as one critical approach to the study of pollinator-mediated evolution.
Two kinds of experiments may be especially valuable. The first type is a comparative experiment with different kinds of pollinators that tests for differences in selection hypothesized to correspond to contrasting modes of pollination. Such predictions could be based on traditional descriptions of syndromes, in which case the experiments could provide direct evidence for or against selection on the floral features thought to characterize syndromes. Furthermore, if the floral associations represent syndromes, such selection by a particular pollinator type would be expected to be similar across a broad set of plant families. Alternatevely, the predictions could be more firmly rooted in multivariate analysis of associations between floral traits and pollinators. For example, based on ordination of floral traits in Penstemon (Wilson et al., 2004), one might predict that hummingbirds would visit Penstemon flowers that are red with strongly exserted anthers, whereas bees would visit primarily yellow or blue-violet flowers with inserted anthers. Finally, such predictions could be based on the perceptual and cognitive abilities of pollinators, at least in cases such as honeybees where they are well understood (Briscoe and Chittka, 2001).
A second valuable type of experiment would be one that maps on to a phylogeny in such a way that the order in which traits probably evolved can be taken into account. For example, if phylogenetic information indicates that a particular flower colour (e.g. white rather than blue) is more recently derived than a particular petal size (e.g. small rather than large), one might predict that pollinators would prefer white over blue if flowers are small (matching the step in the phylogeny), but not necessarily if flowers are large. Such information can help in deciding certain aspects of the experimental design, such as the spatial scale. The scenario just described would support the use of the spatial arrangement shown in Fig. 4B, but in which arrays of small and large flowers are observed simultaneously.
Both a proposed evolutionary history and knowledge of the sensory abilities of potential pollinators could greatly inform our study of selection and the extent to which pollinators choose particular trait combinations. Even if such information is not available, however, it is time to make use of the power of experiments, along with observational methods, to characterize the multivariate nature of pollinator responses to suites of floral traits.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I am grateful to Mascha Bischoff, Jeff Conner, Nelida Pohl, Alastair Robertson, Nickolas Waser and an anonymous reviewer for their valuable discussion of ideas and comments on the manuscript.
LITERATURE CITED
- Armbruster WS. Estimating and testing adaptive surfaces: the morphology and pollination of Dalechampia blossoms. American Naturalist. 1990;135:14–31. [Google Scholar]
- Armbruster WS, Schwaegerle KE. Causes of covariation of phenotypic traits among populations. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 1996;9:261–276. [Google Scholar]
- Armbruster WS, Di Stilio V, Tuxill JD, Flores C, Velásquez Runk JL. Covariance and decoupling of floral and vegetative traits in nine neotropical plants: a re-evaluation of Berg's correlation–pleiades concept. American Journal of Botany. 1999;86:39–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker HG, Hurd PDJ. Intrafloral ecology. Annual Review of Entomology. 1968;13:385–414. [Google Scholar]
- Benitez-Vieyra S, Medina AM, Glinos E, Cocucci AA. Pollinator-mediated selection on floral traits and size of floral display in Cyclopogon elatus, a sweat bee-pollinated orchid. Functional Ecology. 2006;20:948–957. [Google Scholar]
- Berg RL. A general evolutionary principle underlying the origin of developmental homeostasis. American Naturalist. 1959;93:103–105. [Google Scholar]
- Bissell EK, Diggle PK. Floral morphology in Nicotiana: architectural and temporal effects on phenotypic integration. International Journal of Plant Sciences. 2008;169:225–240. [Google Scholar]
- Bradshaw HD, Jr, Schemske DW. Allele substitution at a flower colour locus produces a pollinator shift in monkeyflowers. Nature. 2003;426:176–178. doi: 10.1038/nature02106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briscoe AD, Chittka L. The evolution of color vision in insects. Annual Review of Entomology. 2001;46:471–510. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brodie ED. Correlational selection for color pattern and antipredator behavior in the garter snake Thamnophis ordinoides. Evolution. 1992;46:1284–1298. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1992.tb01124.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell DR. Natural selection in Ipompsis hybrid zones: implications for ecological speciation. New Phytologist. 2003;161:83–90. [Google Scholar]
- Campbell DR, Waser NM, Price MV. Mechanisms of hummingbird-mediated selection for flower width in Ipomopsis aggregata. Ecology. 1996;77:1463–1472. [Google Scholar]
- Campbell DR, Waser NM, Meléndez-Ackerman EJ. Analyzing pollinator-mediated selection in a plant hybrid zone: hummingbird visitation patterns on three spatial scales. American Naturalist. 1997;149:295–315. [Google Scholar]
