Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008 Nov 6;18(6):552–557. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.09.017

Corollary discharge circuits in the primate brain

Trinity B Crapse 1, Marc A Sommer 1
PMCID: PMC2702467  NIHMSID: NIHMS119755  PMID: 18848626

Summary

Movements are necessary to engage the world, but every movement results in sensorimotor ambiguity. Self-movements cause changes to sensory inflow as well as changes in the positions of objects relative to motor effectors (eyes and limbs). Hence the brain needs to monitor self-movements, and one way this is accomplished is by routing copies of movement commands to appropriate structures. These signals, known as corollary discharge (CD), enable compensation for sensory consequences of movement and preemptive updating of spatial representations. Such operations occur with a speed and accuracy that implies a reliance on prediction. Here we review recent CD studies and find that they arrive at a shared conclusion: CD contributes to prediction for the sake of sensorimotor harmony.

Introduction

Imagine a monkey leaping through the forest canopy. As it moves, branches brush against its skin, leaves rustle at its hands and feet, and patterns of light and shade alternate across its eyes (Figure 1A). In principle, the monkey should be startled by these sensory events. The activation of its skin receptors could be interpreted as due to an insect landing on its leg and the sounds and shadows as due to a predator looming. Surprisingly, the monkey does not find these sensory events alarming; they are expected, partly because the monkey has access to an internal report of its own movements called corollary discharge (CD).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Action and perception. (A) A monkey in the forest has obvious need for sensorimotor coordination. Every movement results in an inundation of sensory inputs that must be discriminated and referenced so that the monkey can proceed with the next. (B) Corollary discharge provides a means for this coordination by apprising sensory areas of the goings-on of movement areas. Just as a movement command is issued, a copy of that movement command (corollary discharge) is routed in the opposite direction to inform the appropriate sensory area. This sort of signalling closes the sensorimotor loop and makes sensorimotor operation possible.

Each of the monkey’s movements is initiated by motor commands that travel from movement areas of the brain out to the periphery to contract the appropriate muscles. Neural copies of the movement commands – the CD signals – are issued simultaneously and travel in the opposite direction, impinging upon sensory brain areas (Figure 1B). The CD information tells the sensory areas about the upcoming movements and allows them to prepare for the sensory consequences of the movement. As a result, our monkey in the forest is not surprised by the brush of the branch, the rustle of the leaves, or the change in shade. Were the monkey at rest or moving passively – for example sitting on a branch that sways in the breeze – the same sensory events would be startling indeed.

As a theoretical concept, CD has a rich history [1]. Behavioral and psychophysical evidence for CD has accumulated over a century and received a significant boon in 1950 when two teams of researchers working independently and on different continents arrived at the same conclusion: motor and sensory systems require reciprocal coordination, implying that motor signals travel to sensory structures [2,3]. Now, decades later, we know the motor-to-sensory signal to be quite ubiquitous as evidenced by a wealth of direct physiological evidence collected from a menagerie of species [46]. In this review we examine recent behavioral and physiological studies of CD in primates and place an emphasis on its role in prediction. We close by considering computational treatments of the concept.

CD and human behavior

Human psychophysical studies have provided much insight into CD function and its roles in two operations: resolving ambiguity in the origin of sensory inputs and enabling proper motor performance. Many of these studies have focused on the visuomotor networks of primates, particularly on those that mediate smooth pursuit and saccadic eye movements. Eye movements are beneficial in permitting detailed analysis of objects by the fovea, but they are costly, too, as they generate retinal image motion that is indistinguishable from real object motion. We do not experience the startling percept of world motion as our eyes rotate, however; instead we enjoy a percept of stability. This implies a corrective mechanism within the oculomotor networks of the brain. One model of this mechanism, based closely on the classical concepts of Efferenzkopie (“efference copy”) by von Holst and Mittelstaedt [3] and Willensanstrengung (“effort of will”) by von Helmholtz [7], postulates a cancellation process mediated at least in part by an internal reference signal [8]. This internal reference signal, generated from a CD that represents each eye movement, is a prediction of the imminent retinal image motion. The prediction is compared to the actual retinal motion; if the two signals cancel, then no net motion of the visual world is perceived. If they do not cancel, then the residual is experienced as motion of the world. Psychophysically, evidence for the internal reference signal has been provided in the smooth pursuit system where it has been shown to be rather plastic [9]: the gain of the CD can be manipulated quite readily in the laboratory. As predicted, when the gain is adjusted, false eye movement-induced perceptions of world motion are experienced [9]. This ability to manipulate the internal reference signal has been exploited by investigators to pinpoint the anatomical sites of retinal signal/internal reference signal comparison and cancellation. Evidence suggests that it resides in late parts of the cortical hierarchy such as medial superior temporal area (MST), and parieto-insular-vestibular cortex [1012].

