Summary
The properties of cortical circuits underlying central representations of sensory stimuli are poorly understood. Here we use in vivo cell-attached and whole-cell voltage clamp recordings to reveal how excitatory and inhibitory synaptic input govern odor representations in rat primary olfactory (piriform) cortex. We show that odors evoke spiking activity that is sparse across the cortical population. We find that unbalanced synaptic excitation and inhibition underlies sparse activity: inhibition is widespread and broadly tuned, while excitation is less common and odor-specific. “Global” inhibition can be explained by local interneurons that receive ubiquitous and nonselective odor-evoked excitation. In the temporal domain, while respiration imposes a slow rhythm to olfactory cortical responses, odors evoke fast (15–30 Hz) oscillations in synaptic activity. Oscillatory excitation precedes inhibition, generating brief time windows for precise and temporally sparse spike output. Together, our results reveal that global inhibition and oscillations are major synaptic mechanisms shaping odor representations in olfactory cortex.
Introduction
The functional properties of cortical circuits play a critical role in the central representations of sensory stimuli. However, the synaptic mechanisms governing stimulus-selective spike output in sensory cortices are still debated. Broadly tuned (lateral) inhibition is a fundamental physiological mechanism often proposed to sharpen responses to preferred stimuli, primarily by counteracting weak, nonpreferred excitatory input (Hartline et al., 1956; Priebe and Ferster, 2008). Surprisingly, intracellular studies in visual, auditory, and somatosensory cortex find that synaptic excitation and inhibition are co-tuned to the same stimuli and inhibition elicited by nonpreferred stimuli is often weak (Anderson et al., 2000; Priebe and Ferster, 2008; Wehr and Zador, 2003; Wilent and Contreras, 2005), suggesting that primary sensory cortical circuits lack properties supporting lateral inhibition.
Although the initial steps underlying the processing of olfactory information are beginning to be revealed, how olfactory information is represented in the cortex is not well established. In rodents, olfactory information is initially processed in the olfactory bulb, where olfactory sensory neurons expressing one of ~1000 different types of odorant receptors map onto ~1800 glomeruli (Mombaerts et al., 1996). Within each glomerulus, 50–100 mitral and tufted (M/T) cells receive input from sensory neurons expressing a unique type of odorant receptor and M/T cells are thought to represent particular odorant molecular features (Rubin and Katz, 1999; Uchida et al., 2000; Wachowiak and Cohen, 2001). Recent studies suggest that the spatial and temporal patterns of M/T cell activity encode the initial representations of olfactory information in the brain (Bathellier et al., 2008; Margrie and Schaefer, 2003; Rinberg et al., 2006; Soucy et al., 2009; Spors and Grinvald, 2002). However, odor perception ultimately requires the integration of M/T cell activity in higher cortical brain regions and the synaptic mechanisms underlying cortical odor representations are unknown.
In this study, we explore the mechanisms governing odor representations in the anterior piriform cortex, a three-layered cortical region that plays a critical role in odor discrimination, recognition, and memory (Neville and Haberly, 2004; Wilson et al., 2006). Layer 2/3 (L2/3) pyramidal cells in anterior piriform cortex receive direct sensory input from M/T cell axons via the lateral olfactory tract (LOT), excitatory inputs from other cortical neurons and inhibition via local GABAergic circuits (Fig. 1A; Neville and Haberly, 2004). Individual L2/3 pyramidal cells are likely to receive converging input from M/T cells belonging to different glomeruli (Franks and Isaacson, 2006). Consistent with this idea, histochemical and extracellular studies suggest that individual odors can activate spatially distributed ensembles of neurons, and individual neurons may respond to multiple odors (Illig and Haberly, 2003; Litaudon et al., 2003; Rennaker et al., 2007; Wilson et al., 2006; Zou et al., 2005).
Here we use in vivo cell-attached and whole-cell recordings to reveal how excitatory and inhibitory synaptic input govern odor representations in L2/3 cells of rat primary olfactory cortex. We show that odor-evoked firing activity is sparse and distributed across the cortical neuron population. We find that unbalanced synaptic excitation and inhibition underlie sparse odor representations. Across the cortical population, odor-evoked inhibition is widespread while excitation is less common. In individual cells, excitation is odor-specific and inhibition is nonselective. We show that this “global” inhibition is likely to arise from local interneurons that receive broadly tuned excitation. We also find that odors evoke fast beta frequency (15–30 Hz) oscillations in synaptic activity. Oscillating excitation precedes inhibition, generating a brief (~10 ms) temporal window that restricts spike timing. Together, these results reveal that global inhibition and oscillatory synaptic inputs govern the tuning and timing of odor-evoked activity in olfactory cortex.
Results
Odor-evoked spikes are sparse in olfactory cortex
We first investigated odor representations in vivo using cell-attached recordings of action potentials (APs) from anterior piriform cortex L2/3 neurons in urethane-anesthetized, freely breathing rats (n=59). This recording method provides exceptional isolation of single units and is not biased towards the sampling of active or responsive cells (Hromadka et al., 2008; Margrie et al., 2002). Cell-attached recordings revealed low spontaneous firing rates of L2/3 cells (Fig. 1B,C1; median 0.28 Hz, mean 0.73±0.08 Hz, n=177 cells) and APs were frequently time-locked to the ~2 Hz respiratory rhythm (Buonviso et al., 2006; Litaudon et al., 2003; Rennaker et al., 2007) (Fig. 1B, Supplemental Fig. 1).
Results from a large set of individually sampled neurons (n=177) were used to infer the distribution of odor-evoked firing activity across the cortical population. In order to determine how individual stimuli are represented by the cortical population, we sampled responses to a small, fixed odor set rather than searching for the optimal stimulus for a particular neuron. We tested four monomolecular odors (5% saturated vapor (SV)) with unique and distinct structures and perceptual qualities: cineole (ether, eucalyptus), amyl acetate (ester, banana), limonene (terpene, citrus), and phenylethyl alcohol (alcohol, floral). For each odor tested in every cell (odor-cell pair), we used both changes in mean firing rate and the reliability of firing across trials to categorize activity as odor-evoked or nonresponsive (see Experimental Procedures). Although we observed clear odor-evoked suppression of APs in some cells (n=9 cells, data not shown), the low spontaneous firing rate precluded accurate classification of inhibitory responses.