- Castellanos MC, Wilson P, Thomson JD. ‘Anti-bee’ and ‘pro-bird’ changes during the evolution of hummingbird pollination in Penstemon flowers. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2004;17:876–885. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00729.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clements FE, Long FL. Experimental pollination: an outline of the ecology of flowers and insects. Washington, DC: Carnegie Institute; 1923. [Google Scholar]
- Conner J, Via S. Patterns of phenotypic and genetic correlations among morphological and life-history traits in wild radish, Raphanus raphanistrum. Evolution. 1993;47:704–711. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb02128.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conner JK. Ecological genetics of floral evolution: In. In: Harder LD, Barrett SCH, editors. Ecology and evolution of flowers. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Conner JK, Sahli H, Karoly K. Tests of adaptation: functional studies of pollen removal and estimates of natural selection on anther position in wild radish. Annals of Botany. 2009;103 doi: 10.1093/aob/mcp071. 1547–1556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cresswell JE. Manipulation of female architecture in flowers reveals a narrow optimum for pollen deposition. Ecology. 2000;81:3244–3249. [Google Scholar]
- Cresswell JE, Galen C. Frequency dependent selection and adaptive surfaces for floral trait combinations: the pollination of Polemonium viscosum. American Naturalist. 1991;138:1342–1353. [Google Scholar]
- Faegri K, van der Pijl L. The principles of pollination ecology. Oxford: Pergamon; 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Frey FM. Opposing natural selection from herbivores and pathogens may maintain floral-color variation in Claytonia virginica (Portulacaceae) Evolution. 2004;58:2426–2437. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2004.tb00872.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fulton M, Hodges SA. Floral isolation between Aquilegia formosa and Aquilegia pubescens. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1999;226:2247–2252. [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs G. Ghosts of Gondwana: the history of life in New Zealand. New Zealand: Craig Potton Publishing; 2006. Nelson. [Google Scholar]
- Gómez JM. Phenotypic selection and response to selection in Lobularia maritima: importance of direct and correlational components of natural selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2000;13:689–699. [Google Scholar]
- Gómez JM, Perfetti F, Camacho JPM. Natural selection on Erysimum mediohispanicum flower shape: insights into the evolution of zygomorphy. American Naturalist. 2006;168:531–545. doi: 10.1086/507048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinrich B, Mudge PR, Deringis PG. Laboratory analysis of flower constancy in foraging bumblebees: Bombus ternarius and B. terricola. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1977;2:247–265. [Google Scholar]
- Herrera CM. Deconstructing a floral phenotype: do pollinators select for corolla integration in Lavandula latifolia? Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2001;14:574–584. [Google Scholar]
- Hodges SA, Arnold ML. Spurring plant diversification: are floral nectar spurs a key innovation? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1995;262:343–348. [Google Scholar]
- Jones AG, Arnold SJ, Burger R. Stability of the G-matrix in a population experiencing pleiotropic mutation, stabilizing selection, and genetic drift. Evolution. 2003;57:1747–1760. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00583.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler D, Gase K, Baldwin IT. Field experiments with transformed plants reveal the sense of floral scents. Science. 2008;321:1200–1202. doi: 10.1126/science.1160072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kingsolver JG, Hoekstra HE, Hoekstra JM, et al. The strength of phenotypic selection in natural populations. American Naturalist. 2001;157:245–261. doi: 10.1086/319193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klinkhamer PGL, van der Lugt PP. Pollinator service only depends on nectar production in sparse populations. Oecologia (Berlin) 2004;140:491–494. doi: 10.1007/s00442-004-1569-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lande R, Arnold SJ. The measurement of selection on correlated characters. Evolution. 1983;37:1210–1226. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1983.tb00236.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lloyd DG. Progress in understanding the natural history of New Zealand plants. New Zealand Journal of Botany. 1985;23:707–722. [Google Scholar]
- Maad J. Phenotypic selection in hawkmoth-pollinated Platanthera bifolia: targets and fitness surfaces. Evolution. 2000;54:112–123. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2000.tb00012.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makino TT, Sakai S. Experience changes pollinator responses to floral display size: from size-based to reward-based foraging. Functional Ecology. 2007;21:854–863. [Google Scholar]