An adaptable reference signal also serves a purpose on the motor side of things. Movements are often too fast to be under strict control by external feedback from sensory receptors. They require internal feedback of motor commands, which CD provides. As a ballistic sort of motor expression, the saccadic eye movement is a case in point. Theoretical treatments have long postulated a motoric internal reference signal, representing the effector’s (i.e. the eye’s) instantaneous position, that is utilized by the brain to monitor the trajectory of the movement [13,14]. This sort of monitoring is posited to consist of a forward model of the oculomotor system that uses CDs of the saccade to generate predictions of the expected sensory outcome. The predictions are then used to update the saccade, by implementing corrections mid-flight. A prediction of this hypothesis is that the forward model should adapt when its predictions systematically fail, that is, when they do not match the expected sensory feedback. Shadmehr and colleagues tested this hypothesis in a task that artificially introduces discrepancies between the predicted and actual sensory outcome, the saccadic adaptation task [15]. Forward model involvement would be betrayed by saccades with systematic curvatures revealing mid-flight correction to the target’s new position. They found that saccadic adaptation was preceded by forward model adaptation, a result manifested in saccades with characteristic curvature. Similar adaptations occur for visually guided limb movements [16,17].

CD also plays a part in coordinating the skeletomotor and somatosensory systems. A prominent role is in permitting the primate to distinguish between tactile inputs that are due to self from those due to something else. A psychophysical correlate of this interaction is elevated thresholds to somatic stimulation during active movement, a type of sensorimotor attenuation [18]. A pair of causal studies in humans, one using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to delay movement execution [19] and another using ischemic nerve block to silence proprioception [20], revealed that modulation is mediated by premotor cortical inputs to somatosensory cortex. These inputs modulate somatosensory neurons often by suppression and may consist of predictions of the sensory consequences of the movement [21,22]

Vocalization and auditory systems must also coordinate their activities. CD mechanisms involving speech production and auditory regions, play such a coordinative role in distinguishing auditory experiences resulting from self-generated vocalizations from those arising from other-generated speech sounds. Evidence for this interaction is provided by neurons in auditory areas that are suppressed during self-generated vocalizations, yet remain fully response for comparable stimulation produced by conspecifics [23,24]. This observation extends to language areas of the human brain [25] which exhibit a dampening of the so-called N100 component of the auditory event related potential during self-vocalization [26]. Interestingly, this dampening is significantly reduced in schizophrenic patients suffering from auditory hallucinations, betraying a reduction in speech production/auditory coordination [27,28]. An alteration in this coordination may result in internally generated events such as thoughts being experienced as having an external source. This may serve as a basis for certain aspects of the disease pathology such as auditory hallucinations and self-monitoring deficits.

CD and mechanism

In order to better address mechanism, many researchers have turned to another primate species: the monkey. Studies in the auditory cortex of the marmoset, for example, have provided insights into the neural correlates of auditory-vocal interactions (Figure 2A). As mentioned, neurons throughout the auditory cortex are inhibited during self-generated vocalizations, a possible neural correlate of self-monitoring [23,24]. Recent work has provided more insight into this process and shown the suppression to be part of a mechanism that actually increases the sensitivity of these neurons to auditory feedback, specifically to deviations from the predicted feedback [29]. As the marmoset vocalizes, the frequency tuning of the neurons transiently shifts and seems to monitor for deviations from the expected auditory feedback. When the actual feedback matches the expected feedback, the neurons are silent, i.e. suppressed (Figure 2B, blue trace). But when the feedback differs, as revealed through artificial manipulation, the neurons burst, signalling the disparity (Figure 2B, red trace). This sort of disparity detection suggests that the neurons are reporting prediction error or deviations from the expected outcome. Similar processes are at work in the primate visuosaccadic system in which CD signals induce a transient change in visual-related activity during saccades to better predict the postsaccadic visual scene, i.e. expected visual feedback (more below).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Shifting RFs and the auditory system (A) “Marmoset in a recording studio.” Investigators recorded from auditory cortical neurons as a marmoset vocalized. The auditory feedback from the vocalization was relayed to the marmoset without delay under both normal and altered conditions. (B) Peristimulus time histogram exhibiting the basic effect. This auditory cortical neuron was suppressed during normal vocalization (blue trace). However, when the auditory feedback of the vocalization was shifted in the frequency domain, the neuron exhibited a large increase in firing rate (red trace). The neuron’s frequency tuning was transiently altered at the outset of the vocalization by a CD, rendering it sensitive to deviations from the expected auditory feedback, an example of a shifting RF in the auditory frequency domain. Adapted from [29].