We first determined the odor-selectivity of individual cells, as well as the population response to each individual odor. In other words, we tested the number of odors each cell responded to, and the number of cells each odor can activate. For cells with odor-evoked responses (55/177), most (42/55) fired selectively to only one of the four odors (Fig. 1C2). In terms of the population response, each odor evoked activity in ~10% of tested cells (Fig. 1C3), indicating that the different odors elicited spikes in relatively small fractions of the cortical population. Interestingly, despite their structural diversity, each unique odor activated very similar fractions (range 9–11%) of the cortical population.
To better understand the distribution of odor-evoked activity in olfactory cortex, we explored the intensity of stimulus-evoked responses. For responsive odor-cell pairs, the average increase in firing rate during the odor stimulus (2 s) was 2.01±0.04 Hz (Fig. 1C1; range 0.05 to 24.5 Hz; median: 0.83 Hz, n=72 odor-cell pairs). Strong responses were rarely observed: only 19% of responses exceeded 5 Hz and very few (6%) exceeded 10 Hz (Fig. 1C1). Evoked APs were coupled strongly to the respiratory rhythm (Fig. 1B, Supplemental Fig. 1) and on average odors evoked only an additional 1.6±0.04 spikes (median: 0.6 AP) above baseline during each respiratory cycle throughout the odor stimulus (Fig. 1C4). Thus, odor responses consisted of weak increases in firing rate in the majority of responsive cells, while a small fraction of neurons fired more strongly.
In addition to quantifying odor-selectivity and the population response in terms of odor-cell pairs that were categorized as odor-evoked or nonresponsive, we also used statistical measures calculated from raw firing rate distributions (Rolls and Tovee, 1995; Willmore and Tolhurst, 2001). This provides a description of odor-evoked activity without relying on binary categorization of responses. Lifetime sparseness (SL, range 0 to1=highly selective), a measure of how an individual cell responds to multiple stimuli (see Experimental Procedures), indicated that cells responded selectively (SL mean=0.88±0.002, median =1, n=177 cells). Population sparseness (Sp, range 0 to 1=most sparse), a measure of how an individual stimulus is represented across a population, was also high (mean Sp=0.93, range 0.90–0.96). Taken together, our results indicate that odor representations are sparse in olfactory cortex.
Global inhibition and selective excitation underlie sparse odor representations
What governs the sparse population response of L2/3 cells? To address this question, we used in vivo whole-cell recording (Margrie et al., 2002) to examine the synaptic input underlying spike output in an additional set of L2/3 cells (n=52). Following cell-attached recording of APs (Fig. 2A), excitatory (EPSCs) and inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs) were recorded in voltage-clamp mode in each cell (Fig. 2B). EPSCs were recorded at −80 mV, the reversal potential for inhibition set by our internal solution (ECl=−80 mV). Similarly, IPSCs were recorded at the reversal potential for excitation (~+10 mV). In the absence of applied odors, cells received barrages of spontaneous EPSCs (77±12 Hz) and IPSCs (57±10 Hz, n=12 representative cells, data not shown) and odors evoked synaptic currents that were coupled to the respiration cycle (Fig. 2B, Supplemental Fig. 1). We first examined synaptic responses categorically and determined responsiveness (the presence or absence of odor-evoked activity) for each odor-cell pair from the increase in charge transfer during odor presentation (see Experimental Procedures).
We first compared the fractions of cells in this population that responded to the different odors with APs, EPSCs, and IPSCs. Each of the different odors elicited responses in similar fractions of cells (Fig. 2C). We estimated population sparseness from the fraction of cells responsive to each odor averaged over all odors. While cells with odor-evoked APs were rarely observed (8.3±0.5% of the population), odor-evoked excitation was more common (22.7±1.5%) and inhibition was remarkably widespread (51.8±2.2%, Fig. 2C). Furthermore, Sp calculated from unthresholded synaptic charge measurements during odor presentation indicated that excitatory synaptic responses (Sp=0.72±0.03) were significantly sparser than inhibition (Sp=0.56±0.02, p=0.006). These results suggest that across the cortical population, ubiquitous odor-evoked inhibition contributes to firing activity that is more sparsely distributed than synaptic excitation.
We further explored whether inhibition contributes to sparse odor-evoked firing activity by blocking fast synaptic inhibition with the GABAA receptor antagonist gabazine (SR-95531). However, local cortical superfusion of gabazine (20–100 μM) led to epileptic activity evident as ictal bursts (~1 Hz) of spikes in cell-attached recordings (n=10). Once epileptic events began, odor-evoked activity was lost and spikes became decoupled from respiration. Nonetheless, in two experiments we observed a broadening in the odor tuning of firing activity in the presence of gabazine before the cortex became epileptic (under control conditions the two cells fired in response to only one of four odors vs. two and three odors in the presence of drug, data not shown).
We next considered the odor selectivity of synaptic excitation and inhibition in individual cells. Although cells with odor-evoked EPSCs were more common than APs (Fig. 2C), EPSCs were selectively evoked by only one out of four odors in the majority of cells (60%, Fig. 2D). Strikingly, inhibition was recruited non-selectively; in 66% of cells that received inhibition, it was evoked by three or all four odors (Fig. 2D). Together, these findings suggest that inhibition is “global” in olfactory cortex, i.e. odors evoke widespread inhibition across the population and inhibition within an individual cell is broadly tuned to odors.
If inhibition were truly global in olfactory cortex, we would predict that the relative strength of inhibition evoked by different odors would be more uniform than excitation in individual cells. To address this, we examined the relative strength of excitation and inhibition in all cells that fired APs in response to odors (n=13). Excitation (EPSC charge) elicited by each odor was normalized to the largest odor-evoked excitatory response in each cell. Inhibition (IPSC charge) was normalized similarly. Responses in each cell were then ranked from the odor that produced the weakest excitation to the odor that produced the strongest and averaged across cells (Fig. 2E1). As we hypothesized for global inhibition, the strength of excitatory responses was graded, while the strength of inhibition was uniform across odors (Fig. 2E1).