- Meléndez-Ackerman EJ, Campbell DR. Adaptive significance of flower color and inter-trait correlations in an Ipomopsis hybrid zone. Evolution. 1998;52:1293–1303. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1998.tb02011.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meléndez-Ackerman EJ, Campbell DR, Waser NM. Hummingbird behavior and mechanisms of selection on flower color in Ipomopsis. Ecology. 1997;78:2532–2541. [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell RJ. Adaptive significance of Ipomopsis aggregata nectar production: observation and experiment in the field. Evolution. 1993;47:25–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb01196.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell WA. Informational constraints on optimally foraging hummingbirds. Oikos. 1989;55:145–154. [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell-Olds T, Shaw RG. Regression analysis of natural selection: statistical inference and biological interpretation. Evolution. 1987;41:1149–1161. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1987.tb02457.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nilsson LA. The evolution of flowers with deep corolla tubes. Nature. 1988;334:147–149. [Google Scholar]
- O'Connell LM, Johnston MO. Male and female pollination success in a deceptive orchid: a selection study. Ecology. 1998;79:1246–1260. [Google Scholar]
- Ollerton J, Alarcón R, Waser NM, et al. A global test of the pollination syndrome hypothesis. Annals of Botany. 2009;103 doi: 10.1093/aob/mcp031. 1471–1480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ordano M, Fornoni J, Boege K, Domínguez CA. The adaptive value of phenotypic floral integration. New Phytologist. 2008;179:1183–1192. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8137.2008.02523.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips PC, Arnold SJ. Visualizing multivariate selection. Evolution. 1989;43:1209–1222. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1989.tb02569.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pigliucci M. Phenotypic integration: studying the ecology and evolution of complex phenotypes. Ecology Letters. 2003;6:265–272. [Google Scholar]
- Rausher MD. The measurement of selection on quantitative traits: biases due to environmental covariances between traits and fitness. Evolution. 1992;46:616–626. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1992.tb02070.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rausher MD. Evolutionary transitions in floral color. International Journal of Plant Sciences. 2008;169:7–21. [Google Scholar]
- Riffell JA, Alarcon R, Abrell L, Davidowitz G, Bronstein JL, Hildebrand JG. Behavioral consequences of innate preferences and olfactory learning in hawkmoth–flower interactions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2008;105:3404–3409. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0709811105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schluter D, Nychka D. Exploring fitness surfaces. American Naturalist. 1994;143:597–616. [Google Scholar]
- Smith CE, Stevens JT, Temeles EJ, Ewald PW, Hebert RJ, Bonkovsky RL. Effect of floral orifice width and shape on hummingbird–flower interactions. Oecologia (Berlin) 1996;106:482–492. doi: 10.1007/BF00329706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strauss SY, Whittall JB. Non-pollinator agents of selection on floral traits. In: Harder L, Barrett S, editors. Ecology and evolution of flowers. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Temeles EJ. A new dimension to hummingbird–flower relationships. Oecologia (Berlin) 1996;105:517–523. doi: 10.1007/BF00330015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Temeles EJ, Rankin AG. Effect of the lower lip of Monarda didyma on pollen removal by hummingbirds. Canadian Journal of Botany. 2000;78:1164–1168. [Google Scholar]
- Wardle P. Origin of the New Zealand mountain flora, with special reference to trans-Tasman relationships. New Zealand Journal of Botany. 1978;16:535–550. [Google Scholar]
- Waser NM, Price MV. Pollinator behaviour and natural selection for flower colour in Delphinium nelsonii. Nature. 1983;302:422–424. [Google Scholar]
- Waser NM, Price MV. The effect of nectar guides on pollinator preference – experimental studies with a montane herb. Oecologia (Berlin) 1985;67:121–126. doi: 10.1007/BF00378462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waser NM, Chittka L, Price MV, Williams N, Ollerton J. Generalization in pollination systems and why it matters. Ecology. 1996;77:279–296. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson P. Selection for pollination success and the mechanical fit of Impatiens flowers around bumblebee bodies. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 1995;55:355–383. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson P, Castellanos MC, Hogue JN, Thomson JD, Armbruster WS. A multivariate search for pollination syndromes among penstemons. Oikos. 2004;104:345–361. [Google Scholar]
- Wright JW, Meagher TR. Selection on floral characters in natural Spanish populations of Silene latifolia. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2004;17:382–395. doi: 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00671.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]