As mentioned, saccades are a second common visuomotor behavior of the primate, and like the smooth pursuit system, they require the discriminative powers of CD. CD signals have a hypothesized role in distinguishing visual scene displacements due to the saccade from those due to real object motion in the world [30,31]. At the outset of each saccade, advance warning is provided by CD, mediating a compensatory procedure to cancel the percept of visual-scene displacement. Critical components of this procedure are thought to be neurons of the visual system that transiently shift their receptive field (RF) before each saccade. Although a typical RF is firmly retinotopic and samples a new part of the visual field only after the eye moves, shifting RFs are dynamic and start sampling the new RF location even before a saccade. By sampling the same region of space both before and after the saccade, shifting RFs are thought to perform a comparative operation that leads ultimately to a stable visual percept. Such neurons are found throughout extrastriate and frontal cortex [30,32,33] and, because the RF shifts before the saccade and in a direction and amplitude that matches the imminent saccade, the shift must require CD. At least some of this CD is provided by a midbrain-derived pathway impinging upon the frontal eye field (FEF), an area involved in visual processing and eye–movement control [31,34]. It has been postulated that inactivation of this pathway would result in the disorienting percept of visual scene displacement with each saccade [31]. Under this condition, monkeys trained to detect object motion during saccades should report that stationary visual stimuli move with each saccade. This would be a direct test of the CD/shifting RF visual-stability hypothesis.

Saccadic sequences also require the use of internal feedback signalling mediated by CD. The relative positions of objects must be updated after each intervening saccade, so the primate must continuously update its internal record of the current saccade in order to facilitate planning of the next. Retinal information is too slow to be of any use, and ocular proprioception does not seem to be involved (reviewed by [35]), so the system requires CD of each saccade to properly update the spatial representation. In the laboratory this sort of phenomenon has been studied with the double-step task in which the subject is required to saccade by memory to the locations of two previously flashed sequential targets [36]. This particular task requires a retracing of the flash sequence without the aid of sensory feedback, but rather with the only feedback available: internal feedback provided by CD. Inactivation and lesion studies indicate that some of these CD signals are provided to the FEF from the same midbrain-derived pathway used for shifting RFs [3740]. When the pathway is compromised by transiently inactivating its relay node in MD thalamus, the second saccades are altered, exhibiting patterns of rotational deviation consistent with deficits in taking the previous saccade into account. Other thalamic nuclei, such as VL nucleus and area X, may convey subcortically derived CD too, but these seem to differ from MD in being more involved in the timing of self-initiated saccades rather than updating the spatial representation [41].

Further support for CD involvement in visuospatial performance is provided by stimulation studies in the FEF [42]. Subthreshold microstimulation during the delay period of memory tasks results in displacements of memory-guided saccades in a manner consistent with introduction of fictive CD signals: the monkey executes the planned saccade as if another saccade had been performed during the interim. The fact that stimulation does not disrupt the memory trace itself, suggests that the FEF may be more important as a locus of updating spatial representations and the exportation of these computational outcomes to other brain areas. In this sense, the FEF may be crucial as a gateway for exporting visuosaccadic predictions to the rest of the brain. We discuss this in more detail in the succeeding section.

CD and computation and beyond

Several computational studies have modeled CD-dependent processes and suggested how the nervous system utilizes CD for functions such as spatial updating [43], and perisaccadic perception [44]. But a question that remains is, from a computational perspective, what is CD? While practical criteria for identifying CD at the behavioral and neuronal levels have been provided [45] a fair question to pose at this juncture is whether there is a more general definition of CD. Stated differently, is there a function common to all instantiations of CD that captures the essence of what CD actually is and does?