Differing amounts of excitation and uniform inhibition implies that odors trigger APs based on the strength of excitation rather than odor-specific inhibition. Indeed, odors that elicited APs (preferred odors) also evoked greater excitation (average EPSC charge: 46.5±1.5 pC) than those that failed to produce spikes (nonpreferred odors, 16.9±0.7 pC, p=0.002) in the same cells (Fig. 2E2). In contrast, preferred and nonpreferred odors evoked identical amounts of inhibition (Fig. 2E2; average IPSC charge: 78.6±3.7 pC and 77.3±1.7 pC, respectively, p=0.81). Together, these results suggest that odor-evoked excitation must be strong enough to overcome global inhibition to generate APs in olfactory cortex.
To verify that our observations were not specific to our panel of test odors, we studied an additional set of cells (n=34 cells) using double the number of odors. We observed the same relative relationships in the selectivity (Fig. 3A1) and population responses (Fig. 3A2) of odor-evoked activity, i.e. APs were evoked sparsely and selectively, synaptic excitation was more common but specific, and inhibition was widespread and most broadly tuned. In a subset of these cells, we also examined the relationship between synaptic excitation and inhibition across a range of odor intensities. We varied odor concentration for cells that responded with excitation to multiple odors at our standard concentration of 5% SV. We found that odors were much more likely to evoke inhibition compared to excitation across a range of concentrations (0.25–10% SV). Reducing odor concentration from 5% to 2% SV led to a loss of excitatory responses to some odors while inhibitory responses to the same odors remained (Fig. 3B,C; n=9 cells). Indeed, as odor intensity was reduced further, odor-evoked inhibition could be observed in the absence of excitation (Fig. 3C). Furthermore, when normalized to the maximal synaptic responses we recorded at 10% SV, the relative amplitudes (charge) of inhibition at low odor concentrations was greater than excitation (Fig. 3D). Thus, inhibition is preferentially recruited across a wide range of odor intensities. Together, these results provide strong evidence that global inhibition is a fundamental property of olfactory cortical circuits.
Excitation onto local interneurons is broadly tuned
What accounts for global inhibition in olfactory cortex? One possibility is that, unlike principal cells, the local interneurons underlying inhibition receive widespread and broadly tuned excitation. To address this question, we filled cells with biocytin during whole-cell recording for post hoc classification. Interneurons were targeted by recording from cells in layer 1 (Neville and Haberly, 2004). Indeed, synaptic excitation was largely nonselective in morphologically identified interneurons (Fig. 4A1,2; n=18 cells), while identified pyramidal cells received selective excitation (Fig. 4B1,2; n=27 cells) similar to results from our larger L2/3 population. On average, individual odors evoked excitation in a greater fraction of interneurons compared to pyramidal cells (Fig. 4A3, B3; interneurons: 50±3.9%; pyramidal cells: 11±2.3%, p=0.003) and inhibition was recruited similarly in both cell types (p=0.2). These findings suggest that nonselective odor-evoked excitation of local interneurons could underlie global inhibition.
One mechanism that could lead to broadly tuned excitation onto interneurons is if they receive a higher convergence of olfactory bulb M/T cell inputs than pyramidal cells. We examined this possibility in vivo by placing a stimulating electrode in the LOT to directly activate M/T cell axons and recording LOT-evoked responses in L2/3 cells (Fig. 5A). At high stimulus intensities, LOT stimulation evoked monosynaptic EPSCs (Fig. 5B1) at a holding potential of −80 mV. We then lowered stimulus strength to reduce the number of recruited axons, such that stimulation failed to evoke EPSCs (Fig. 5B2). Changing the membrane potential to +10 mV revealed LOT-evoked IPSCs at the same stimulus intensity that failed to evoke EPSCs (Fig B3). Subsequent application of the glutamate receptor antagonist NBQX (500 μM) to the cortical surface abolished the IPSCs, indicating that they were evoked disynaptically (Fig 5B3). The onset times of IPSCs evoked with this “minimal” stimulation lagged behind monosynaptic EPSCs in the same cells (Fig. 5C), further confirming their disynaptic nature (Pouille and Scanziani, 2001). Disynaptic IPSCs could routinely be recruited in the absence of LOT-evoked EPSCs (Fig. 5D, n=5). These experiments suggest that interneurons governing inhibition in olfactory cortex receive a higher convergence of M/T cell input than pyramidal cells.
Oscillatory synaptic inputs govern spike timing
In sensory cortices receiving balanced excitation and inhibition, excitation precedes inhibition in response to brief impulse-like stimuli. This difference in the relative timing of excitation and inhibition is proposed to shape stimulus selectivity and precisely timed spike output (Priebe and Ferster, 2008; Wehr and Zador, 2003; Wilent and Contreras, 2005). In the mammalian olfactory system, respiratory modulation is a prominent feature governing the time course of odor-evoked activity (Cang and Isaacson, 2003; Litaudon et al., 2003; Margrie and Schaefer, 2003; Rennaker et al., 2007). We wondered whether the temporal relationship between odor-evoked excitation and inhibition could account for the timing of respiratory-coupled APs (Fig. 6A1). However, aligning odor-evoked synaptic currents to the respiratory rhythm revealed that inhibition and excitation were temporally overlapping (Fig. 6A,B; n=12 cells) and we could not resolve an obvious relationship between synaptic inputs and spikes times.
What then determines spike timing during slow, respiratory-coupled barrages of synaptic activity? Synchronized activity of ensembles of neurons is known to generate odor-evoked oscillations in local field potentials (LFPs) and phase-locked APs in higher olfactory centers of vertebrates and invertebrates (Adrian, 1942; Eeckman and Freeman, 1990; Freeman, 1978; Litaudon et al., 2008; Perez-Orive et al., 2002). To explore a temporal relationship between APs and synaptic input of L2/3 cells that may exist on a finer time scale than respiration, we recorded odor-evoked LFPs in layer 1 of anterior piriform cortex.
We found prominent, odor-evoked beta-frequency oscillations (mean=18.0 ± 1.7 Hz, n=10 rats) in the LFP (Fig. 7A), consistent with previous studies of behaving and anesthetized rats (Chapman et al., 1998; Lowry and Kay, 2007; Neville and Haberly, 2003). Beta oscillations were qualitatively similar for different odors and coupled to respiration (Fig 7B1). Simultaneous cell-attached recording of L2/3 cells and the LFP revealed that APs were phase-locked to LFP beta oscillations (Fig. 7B). In all cells, odor-evoked APs were coherent with the LFP at beta frequencies (Fig. 7C, n=9 cells). Intriguingly, the peaks of peri-oscillation triggered spike histograms (Fig. 7D1) indicated that APs were not coupled to the same phases of the beta oscillation across different cells. Rather, APs in each individual cell were preferentially coupled to specific phases of the LFP oscillation (Fig 7D2, n=7/9 cells, Rayleigh test, p < 0.05). LFP oscillations simultaneously recorded at the most rostral and caudal edges of anterior piriform cortex (~2.5 mm apart) were virtually coincident (lag: 1.2 ms, 0.11 radians), ruling out the possibility that cell specific AP-LFP phase relationships reflected varying distances between the site of LFP and AP recording. Furthermore, in cells that responded with APs to multiple odors (n=3 cells), the AP-LFP phase relationship appeared identical for each odor (data not shown). These results showing precise phase relationships between APs in individual neurons and synchronized network oscillations point to a temporally sparse code for odor representations in olfactory cortex (Laurent, 2002).
Given the tight temporal association between APs and beta oscillations, we examined the relationship between odor-evoked intracellular synaptic responses and the LFP (Fig. 8A1,2). We found that respiration-coupled barrages of EPSCs and IPSCs were coherent to the LFP at beta frequencies in all cells (Fig 8A3; n=9, p<0.05, coherence confidence limit). LFP-triggered averages of synaptic currents revealed that EPSCs always preceded IPSCs on a brief, millisecond timescale (Fig. 8B; average lag=9±0.3 ms). Strikingly, odor-evoked APs were largely confined to the narrow time windows when EPSCs led IPSCs in the same cells (Fig. 8C,D; n=3 cells). On average, 67±11% of APs during odor presentation occurred during the LFP period (~ 0.7π, 20 ms) corresponding to the time window between the onset of the EPSC and the 50% rise time of the IPSC. In contrast, only 32±12% of APs occurred during the same length of LFP period (~ 0.7 π) when measured from the onset of IPSC. In addition, only 8±2% of APs occurred during the LFP period (~ 0.4 π, 13 ms) corresponding to the interval from the 50% rise time of the IPSC to the time of its peak. We also found that synaptic excitation and inhibition were always coupled to distinct phases of the LFP beta oscillation in each cell (Fig. 8D, n=9 cells), consistent with the cell specific distribution of AP-LFP phases. Thus, while respiration imposes slow epochs of overlapping excitation and inhibition, odors evoke rapidly oscillating synaptic currents. Phase differences in oscillating EPSCs and IPSCs enforce precise spike timing in olfactory cortex.
Discussion
In this study, we show that odor representations are sparse in olfactory cortex. We find that sparse population activity is governed by selective excitation and global inhibition. Interneurons receiving widespread and broadly tuned excitation are poised to mediate global inhibition. We also explore the timing of odor-evoked spikes. We find that, in addition to slow respiratory patterning, spikes in principal cells are coupled to fast, beta frequency oscillations in the LFP. These precise and temporally sparse spikes are generated by oscillatory synaptic excitation that leads inhibition.
“Sparse” cortical odor representations
We wished to understand how neuronal populations in olfactory cortex represent individual odors. In other words, what is the typical response of the cortical population to a particular odor? Our approach differs from those that study representations of sensory stimuli by searching for the optimal stimulus for each cell, i.e. to define the “receptive field” of particular neurons. Measuring receptive fields is problematic in olfactory cortex since the number of odors that can potentially be encoded is vast and the topographical mapping of odor space within the cortex is unknown. Here, we used a small, fixed set of odors and data from individually recorded cells to reconstruct the overall population response. This approach allowed us to infer how individual stimuli (odors) are represented across the cortical population. A similar strategy has been used to explore the nature of stimulus representations in the insect olfactory system (Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Szyszka et al., 2005; Turner et al., 2008) and mammalian auditory cortex (Hromadka et al., 2008).
In contrast to extracellular unit recording, cell-attached recordings are not biased toward the detection of neurons with high firing rates. We used this method to sample the distribution of firing rates in olfactory cortex. We find that L2/3 cells in vivo have very low spontaneous activity (<1 Hz) and individual odors caused an increase in firing in ~10% of the cortical population. This is consistent with the idea that unique odors are represented by ensembles of active cells and that these cells are distributed similarly across the cortical population.
Given that individual odors can activate 10% of the cortical population, is it valid to describe odor representations as “sparse” in piriform cortex? It is important to bear in mind that the odor-responses of “active” cells were extremely weak. For responsive cells, odor-evoked increases in firing rate averaged only 2 Hz and only 6% of these cells had “well-driven” responses (>10 Hz). While we tested odors at a moderate concentration of 5% saturated vapor, it is likely that at lower concentrations even fewer cells within the cortical population would be active. Low spontaneous and evoked firing rates have also been reported in other cortical regions from anesthetized and awake animals when activity is measured using patch-clamp recording techniques (Brecht and Sakmann, 2002; DeWeese et al., 2003; Hromadka et al., 2008; Margrie et al., 2002). Together, the low firing rates, the small fraction of the population activated by individual odors, and the rarity of well-driven responses indicate that odor representations are sparse in olfactory cortex (Laurent, 2002; Olshausen and Field, 2004; Rolls and Tovee, 1995; Willmore and Tolhurst, 2001).
It has been reported that responses of olfactory bulb mitral cells to odorants are weaker and less frequently observed in awake behaving animals compared to ketamine/xylazine-anesthetized animals (Rinberg et al., 2006). Thus, odor representations in the olfactory bulb can be sparser in awake animals vs. those in the anesthetized state. While our experiments were performed under urethane anesthesia, a lower level of odor-evoked mitral cell activity may lead to sparser cortical odor representations in the awake, behaving state.
Global inhibition
Extracellular and immunohistological studies suggest that odors can activate ensembles of cells that are spatially dispersed (Illig and Haberly, 2003; Rennaker et al., 2007). The distribution of odor-evoked activity in olfactory cortex is fundamentally determined by the convergence (Franks and Isaacson, 2006) and divergence of M/T cell axon collaterals (Ojima et al., 1984). Anatomical studies suggest that single M/T cell axons terminate in broad, overlapping patches of olfactory cortex (Buonviso et al., 1991; Ojima et al., 1984). In addition, associative connections between pyramidal cells can amplify and further distribute excitation across the cortical population (Neville and Haberly, 2004; Rennaker et al., 2007). How does the olfactory cortical network counterbalance broadly distributed afferent excitatory input and highly associative connections to accomplish sparse odor-evoked spiking activity?
We propose that global inhibition is a major feature governing sparse odor representations in olfactory cortex. In contrast to the balanced excitation and inhibition elicited by stimuli in other primary sensory cortices (Anderson et al., 2000; Priebe and Ferster, 2008; Tan et al., 2004; Wehr and Zador, 2003; Wilent and Contreras, 2005), odor-evoked inhibition is widespread and nonselective in olfactory cortex. Global inhibition is poised to dampen odor-evoked excitatory responses across olfactory cortex such that only cells receiving strong and preferred excitation are driven to spike. In addition to promoting sparseness, global inhibition can contribute to cortical odor coding by providing gain control, noise suppression, and state-dependent modulation of cortical activity (Hensch and Fagiolini, 2004; Murakami et al., 2005).
We show that global inhibition is likely to reflect the fact that local interneurons receive ubiquitous odor-evoked excitation that is broadly tuned. We suggest that broadly tuned excitation of olfactory cortex interneurons is due to a higher convergence of M/T cell inputs to interneurons than principal cells. In support of this idea, we found that low intensity LOT stimulation consistently evoked disynaptic inhibition in the absence of excitation in pyramidal cells. While feedforward interneurons in olfactory cortex are likely to play an important role (Luna and Schoppa, 2008), local feedback circuits may also contribute to global inhibition.
Oscillating synaptic inputs govern spike timing
Neuronal oscillations are thought to be an important feature that contributes to the processing of information in cortical networks (Buzsaki and Draguhn, 2004; Salinas and Sejnowski, 2001). Fast rhythmic activity in the LFP is well documented in the olfactory systems of both vertebrates and invertebrates (Adrian, 1942; Chapman et al., 1998; Eeckman and Freeman, 1990; Freeman, 1978; Friedrich et al., 2004; Lowry and Kay, 2007; Neville and Haberly, 2003; Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Wehr and Laurent, 1996) and synchronous activity of neural ensembles is proposed to be important for odor coding, discrimination, and learning (Laurent, 2002).
We found that odors evoked respiration-coupled, beta-frequency oscillations in the olfactory cortex LFP. Although the precise mechanisms underlying beta frequency oscillations are unclear, they are thought to involve bidirectional connectivity between olfactory bulb and cortex (Neville and Haberly, 2003) and have been implicated during olfactory behavior (Kay and Stopfer, 2006). We show that while firing activity of individual L2/3 cells is slowly modulated by respiration, spike timing is precisely phase locked to beta frequency oscillations in the LFP. Furthermore, individual cells prefer to spike at difference phases of the LFP beta oscillation. Thus, across the cell population and within each breath, odors evoke spikes that are temporally sparse (Laurent, 2002).
What determines the LFP phase at which individual cells spike? Using whole-cell voltage clamp recordings, we show that cells receive excitatory and inhibitory currents coupled to discrete phases of the beta oscillation cycle. Inhibition always lagged excitation on a millisecond timescale and this temporal offset between oscillating EPSCs and IPSCs generated brief time windows governing spike timing. Thus, despite relatively slow respiratory patterning, rapidly oscillating synaptic activity enforces precise spike timing in olfactory cortex. Our results suggest that spike timing is important for odor representations in olfactory cortex and raises the intriguing possibility that cell-specific spike timing within active ensembles of L2/3 cells contributes to odor coding in brain regions receiving L2/3 projections.
Intriguingly, many of our findings parallel those obtained in the locust mushroom body (Laurent, 2002), a structure positioned at an equivalent stage of the insect olfactory system, but which shares no obvious homology or evolutionary relationship with the mammalian piriform cortex. The pyramidal cell equivalent in the mushroom body is the Kenyon cell and the similarities include: lifetime and population sparseness of principal cell responses, very low response firing rate deviation from baseline, direct and specific excitatory drive, broadly tuned inhibition, stimulus-triggered bursts of beta-range oscillations, and a phase-delay of inhibition relative to excitation. Indeed, there are relatively few functional differences across these diverse phyla. In locusts, broadly tuned inhibition of mushroom body Kenyon cells is mediated by feed-forward interneurons located in another region, the lateral horn (Perez-Orive et al., 2002). In the piriform cortex, broadly tuned inhibition is generated locally by feed-forward and perhaps feedback interneurons. While Kenyon cells fire spikes with a similar mean phase relationship to odor-evoked LFPs (Perez-Orive et al., 2002) recorded in the antenna lobe (the equivalent of the mammalian olfactory bulb), we find that the firing phase of individual pyramidal cells relative to the LFP varies across all cells. Overall, the remarkable similarities between the two different systems may reflect fundamental principles governing the processing of olfactory information in higher brain regions.
Sparse representations of stimuli have been found across a variety of sensory systems (Brecht and Sakmann, 2002; Davison and Katz, 2007; Hahnloser et al., 2002; Hromadka et al., 2008; Margrie et al., 2002; Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Rinberg et al., 2006; Vinje and Gallant, 2000). Sparseness is proposed to promote the efficient coding of sensory information in the brain by having a relatively small fraction of neurons within a large population active at any given time (Hromadka et al., 2008; Laurent, 2002; Olshausen and Field, 2004; Rolls and Tovee, 1995; Willmore and Tolhurst, 2001). Global inhibition and synchronized oscillatory synaptic currents are well suited to generate odor representations in olfactory cortex that are both spatially and temporally sparse.
Sparse coding is suggested to be an efficient means for representing sensory stimuli and is advantageous for associative learning (Laurent, 2002; Olshausen and Field, 2004). Thus, this coding scheme is ideal for the olfactory cortex given the immensity of potential odors and its importance for odor learning, recognition, and classification (Wilson et al., 2006). Indeed, in the insect olfactory system as well as the mammalian olfactory bulb, sparse activity is thought to be critical in the coding of odors (Davison and Katz, 2007; Fantana et al., 2008; Laurent, 2002; Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Rinberg et al., 2006). Our results suggest that sparse coding may be a fundamental strategy of olfactory systems that is highly conserved across diverse species.
Experimental Procedures
Surgical procedure
All animal experiments were performed in strict accordance with the guidelines of the National Institutes of Health and the University of California Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Sprague Dawley rats (p16–21) were anesthetized with urethane (1.8 g/kg) supplemented with atropine (0.2 mg/kg). Skin incisions were infused with lidocaine. Similar results were found in animals anesthetized with ketamine (n=3 cells, data not shown). Body temperature was maintained at 35–37 °C and animals were head-fixed on a custom stereotaxic fixture. After removing a section of temporomandibular muscle, the lateral olfactory tract (LOT) was visualized through the ventral surface of the skull. A small (~1 mm2) craniotomy was made lateral to the rhinal sulcus, ~1 mm caudal to the middle cerebral artery, and dorsal to the top edge of the LOT to expose the anterior piriform cortical surface. A larger craniotomy (~5 mm2) was made when LFPs were simultaneously recorded. For LOT stimulation experiments, an additional craniotomy was made ~1.5 mm anterior to the recording site. Respiration was monitored with a chest mounted piezo-electric strap.
Odor Stimuli
Odors were delivered via a computer-controlled olfactometer with a 1 liter/minute constant flow. Odors were diluted 1:10 in mineral oil, and further diluted with charcoal-filtered air to achieve a 5% saturated vapor (SV) in most experiments unless otherwise noted. Odors were presented ~1 cm from the snout in pseudo-randomized order. Odors were presented for 2 s with 60 s between presentations of individual odors. Odors were: cineole, amyl acetate, R-limonene, phenyl ethylalchol, eugenol, dimethyl pyrzadine, citral, and ethyl butyrate.
Electrophysiology
Cell-attached and whole-cell recordings were made with patch pipettes (5–7 MΩ) filled with (in mM): 130 cesium gluconate, 5 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 0.2 EGTA, 12 phosphocreatine, 3 Mg-ATP, 0.2 Na-GTP (7.25 pH; 290–300 mOsm). For data collected using only cell-attached recordings (n=177 cells), neurons were distinguished from glia or other non-neuronal structures by only considering cases in which at least one AP was detected over several minutes of recording. EPSCs were recorded at −80 mV, the reversal potential for inhibition set by our internal solution (ECl=−80 mV). Similarly, IPSCs were recorded at the reversal potential for excitation (~+10 mV). Series resistance for whole-cell recording was ≤30 MΩ and continuously monitored. Cells in which series resistance changed by >10% were excluded. L2/3 or layer 1 cells were targeted based on the z-axis readout of an MP-285 micromanipulator (Sutter). A stimulating electrode (FHC) placed within the LOT was used for LOT-evoked synaptic responses. Local field potentials (LFP) were recorded using a tungsten electrode (FHC) in layer 1a ~0.5 mm anterior to the patch electrode recording site.
Histology
Biocytin (0.2%) was added to the internal solution for experiments with post-hoc histological reconstruction. Briefly, after electrophysiology recordings, an overdose of urethane was given to the animal, after which the animal was decapitated and the whole brain extracted and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M phosphate-buffered saline. The recorded hemisphere was then sectioned into 200μm parasagital slices. To recover biocytin-filled cells in whole-mount, cells were revealed by a DAB reaction with nickel intensification. Slices were dehydrated in alcohols and xylenes and mounted in damar resin. These cells were then manually reconstructed using Neurolucida. Cells were identified as interneurons or pyramidal cells based on the following criteria: all layer 1 cells and L2/3 cells with a bipolar or multipolar dendritic tree were categorized as interneurons (Neville and Haberly, 2004). Pyramidal cells were identified as L2/3 cells possessing a clear apical dendrite and dendritic tree branching towards the LOT, in addition, cells must have basal dendrites that are confined within layers 2/3 (Neville and Haberly, 2004).
Data Acquisition and Analysis
Recordings were made with a MultiClamp 700A (Molecular Devices), digitized at 5 kHz (Instrutech), and acquired using AxographX (Axograph). Data were analyzed using custom routines in Matlab (Mathworks). Power and coherence spectra with confidence limits were calculated using multitapered methods (Jarvis and Mitra, 2001) and the Chronux package (NIMH). Cells were included in analysis only if >3 odor presentation trials for APs, EPSCs, and IPSCs were obtained. To determine AP responses to odors, we measured APs during a baseline period (2 s) prior to the odor application and during the 2 s odor presentation. Spikes were counted in 200 ms bins. Given the low firing rates of L2/3 cells, we used a combination of two criteria to determine evoked spike activity: 1) average firing rate threshold and 2) spike reliability. Cell-odor pairs needed to satisfy both criteria in order to be categorized as “responsive”. For cells that had spontaneous APs: 1) Average firing rate threshold: The average firing rate during the 2 s odor presentation needed to exceed the mean baseline rate + 2.5 standard deviations (S.D.) for ≥3 bins. 2) The firing rate in >50% of trails during odor presentation needed to exceed mean baseline firing rate + 2.5 S.D. in ≥1bin. We chose a threshold of 2.5 S.D. based on a simple receiver-operating characteristic (ROC) analysis (Fantana et al., 2008). Varying the threshold (in terms of mean firing rate + X S.D.) demonstrated that a threshold of 2.5 S.D. produces a true positive to false positive ratio of 93% (n=177 cells). Thus, we were confident that our method was appropriate for sensitively detecting odor-evoked responses.
For cells with no spontaneous APs: 1) Average firing rate threshold: The average firing rate during the 2s odor presentation needed to exceed 0.5 Hz. 2) The firing rate in >50% of trails during odor presentations needed to exceed 0.5 Hz. The median spontaneous rate was 0.28 Hz, thus, 0.5 Hz was a conservative threshold. We find that varying this threshold from 0.25 to 1 Hz did not alter the number of responsive cell-odor pairs.
Average odor-evoked spiking activity and synaptic currents were aligned to the first inhalation cycle in the presence of odor. Odor-evoked synaptic activity was measured by calculating the charge transfer (QOdor) during the 2 s odor presentation. Baseline response (QBaseline) was calculated from a 2 s period preceding odor onset. The criteria for a “positive” odor-evoked synaptic response was defined as Response Index (RI)=(QOdor/QBaseline) ≥1.6. This threshold value was also derived from ROC analysis of varying RI thresholds to obtain the optimal threshold producing a true positive to false positive ratio of >90% (Supplemental Figure 2).
To eliminate ambiguity inherent to binary classification of odor-cell pairs as responsive or nonresponsive, we used an additional selectivity measurement: lifetime sparseness (SL(Rolls and Tovee, 1995; Willmore and Tolhurst, 2001), which is independent of detection threshold. In brief, SL was calculated as (1 − {[SNj rj/N]2/{SNj [rj2/N]})/(1 − 1/N), where rj was the response of the neuron to odorant j (mean firing rate or charge transfer during odor presentation), and N was the total number of odors. This provides a measure of how much the response of a neuron was attributable entirely to one odor (highly selective, SL=1) versus equally distributed across all odors (SL=0). Population sparseness (Sp) was calculated with the same method, however, rj was the response of cell j to a single odor, and N was the total number of cells tested with this odor. In this case, Sp provides a measure of how much of the total population response was attributed entirely to one cell (highly sparse, Sp=1) versus equally distributed across all cells (Sp=0).
Beta oscillations were detected by digitally filtering the LFP between 8–30 Hz, which did not result in any phase shift, as confirmed by comparing beta troughs in filtered and raw traces. The oscillation cycle amplitude was defined as the peak-to-trough amplitude i.e. the difference between the peaks of a given cycle to the subsequent trough of the same cycle. Events with amplitudes ≥4 S.D. from the mean were detected. The peri-oscillation time histogram (POTH) for spikes and oscillation-triggered average for synaptic currents were determined using a method similar to spike-triggered averaging. In this case, however, the average was triggered by the trough of an oscillation cycle recorded in the LFP. Rayleigh test of non-uniformity was used for the POTH in each cell to evaluate significance of AP-LFP phase coupling. The POTH was fitted with a local linear regression (Chronux) in order to extract the peak firing time during an LFP oscillation cycle.
The phase-lag between EPSC and IPSC for each cell was accessed in two ways: time lag between the oscillation-triggered EPSC and IPSC transformed into phase as well as the phase lag between LFP-EPSC and LFP-IPSC at peak coherence. Both methods yielded identical results. Summary data and error bars are presented as mean±sem and statistical analysis was performed with paired t-tests unless otherwise noted.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to B. Atallah, R. Malinow, M. Scanziani, and C. Zuker for advice and encouragement. Supported by NIDCD (R01DC04682, J.S.I.) and NRSA (5F31DC009366, C.P.).
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Adrian ED. Olfactory reactions in the brain of the hedgehog. J Physiol. 1942;100:459–473. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1942.sp003955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JS, Carandini M, Ferster D. Orientation tuning of input conductance, excitation, and inhibition in cat primary visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2000;84:909–926. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.2.909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bathellier B, Buhl DL, Accolla R, Carleton A. Dynamic ensemble odor coding in the mammalian olfactory bulb: sensory information at different timescales. Neuron. 2008;57:586–598. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brecht M, Sakmann B. Dynamic representation of whisker deflection by synaptic potentials in spiny stellate and pyramidal cells in the barrels and septa of layer 4 rat somatosensory cortex. J Physiol. 2002;543:49–70. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.018465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buonviso N, Amat C, Litaudon P. Respiratory modulation of olfactory neurons in the rodent brain. Chem Senses. 2006;31:145–154. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjj010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buonviso N, Revial MF, Jourdan F. The Projections of Mitral Cells from Small Local Regions of the Olfactory Bulb: An Anterograde Tracing Study Using PHA-L (Phaseolus vulgaris Leucoagglutinin) Eur J Neurosci. 1991;3:493–500. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1991.tb00836.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G, Draguhn A. Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science. 2004;304:1926–1929. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cang J, Isaacson JS. In vivo whole-cell recording of odor-evoked synaptic transmission in the rat olfactory bulb. J Neurosci. 2003;23:4108–4116. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-04108.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman CA, Xu Y, Haykin S, Racine RJ. Beta-frequency (15–35 Hz) electroencephalogram activities elicited by toluene and electrical stimulation in the behaving rat. Neuroscience. 1998;86:1307–1319. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(98)00092-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davison IG, Katz LC. Sparse and selective odor coding by mitral/tufted neurons in the main olfactory bulb. J Neurosci. 2007;27:2091–2101. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3779-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeWeese MR, Wehr M, Zador AM. Binary spiking in auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2003;23:7940–7949. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-21-07940.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eeckman FH, Freeman WJ. Correlations between unit firing and EEG in the rat olfactory system. Brain Res. 1990;528:238–244. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91663-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fantana AL, Soucy ER, Meister M. Rat olfactory bulb mitral cells receive sparse glomerular inputs. Neuron. 2008;59:802–814. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franks KM, Isaacson JS. Strong single-fiber sensory inputs to olfactory cortex: implications for olfactory coding. Neuron. 2006;49:357–363. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.12.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman WJ. Spatial properties of an EEG event in the olfactory bulb and cortex. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1978;44:586–605. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(78)90126-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedrich RW, Habermann CJ, Laurent G. Multiplexing using synchrony in the zebrafish olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:862–871. doi: 10.1038/nn1292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser RH, Kozhevnikov AA, Fee MS. An ultra-sparse code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a songbird. Nature. 2002;419:65–70. doi: 10.1038/nature00974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartline HK, Wagner HG, Ratliff F. Inhibition in the eye of Limulus. J Gen Physiol. 1956;39:651–673. doi: 10.1085/jgp.39.5.651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hensch TK, Fagiolini M. Excitatory-inhibitory balance: synapses, circuits, systems. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Hromadka T, Deweese MR, Zador AM. Sparse representation of sounds in the unanesthetized auditory cortex. PLoS Biol. 2008;6:e16. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Illig KR, Haberly LB. Odor-evoked activity is spatially distributed in piriform cortex. J Comp Neurol. 2003;457:361–373. doi: 10.1002/cne.10557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarvis MR, Mitra PP. Sampling properties of the spectrum and coherency of sequences of action potentials. Neural Comput. 2001;13:717–749. doi: 10.1162/089976601300014312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay LM, Stopfer M. Information processing in the olfactory systems of insects and vertebrates. Semin Cell Dev Biol. 2006;17:433–442. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2006.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laurent G. Olfactory network dynamics and the coding of multidimensional signals. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:884–895. doi: 10.1038/nrn964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Litaudon P, Amat C, Bertrand B, Vigouroux M, Buonviso N. Piriform cortex functional heterogeneity revealed by cellular responses to odours. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:2457–2461. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02654.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Litaudon P, Garcia S, Buonviso N. Strong coupling between pyramidal cell activity and network oscillations in the olfactory cortex. Neuroscience. 2008;156:781–787. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.07.077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowry CA, Kay LM. Chemical factors determine olfactory system beta oscillations in waking rats. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98:394–404. doi: 10.1152/jn.00124.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luna VM, Schoppa NE. GABAergic circuits control input-spike coupling in the piriform cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:8851–8859. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2385-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margrie TW, Brecht M, Sakmann B. In vivo, low-resistance, whole-cell recordings from neurons in the anaesthetized and awake mammalian brain. Pflugers Arch. 2002;444:491–498. doi: 10.1007/s00424-002-0831-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margrie TW, Schaefer AT. Theta oscillation coupled spike latencies yield computational vigour in a mammalian sensory system. J Physiol. 2003;546:363–374. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.031245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mombaerts P, Wang F, Dulac C, Chao SK, Nemes A, Mendelsohn M, Edmondson J, Axel R. Visualizing an olfactory sensory map. Cell. 1996;87:675–686. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81387-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murakami M, Kashiwadani H, Kirino Y, Mori K. State-dependent sensory gating in olfactory cortex. Neuron. 2005;46:285–296. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neville KR, Haberly LB. Beta and gamma oscillations in the olfactory system of the urethane-anesthetized rat. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90:3921–3930. doi: 10.1152/jn.00475.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neville KR, Haberly LB. Olfactory Cortex. 5. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Ojima H, Mori K, Kishi K. The trajectory of mitral cell axons in the rabbit olfactory cortex revealed by intracellular HRP injection. J Comp Neurol. 1984;230:77–87. doi: 10.1002/cne.902300107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olshausen BA, Field DJ. Sparse coding of sensory inputs. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14:481–487. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perez-Orive J, Mazor O, Turner GC, Cassenaer S, Wilson RI, Laurent G. Oscillations and sparsening of odor representations in the mushroom body. Science. 2002;297:359–365. doi: 10.1126/science.1070502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pouille F, Scanziani M. Enforcement of temporal fidelity in pyramidal cells by somatic feed-forward inhibition. Science. 2001;293:1159–1163. doi: 10.1126/science.1060342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Priebe NJ, Ferster D. Inhibition, spike threshold, and stimulus selectivity in primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2008;57:482–497. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rennaker RL, Chen CF, Ruyle AM, Sloan AM, Wilson DA. Spatial and temporal distribution of odorant-evoked activity in the piriform cortex. J Neurosci. 2007;27:1534–1542. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4072-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rinberg D, Koulakov A, Gelperin A. Sparse odor coding in awake behaving mice. J Neurosci. 2006;26:8857–8865. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0884-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Tovee MJ. Sparseness of the neuronal representation of stimuli in the primate temporal visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1995;73:713–726. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.2.713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin BD, Katz LC. Optical imaging of odorant representations in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Neuron. 1999;23:499–511. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80803-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salinas E, Sejnowski TJ. Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:539–550. doi: 10.1038/35086012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soucy ER, Albeanu DF, Fantana AL, Murthy VN, Meister M. Precision and diversity in an odor map on the olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:210–220. doi: 10.1038/nn.2262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spors H, Grinvald A. Spatio-temporal dynamics of odor representations in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2002;34:301–315. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00644-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szyszka P, Ditzen M, Galkin A, Galizia CG, Menzel R. Sparsening and temporal sharpening of olfactory representations in the honeybee mushroom bodies. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:3303–3313. doi: 10.1152/jn.00397.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan AY, Zhang LI, Merzenich MM, Schreiner CE. Tone-evoked excitatory and inhibitory synaptic conductances of primary auditory cortex neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92:630–643. doi: 10.1152/jn.01020.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner GC, Bazhenov M, Laurent G. Olfactory representations by Drosophila mushroom body neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99:734–746. doi: 10.1152/jn.01283.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uchida N, Takahashi YK, Tanifuji M, Mori K. Odor maps in the mammalian olfactory bulb: domain organization and odorant structural features. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:1035–1043. doi: 10.1038/79857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vinje WE, Gallant JL. Sparse coding and decorrelation in primary visual cortex during natural vision. Science. 2000;287:1273–1276. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5456.1273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wachowiak M, Cohen LB. Representation of odorants by receptor neuron input to the mouse olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2001;32:723–735. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00506-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wehr M, Laurent G. Odour encoding by temporal sequences of firing in oscillating neural assemblies. Nature. 1996;384:162–166. doi: 10.1038/384162a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wehr M, Zador AM. Balanced inhibition underlies tuning and sharpens spike timing in auditory cortex. Nature. 2003;426:442–446. doi: 10.1038/nature02116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilent WB, Contreras D. Dynamics of excitation and inhibition underlying stimulus selectivity in rat somatosensory cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1364–1370. doi: 10.1038/nn1545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willmore B, Tolhurst DJ. Characterizing the sparseness of neural codes. Network. 2001;12:255–270. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson DA, Kadohisa M, Fletcher ML. Cortical contributions to olfaction: plasticity and perception. Semin Cell Dev Biol. 2006;17:462–470. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2006.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zou Z, Li F, Buck LB. Odor maps in the olfactory cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:7724–7729. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0503027102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar] [Retracted]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.