The reviewed psychophysical, physiological, and causal studies provide possible answers to this question. Some common principles are that CD is a signal involved in motor planning, and most prominently, in distinguishing between those sensory inputs that result from self from those that result from something else. This latter function is illustrated best in the shifting RFs of the auditory [29] and visual systems [3033], the predictive reference signals of the smooth pursuit system [8], and the forward model motoric processes of the saccadic system [15]. These mechanisms necessitate a predictive component, as they must generate and compare the expected consequences of the movement, be it motor or sensory, with the actual consequences, and they must do so in a rapid manner despite processing delays. Deviations from the prediction are ultimately manifest at both the physiological and behavioral levels as bursts of action potentials where silence would otherwise reign [29] and shifts in psychometric curves [8,15]. So a short answer is that the underlying function of all CD signals is the prediction of motor consequences. This formulation, with its reliance upon prediction and comparison, is conceptually similar to the forward model framework prevalent in current sensorimotor discussions whereby CDs of movement commands are converted into expected sensory outcomes [14].

If CD is motor-related prediction, how might these predictive operations be implemented? Computationally, they would most likely have to be inferential in nature, that is, probabilistic. Probabilistic computations, such as those based on empirical Bayesian principles, would enable rapid predictions to be generated [46,47]. These computations would allow prior and conditional probability distributions, stored in neural networks, to be utilized to generate a posterior distribution, i.e., the outcome expected on the balance of known probabilities and other inputs (in other words, a prediction). Comparisons between the predicted outcome and actual outcome, so prominent in the reviewed CD circuits, could then proceed. Psychophysical and physiological evidence provides support that such computations are indeed performed by the brain under a variety of contexts [4749].

We have proposed elsewhere that predictive operations, grounded in such probabilistic inference, are implemented in the primate visuosaccadic system for the purpose of constructing a stable transaccadic percept [50]. At the center of the model are CD and shifting RFs of the FEF, and we propose they together constitute an inferential architecture that engages in predictive coding. Theoretically, these components enable predictions of the future scene (postsaccadic scene) to be generated based upon predictive, probabilistic extrapolations from the current scene (presaccadic; Figure 3). These predictions provide activity constraining expectations to the rest of the brain and may contribute to a percept of stability. The model predicts that deviations (prediction errors) from the expected visual input are manifest in the postsaccadic visual responses of FEF neurons (analogous to the auditory disparity detectors of the reviewed marmoset study [29]). Preliminary data from our laboratory provide support for such a large-scale prediction error theory of transaccadic stability (T.B. Crapse and M.A. Sommer, abstract 165.11, Society for Neuroscience, Washington DC, 2008).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Corollary discharge and predictive computations in the visuosaccadic system. (A) Lateral view of macaque brain illustrating the interactions between the frontal eye field (FEF) and the posterior lobes. Triggered by CD, the FEF exports predictive signals to the posterior lobes via feedback connections. The posterior lobes report back to the FEF about the actual visual input via feedforward connections. (B) Corresponding schematic diagram depicting the computational roles of the FEF. FEF module inset: (1) A series of computations, triggered by CD from the midbrain and enabled by shifting RFs, results in a prediction of the postsaccadic scene (λρ) based upon information extracted from the current or presaccadic scene. (2) The prediction is then convolved with a forward model (Ψ) to yield guidance signals (Ψ(λρ)) that are imposed upon the posterior lobes. This signal constrains the state space of the posterior lobes and biases the operations performed within them. (3) Once the saccade is complete, information about the actually occurring postsaccadic scene (λα) is routed to the FEF. (4) The FEF compares the input representing the actual postsaccadic scene with the initial prediction (ε = λα − λρ). This is useful for calibration, error correction, and the detection of unpredicted visual events.

Future computational studies will continue to provide further insights into CD and its role in predictive computations of the primate brain. Together with behavioral and neurophysiological studies, including those reviewed here, the modeling work should help to explain in a more formal way how CD contributes to sensorimotor harmony as primates interact with the world. The field of motor control will benefit from expanded efforts to determine not only how sensory analysis and spatial cognition influence movements, but also how movement signals influence sensory analysis and spatial cognition.

Acknowledgments

Supported by grants to M.A.S. from the NIH (EY017592) and the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Grusser O. On the history of ideas of efference copy and reafference. Clio Med. 1995;33:35–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Sperry R. Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. The Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1950;43:482–489. doi: 10.1037/h0055479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Von Holst E, Mittelstaedt H. The reafference principle. Naturwissenschaften. 1950;37:464–467. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Crapse TB, Sommer MA. Corollary discharge across the animal kingdom. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:587–600. doi: 10.1038/nrn2457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cullen KE. Sensory signals during active versus passive movement. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2004;14:698–706. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Poulet JF, Hedwig B. New insights into corollary discharges mediated by identified neural pathways. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.von Helmholtz H. Helmholtz’s Treatise on Physiological Optics. 2. New York: Optical Society of America; 1925. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Thier P, Haarmeier T, Chakraborty S, Lindner A, Tikhonov A. Cortical substrates of perceptual stability during eye movements. Neuroimage. 2001;14:S33–39. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Haarmeier T, Thier P. Modification of the Filehne illusion by conditioning visual stimuli. Vision Res. 1996;36:741–750. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00154-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dicke PW, Chakraborty S, Thier P. Neuronal correlates of perceptual stability during eye movements. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;27:991–1002. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06054.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lindner A, Haarmeier T, Erb M, Grodd W, Thier P. Cerebrocerebellar circuits for the perceptual cancellation of eye-movement-induced retinal image motion. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:1899–1912. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.11.1899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Haarmeier T, Thier P, Repnow M, Petersen D. False perception of motion in a patient who cannot compensate for eye movements. Nature. 1997;389:849–852. doi: 10.1038/39872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Davidson PR, Wolpert DM. Widespread access to predictive models in the motor system: a short review. J Neural Eng. 2005;2:S313–319. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/2/3/S11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wolpert DM, Miall RC. Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control. Neural Netw. 1996;9:1265–1279. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(96)00035-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15•.Chen-Harris H, Joiner WM, Ethier V, Zee DS, Shadmehr R. Adaptive control of saccades via internal feedback. J Neurosci. 2008;28:2804–2813. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5300-07.2008. Excellent study that provided human psychophysical evidence for rapid forward model adaptation in the visuosaccadic system. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Synofzik M, Lindner A, Thier P. The cerebellum updates predictions about the visual consequences of one’s behavior. Curr Biol. 2008;18:814–818. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Synofzik M, Thier P, Lindner A. Internalizing agency of self-action: perception of one’s own hand movements depends on an adaptable prediction about the sensory action outcome. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:1592–1601. doi: 10.1152/jn.00104.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Chapman CE, Bushnell MC, Miron D, Duncan GH, Lund JP. Sensory perception during movement in man. Exp Brain Res. 1987;68:516–524. doi: 10.1007/BF00249795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Voss M, Ingram JN, Haggard P, Wolpert DM. Sensorimotor attenuation by central motor command signals in the absence of movement. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:26–27. doi: 10.1038/nn1592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20•.Christensen MS, Lundbye-Jensen J, Geertsen SS, Petersen TH, Paulson OB, Nielsen JB. Premotor cortex modulates somatosensory cortex during voluntary movements without proprioceptive feedback. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:417–419. doi: 10.1038/nn1873. A pair of causal studies that reveal a role for premotor structures in modulating somatosensation during active movements. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Blakemore SJ, Frith CD, Wolpert DM. Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli. J Cogn Neurosci. 1999;11:551–559. doi: 10.1162/089892999563607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Blankenburg F, Ruff CC, Deichmann R, Rees G, Driver J. The cutaneous rabbit illusion affects human primary sensory cortex somatotopically. PLoS Biol. 2006;4:e69. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Eliades SJ, Wang X. Sensory-motor interaction in the primate auditory cortex during self-initiated vocalizations. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89:2194–2207. doi: 10.1152/jn.00627.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Muller-Preuss P, Ploog D. Inhibition of auditory cortical neurons during phonation. Brain Res. 1981;215:61–76. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(81)90491-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Creutzfeldt O, Ojemann G, Lettich E. Neuronal activity in the human lateral temporal lobe. II. Responses to the subjects own voice. Exp Brain Res. 1989;77:476–489. doi: 10.1007/BF00249601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ford JM, Mathalon DH, Heinks T, Kalba S, Faustman WO, Roth WT. Neurophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:2069–2071. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.12.2069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ford JM, Roach BJ, Faustman WO, Mathalon DH. Out-of-synch and out-of-sorts: dysfunction of motor-sensory communication in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:736–743. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.09.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28•.Heinks-Maldonado TH, Mathalon DH, Houde JF, Gray M, Faustman WO, Ford JM. Relationship of imprecise corollary discharge in schizophrenia to auditory hallucinations. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:286–296. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.3.286. One of a continuing series of papers examining the relationship between CD and the positive symptoms of schizophrenia such as auditory hallucinations. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29••.Eliades SJ, Wang X. Neural substrates of vocalization feedback monitoring in primate auditory cortex. Nature. 2008;453:1102–1106. doi: 10.1038/nature06910. An outstanding study that uncovered auditory cortical neurons that transiently shift their frequency tuning during vocalizations. These neurons seem to monitor for deviations from the expected auditory feedback, a possible neural correlate of self-monitoring. Intriguingly, they may be the auditory equivalent of shifting RFs in the primate visuosaccadic system. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Duhamel JR, Colby CL, Goldberg ME. The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. Science. 1992;255:90–92. doi: 10.1126/science.1553535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31•.Sommer MA, Wurtz RH. Influence of the thalamus on spatial visual processing in frontal cortex. Nature. 2006;444:374–377. doi: 10.1038/nature05279. First study to reveal a causal link between CD and shifting RF generation in the primate visuosaccadic system. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Nakamura K, Colby CL. Updating of the visual representation in monkey striate and extrastriate cortex during saccades. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:4026–4031. doi: 10.1073/pnas.052379899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Umeno MM, Goldberg ME. Spatial processing in the monkey frontal eye field. I. Predictive visual responses. J Neurophysiol. 1997;78:1373–1383. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.3.1373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sommer MA, Wurtz RH. What the brain stem tells the frontal cortex. I. Oculomotor signals sent from superior colliculus to frontal eye field via mediodorsal thalamus. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:1381–1402. doi: 10.1152/jn.00738.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Sommer MA, Wurtz RH. Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements. Annual Reviews in Neuroscience. 2008;31:317–338. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hallett PE, Lightstone AD. Saccadic eye movements to flashed targets. Vision Res. 1976;16:107–114. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(76)90084-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bellebaum C, Daum I, Koch B, Schwarz M, Hoffmann KP. The role of the human thalamus in processing corollary discharge. Brain. 2005;128:1139–1154. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bellebaum C, Hoffmann KP, Koch B, Schwarz M, Daum I. Altered processing of corollary discharge in thalamic lesion patients. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;24:2375–2388. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05114.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Sommer MA, Wurtz RH. A pathway in primate brain for internal monitoring of movements. Science. 2002;296:1480–1482. doi: 10.1126/science.1069590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Sommer MA, Wurtz RH. What the brain stem tells the frontal cortex. II. Role of the SC-MD-FEF pathway in corollary discharge. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:1403–1423. doi: 10.1152/jn.00740.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41•.Tanaka M. Inactivation of the central thalamus delays self-timed saccades. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:20–22. doi: 10.1038/nn1617. Elegant study that revealed a role of the thalamus in relaying CD important for the timing of internally generated saccades. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.White RL, 3rd, Snyder LH. Subthreshold microstimulation in frontal eye fields updates spatial memories. Exp Brain Res. 2007 doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0947-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Keith GP, Crawford JD. Saccade-related remapping of target representations between topographic maps: a neural network study. J Comput Neurosci. 2008;24:157–178. doi: 10.1007/s10827-007-0046-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hamker FH, Zirnsak M, Calow D, Lappe M. The peri-saccadic perception of objects and space. PLoS Comput Biol. 2008;4:e31. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0040031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wurtz RH, Sommer MA. Identifying corollary discharges for movement in the primate brain. Prog Brain Res. 2004;144:47–60. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)14403-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Knill DC, Pouget A. The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27:712–719. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Bays PM, Wolpert DM. Computational principles of sensorimotor control that minimize uncertainty and variability. J Physiol. 2007;578:387–396. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.120121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Schultz W, Dickinson A. Neuronal coding of prediction errors. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:473–500. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Bar M. The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:280–289. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Crapse TB, Sommer MA. The frontal eye field as a prediction map. Progress in Brain Research. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00656-0. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES