Abstract
This longitudinal study examined the effect of the birth of the first child on relationship functioning using data from 218 couples (436 individuals) over the course of the first 8 years of marriage. Compared to pre-birth levels and trajectories, parents showed sudden deterioration following birth on observed and self-reported measures of positive and negative aspects of relationship functioning. The deterioration in these variables was small to medium in size and tended to persist throughout the remaining years of the study. Mothers and fathers showed similar amounts of change after birth. The amount of post-birth deterioration in relationship functioning varied systematically by several characteristics of the individual, the marriage, and the pregnancy itself. In a group of couples who did not have children, results indicated more gradual deterioration in relationship functioning during the first 8 years of marriage without the sudden changes seen in parents, suggesting that the results seen in the parent sample may be due to birth.
Keywords: Transition to parenthood, Parent, Relationship functioning, Prediction
For decades, the marital and family literature has debated whether having a baby causes substantial declines in the average couple’s relationship. It is difficult to overstate the importance of this question. For married couples, the first child is often born within the first five years of marriage — a period that has been shown to hold the highest risk for divorce (Bramlett & Mosher, 2001). If parenthood truly is a crisis, as suggested by early investigators (e.g., Lemasters, 1957), then clarification of the impact of a baby’s birth could lead to increased understanding of early marital difficulties. Moreover, the quality of a couple’s relationship following a baby’s birth has critical implications for numerous aspects of the baby’s early development, including physiological arousal (Gottman, Driver, & Tabares, 2002), attachment (e.g., De Wolff et al., 1997) and language development (e.g., Horwitz et al., 2003). Later child development (e.g., psychological, social, and school functioning) is also related to the status and quality of parent relationships (e.g., Amato, 2001, Davies & Cummings, 1994).
Despite this importance, there is little agreement among leading researchers about the impact of the transition to parenthood on relationship functioning. Cowan and Cowan state “we can conclude with some confidence that the transition to parenthood constitutes a period of stressful and sometimes maladaptive change for a significant proportion of new parents” (1995, p. 412). However, Huston and Holmes conclude “The preponderance of the data fail to support the view that parenthood typically undermines marital satisfaction” (2004, p. 109). These disagreements arise primarily from the different methodologies that have been used to study the effect of the transition to parenthood; therefore, these methodologies are reviewed below.
Impact of the Transition to Parenthood on Relationships
Cross-Sectional Studies of Parenthood
One of the most straightforward ways to examine the impact of having a child is to conduct a cross-sectional study comparing parents with non-parents. In the most recent meta-analysis of this research, parents reported significantly lower relationship satisfaction than non-parents (d = -.19; Twenge, Campbell, & Foster, 2003). Although this finding provides preliminary support for the notion that having a child is associated with declines in relationship quality, there are important limitations of cross-sectional designs in determining the true effect. Parents and non-parents are not equivalent groups and, as identified by Huston and Holmes (2004), parents may be less satisfied than non-parents for several other reasons: 1) they may be less satisfied even before having children; 2) having children serves as a barrier to divorce but not to dissatisfaction; 3) couples who become parents may differ from non-parents in other unmodeled but meaningful ways; and 4) a cross-sectional sample of parents is likely to include relationships of longer duration than a sample of non-parents. Thus, cross-sectional designs can provide only limited information on the effect of having children on relationship functioning.
Longitudinal Studies Beginning in Pregnancy
Given the critical limitations of cross-sectional designs, researchers have recruited pregnant couples and examined longitudinal changes in relationship functioning from pregnancy onward (e.g., Belsky, Lang, & Huston, 1986; Belsky & Rovine, 1990; Clements & Markman, 1996; Collins, Dunkel-Schetter, Lobel, & Scrimshaw, 1993; Cowan & Cowan, 2000; Gottman et al., 2002; Grote & Clark, 2001; O’Brien & Peyton, 2002; Rholes, Simpson, Campbell, & Grich, 2001). These longitudinal studies have shown declines in relationship satisfaction following birth of a full standard deviation or more in 20-59% of couples (see Cowan & Cowan, 1995) and “precipitous” drops in 70% of couples (e.g., Gottman et al., 2002). Indeed, one study showed that almost one third of partners fall into the clinical range of marital distress during the first 18 months after birth (Cowan & Cowan, 2000). Research has also demonstrated that parents show declines in positive relationship events and relationship-focused leisure time (e.g., MacDermid, Huston, & McHale, 1990), social support (Simpson, Rholes, Campbell, Tran, & Wilson, 2003), and increases in marital conflict (Cowan & Cowan, 2000).
These deteriorations in relationship functioning after birth have been documented for couples from a wide range of ethnicities, including both Caucasian and African-American couples within the United States (Crohan, 1996) as well as for couples in Asia (Lu, 2006) and Europe (e.g., Clulow, 1982; Lorensen, Wilson, & White, 2004; Salmela-Aro, Aunola, Saisto, Halmesmaki, & Nurmi, 2006). Likewise, lesbian couples, who conceived a child through artificial insemination, evidenced decreases in love and increases in relationship conflict after the birth of their baby (Goldberg & Sayer, 2006). Therefore, some relationship deterioration after birth appears to be a relatively universal event for the average couple.
At the same time, evidence suggests that there may be gender differences in the magnitude and timing of changes in relationship functioning after birth. Several studies suggest that mothers tend to demonstrate sudden declines in relationship satisfaction after birth while fathers show more gradual declines that are not evident until 6 to 15 months after birth (e.g., Belsky & Hsieh, 1998; Grote & Clark, 2001). Additionally, some studies suggest that the magnitude of changes in relationship satisfaction differs by gender (e.g., O’Brien & Peyton, 2002), although the direction of effect may depend on the timing of measurement.
Despite these important findings, there are limitations to the knowledge gained from longitudinal studies of parents. Similar to cross-sectional research, it is impossible to determine from these studies whether the declines that are typically seen after the birth would have occurred if the same couples had not had a baby. Further, there are two major reasons to suspect that these studies overestimate the impact of the transition to parenthood on relationship functioning (see Huston & Holmes, 2004). First, by first assessing couples during pregnancy, it is possible that the observed declines are simply a return to the couple’s baseline from a “honeymoon” of cooperation and togetherness that couples experience during pregnancy (Feeney et al., 2001). Thus, it seems important to examine trajectories both before and after birth. Second, given that relationship satisfaction declines on average during the early years of marriage (e.g., Clements & Markman, 1996; Huston, Caughlin, Houts, Smith, & George, 2001; Karney & Bradbury, 1997), natural maturation processes may be mistaken for the effects of birth.
Inclusion of Non-Parents in Longitudinal Samples
To partially address the last concern, studies have begun to utilize non-parents as a non-equivalent control group. With a non-parent group, it is possible to determine whether changes experienced after the birth of a child are also experienced by couples without children. The inclusion of non-parents as a non-equivalent control group allows one to investigate whether changes observed in parents after the birth of a baby are also observed in couples who do not have a baby. Indeed, results suggest that parents and non-parents tend to experience similar declines in relationship quality in most studies (e.g., Clements & Markman, 1996; Huston & Holmes, 2004; Kurdek, 1993; MacDermid et al., 1990; McHale & Huston, 1985; White & Booth, 1995), though there are exceptions (e.g., Crohan, 1996; Lawrence, Rothman, Cobb, Rothman, & Bradbury, 2008; Shapiro et al., 2000). However, parents seem to consistently show sharper declines in cohesion and relationship-focused activities compared to non-parents (e.g., Kurdek, 1993; MacDermid et al., 1990).
In considering the implications of these studies, it is important to recognize that, because parents and non-parents differ in important ways before birth (e.g., MacDermid et al., 1990; Shapiro et al., 2000), changes that non-parents experience cannot be assumed to represent what parents would experience if they had not had a child. As a result, the field has yet to answer the central question of whether the transition to parenthood can be identified as the causal factor in the commonly noted declines in relationship quality among parents.
Using Information about Timing of Changes
Moving from a statement of the transition to parenthood being “associated with” to “causing” changes in the couple relationship is difficult because an experimental design is not possible; couples cannot be randomly assigned to give birth or not give birth. Instead, the field must rely on quasi-experimental designs — designs that use strategies other than random assignment or additional pieces of information to rule out potential confounds. One essential piece of information that can address whether parenthood plays a causal role in relationship deterioration after birth has been largely ignored in the literature — the shape of change in relationship functioning before and after the birth of the first baby.1 By analyzing the levels and trajectories of change in relationship functioning before and after birth, a particular type of quasi-experimental design — an interrupted time-series (ITS) design (c.f., Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002) — can provide a wealth of information that permits us to comment on the probable effect of the transition to parenthood on couple functioning.
An ITS design allows one to examine a series of measurements of a construct over time to determine if a particular event (here, birth of the first child) affected that construct’s time series in a predictable fashion. Within the context of the transition to parenthood, we can ask whether the birth affects ongoing changes in relationship functioning in predictable ways. In particular, given the changes that were occurring before birth, an ITS design can illuminate whether relationships show no additional change after birth, whether they show a sudden increase or decrease in relationship constructs (i.e., a change in level) following birth, or whether they show modifications to the rate of change in a construct from pre- to post-birth (i.e., a change in slope). For example, using an ITS design, we can ask whether there is a sudden increase/decrease in relationship satisfaction following the birth of the baby as well as whether any changes/stability before birth in relationship satisfaction continued to hold after the birth of the baby. As a result, this approach isolates change that can be attributed to birth from change that was expected based on ongoing changes in the couples’ relationship. Additionally, by following couples for years after birth, this approach also allows us to determine whether the initial changes that couples experience after birth persist over time.
Combining ITS designs with the series of statistical model fitting procedures used in the present study, we can also determine the most parsimonious model of change in relationship constructs over time for both parents and non-parents. For example, it may be that a linear or quadratic model of change is the most parsimonious model for non-parents but that models for parents require additional shifts in intercepts or slopes as discussed above. Therefore, the combination of a model-fitting and ITS approach permits direct statistical comparison of different change models and, within each of these models, statistical tests of various model components. ITS designs have been used investigate the effects of various interventions (e.g., Biglan, Metzler, & Ary, 1994; Tilden & Shepherd, 1987; Velicer, 1994) using both within- and between-person time series, but it has not, to our knowledge, been applied to the transition to parenthood. If the assumptions of the model are met, causal conclusions from an ITS design are nearly as strong as from a randomized design (Shadish et al., 2002).
Predictors of Relationship Changes over the Transition to Parenthood
To this point, we have been discussing the transition to parenthood as though all couples evidence deterioration in relationship functioning. However, not all couples experience the same changes over the transition to parenthood; as noted earlier, approximately one third (e.g., Shapiro, Gottman, & Carrère, 2000) to one half (e.g., Belsky & Hsieh, 1998; Belsky & Rovine, 1990) of couples evidence stability or increases in relationship satisfaction or love over the transition to parenthood. Therefore, it is important to examine variability in post-birth changes.
One way to understand changes in post-birth relationship functioning is by using the conceptual framework of the Vulnerability-Stress-Adaptation (VSA) model (Karney & Bradbury, 1995). In the VSA model, changes in relationship quality are understood to be a function of three interrelated constructs: enduring vulnerabilities, stressful events, and adaptive processes. Enduring vulnerabilities of the individual and the couple (e.g., limited education, cohabitation history) increase chances of experiencing events as stressful and adapting poorly to those events. Additionally, although all the couples in the current study’s sample of parents experienced the same potentially stressful event (birth of their first baby), the nature of that stressful event can vary substantially between couples. For example, the timing of the birth and the gender of the baby could alter the impact of the first baby on relationship functioning. Finally, couples have different levels of adaptive processes (e.g., communication, commitment) to help them cope with the stresses placed on their relationship functioning after birth. The VSA model suggests that each of these components, enduring vulnerabilities, the nature and timing of stressful events, and the degree to which couples can engage in adaptive processes, must be considered to understand changes in relationship functioning (Karney & Bradbury, 1995). Thus, the hypotheses for the current study are organized around this VSA model. Before detailing those hypotheses, we first discuss the existing literature on predictors of post-birth functioning by these three constructs.
Enduring Vulnerabilities
Several studies have investigated how various enduring vulnerabilities impact the transition to parenthood. For example, poor functioning in individuals’ families of origin has been shown to predict declines in both marital satisfaction (Cowan & Cowan, 2000; Perren, Wyl, Burgin, Simoni, & Klitzing, 2005) and observed communication (Perren et al., 2005) over the transition to parenthood. However, the literature is mixed on whether conflict in both partners’ family of origin (Perren et al., 2005) or only the husband’s family of origin (Cowan & Cowan, 2000) is predictive of declines in relationship functioning. Furthermore, although not investigated over the transition to parenthood, divorce and conflict in the family of origin may have differential impacts on satisfaction (e.g., Story, Karney, Lawrence, & Bradbury, 2004).
Additionally, there is a growing body of literature suggesting that couples who cohabit before marriage are at higher risk for a number of subsequent marital difficulties (e.g., Cohan, & Kleinbaum, 2002; Stanley, Whitton, & Markman, 2004). The effect of premarital cohabitation on relationship functioning after birth has yet to be examined to our knowledge. Finally, level of religiosity has been shown to moderate the impact of the transition to parenthood on marital satisfaction. Highly religious mothers experienced greater gains in marital satisfaction after birth than those with lower levels of religiousness (Nock, Sanchez, & Wright, in press).
Nature of Stressful Event
Belsky and Rovine (1990) found that couples with unplanned pregnancies saw more deterioration in wife-reported (but not husband-reported) love, conflict, and ambivalence. Therefore, in the present study, individuals’ reports of whether the pregnancy was planned were used as predictors of change after birth. Additionally, the existing evidence is mixed on whether having a child shortly after marriage is a risk factor for subsequent declines in relationship functioning. Belsky and Rovine (1990) found that couples who had been together longer before birth showed smaller increases in relationship conflict and smaller decreases in husbands’ reports of love. In contrast, O’Brien and Peyton (2002) found no relation between relationship duration and changes after birth. In the present study, timing of the birth after marriage will be investigated as a predictor of change in relationship functioning.
Additionally, the amount of personal income at birth has been examined as a predictor of the transition to parenthood. One study found that higher income served as a buffer against increases in marital conflict and fathers’ ambivalence after birth (Belsky & Rovine, 1990) while other studies (e.g., O’Brien & Peyton, 2002) have found no effect. One possible explanation for these mixed findings is that income is a single indicator of what is likely a complex construct composed of financial resources, employment pressures, and financial stressors. In the present study, both current income and amount of financial stress are used as predictors to separate these constructs. In the present study, these variables are considered to reflect differing aspects of the stressful event (rather than enduring vulnerabilities) because they varied across time within couples; thus, giving birth at a time of relatively higher income and less financial stress for that couple could serve to buffer the couple against some of the stressors of birth.
Finally, the impact of the child’s gender on relationship functioning is also mixed: some studies have shown that having a female child places couples at higher risk for relationship problems after birth (e.g., Cox et al., 1999; Raley & Bianchi, 2006) while other studies have found no effect of child gender (e.g., Kurdek, 1993). This study will examine the impact of child gender on relationship functioning.
Adaptive Processes
Perhaps the most examined predictors of change after the transition to parenthood are levels of relationship functioning during pregnancy. Higher levels of both self-reported (e.g., Cox, Paley, Burchinal, & Payne, 1999; Crohan, 1996; Kluwer & Johnson, 2007) and observed (e.g., Shapiro et al., 2000) poor communication or conflict during pregnancy have been consistently found to predict larger declines in relationship functioning. However, previous studies have been mixed as to whether having higher levels of positive relationship qualities (e.g., satisfaction, romance) are associated with more or less deterioration after birth. In one study, greater marital satisfaction assessed just after birth predicted fewer declines in marital satisfaction during the following three years (O’Brien & Peyton, 2002). However, another study found that higher levels of romance during pregnancy predicted significantly greater deterioration in husbands’ self-reported love as well as increases in both husbands’ and wives’ reported levels of relationship conflict (Belsky & Rovine, 1990).
Additionally, the present study examined two constructs that have not, to our knowledge, been examined as predictors of relationship functioning over the transition to parenthood: relationship commitment and confidence. Commitment has been shown to be an important construct in understanding general couple functioning and stability (Impett, Beals, & Peplau, 2001; Stanley & Markman, 1992); thus we wished to examine how it relates to the transition to parenthood. Additionally, one’s sense of confidence in the future of the relationship has been linked with not only better general relationship functioning, but also with lower levels of subsequent depression (Whitton et al., 2007). However, overly positive expectations about one’s relationship and partner have also been found to predict more deterioration in relationship satisfaction among newlywed couples, especially when paired with high levels of poor communication (McNulty & Karney, 2004).
The Present Study
In Study 1, we explore four questions. First, within parents, is there evidence of change in relationship functioning following the birth of the baby that is distinct from any change that was occurring before birth? While a true experiment cannot be conducted to assess the effects of the transition to parenthood, marked post-birth changes from trajectories of relationship changes that were otherwise expected would argue for a causal contribution of the transition to parenthood. Changes in several aspects of relationship functioning will be explored: relationship satisfaction, observed and self-reported communication, problem intensity, as well as relationship dedication and confidence. Second, if a change in relationship functioning is evident, what types of changes occur? Specifically, we will examine both sudden changes in mean levels of relationship functioning as well as more gradual modifications to rates of change after the birth of the baby. Consistent with previous research, we expect that parents will show a post-birth decline in relationship functioning that is clearly associated with the transition itself. However, rather than simply fitting a linear or quadratic model used in previous research, the present analyses seeks to fit the ITS model that best estimates the type and shape of this decline so that the most accurate picture of post-birth change can be obtained and tested. Third, if changes in relationship functioning are found, we will determine whether there is there significant between-individual variability in those changes after birth. Fourth, for relationship functioning variables with significant post-birth variability, we will examine which enduring vulnerabilities, aspects of the stressful event, and adaptive processes predict changes seen after the birth of the baby.
In Study 2, a sample of couples who did not have children will be used to determine whether developmental processes unrelated to having a baby can serve as alternative explanations to the findings in Study 1. Specifically, we will determine whether changes that appear to be a result of the birth of the first baby in the parent sample are also observed in a sample of couples who did not have children during the same period. If similar changes are not found, then we can be more confident that the changes in Study 1 were attributable to birth.
Study 1: Parent Sample
Method
Participants
Couples in the present study had participated in a larger investigation of relationship development and the effectiveness of premarital education (see Markman et al., 2004; Stanley et al., 2001). Couples who had children from previous relationships or who did not complete an assessment at any point before having a child together were excluded. There were 132 couples who had their first child during the first eight years of the study (“parents”); these couples were used in the present study’s main analyses. When parents began participation they were 26.38 years old (SD = 4.42), on average, with 15.63 years of education (SD = 1.89). The median personal income level was $20,000-29,999 and they were 89.8% White and 8.0% Latino; 2.2% were of some other race/ethnicity. Sixty eight percent of couples cohabited before marriage. Twenty seven percent of couples reported attending church at least once a week and, on average, couples described themselves as “somewhat” religious. Forty six percent of couples give birth to a girl; additionally, approximately 10% of both fathers and mothers reported that their pregnancy was unplanned. During the course of the present study, three couples divorced and three couples withdrew from the study; however, all available data from these couples was utilized in analyses.
Procedures
Couples were recruited before marriage through the religious organizations that would perform their weddings. These religious organizations were randomly assigned to require couples using their wedding services to either receive the naturally occurring premarital education services within the religious organization or the Prevention and Relationship Enhancement Program (PREP; Markman, Stanley, & Blumberg, 2002; Stanley, Blumberg, & Markman, 1999). Sixty-two percent of the parent sample completed PREP; the remainder of the sample received naturally occurring premarital education. The naturally-occurring interventions reflected “treatment as usual” for premarital education efforts. Couples in that condition received an average of 5.9 hours of premarital education that reflected the gamut of approached typically conducted by religious organizations (see Stanley et al., 2001 for more details). Below, we test whether couples who received PREP differed from couples who received the naturally-occurring intervention during the transition to parenthood.
As part of the larger study, couples were asked to complete measures and videotaped interactions before they were married and prior to premarital education (T1), just after education (T2), and yearly thereafter (T3-T10). However, in the present study, data on the dependent variables from the T1 assessment were omitted from analyses to avoid confounding change during the education program with natural change over time. Except for demographic information that was not expected to change over time (e.g., ethnicity) and, where noted, the following measures were administered at every follow-up assessment. During each laboratory visit, partners completed questionnaires individually and completed a videotaped 10-15 minute problem-solving discussion together. Couples were paid $40 - $100 for each assessment, depending on the time point. All procedures of the study were approved by the university Institutional Review Board and each individual in the study provided written informed consent.
Measures of Relationship Variables
Marital satisfaction
We used the first item from the Marital Adjustment Test (MAT; Locke & Wallace, 1959) to assess satisfaction. The item asks participants to rate the “degree of happiness” they experience in their relationships on a 1 (very unhappy) to 7 (perfectly happy) scale. Consistent with previous calls (e.g., Fincham & Bradbury, 1987; Huston & Vangelisti, 1995), this item, rather than the full MAT, was used to obtain a pure measure of marital satisfaction that was not confounded with communication and commitment. This single item of satisfaction has been found to provide high levels of information about individuals’ relationship satisfaction across a broad range of relationship functioning (Funk & Rogge, 2007)
Observed negative communication
The Interactional Dimensions Coding System, a global coding system for couples’ discussions of relationship problems (Julien, Markman, & Lindahl, 1989; Kline et al., 2004), was used to code couples’ videotaped problem discussions. Intercoder reliability was high, with intraclass correlations ranging from .66 to .95 (Mdn = .87; Kline et al., 2004). This study used only the negative communication subscale (made up of withdrawal, denial, conflict, dominance, and negative affect, α = .86 for mothers; .88 for fathers).
Relationship confidence
We used the 10-item Confidence Scale (Stanley, Hoyer, & Trathen, 1994; Whitton et al., 2007) to measure one’s sense that he/she and the partner can effectively manage their relationship and stay together. Participants rated each item from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Cronbach’s alpha was .83 for mothers and .79 for fathers.
Relationship dedication
We used the 14-item Dedication Scale from the revised Commitment Inventory (Stanley & Markman, 1992) to measure dedication. This measure captures willingness to sacrifice for the relationship, intrinsic desire to maintain the relationship for the long term, considering the relationship a priority, and couple identity. Participants rated each item on a 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) scale. The Dedication Scale has shown acceptable levels of internal consistency across a range of samples (e.g., Adams & Jones, 1997; Stanley & Markman, 1992). In the current sample, the internal consistency is lowest at T1 (mothers, α = .52; for fathers, α = .68) due to restriction of range; almost all couples reported being very dedicated to their relationship. At later time points, reliability is higher (e.g., at T7, α = .74 for mothers and α = .76 for fathers).
Poor conflict management
The Communication Danger Signs Scale (Stanley & Markman, 1997) was used to assess self-reported conflict management. It has demonstrated adequate validity and reliability in two random samples (Johnson et al., 2002; Stanley, Whitton, & Markman, 2004). Only the seven (of eight) items related to the frequency of negative interaction patterns were used. Sample items include “My partner criticizes or belittles my opinions, feelings, or desires” and “Little arguments escalate into ugly fights with accusations, criticisms, name calling, or bringing up past hurts.” Each item is rated on a 1 (almost never) to (frequently) scale. For mothers, α = .71; for fathers, α = .74.
Problem intensity
The Marital Agendas Protocol (MAP; Notarius & Vanzetti, 1983) asks participants to rate the intensity of their relationship problems in 16 areas (e.g., communication, money, sex) on a 0 to 100 scale. Scores reflect the average intensity across all areas, and higher scores reflect the intensity of how much of a problem the issues typically faced by young couples are from the participant’s perspective. The MAP has been shown to be reliable and discriminates distressed from non-distressed couples (Notarius & Vanzetti, 1983). In the present study, for mothers, α = .70; for fathers, α = .75.
Measures of Enduring Vulnerabilities
Demographic information
A short form gathered descriptive information about each person including race/ethnicity, individual annual income, years of education, and age. Religiosity was measured on this form by asking, “All things considered, how religious would you say that you are?” Scores ranged from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very religious).
Family of origin
Two aspects of participants’ families of origin were explored: the level of interparental conflict and whether or not the participant’s parents divorced. Three items regarding intensity and frequency of interparental conflict were derived from Grych, Seid, and Fincham’s (1992) Children’s Perceptions of Interparental Conflict (CPIC) scale; a modified response scale was used in the present study that ranges from 1 (definitely false) to 6 (definitely true). These items have been shown to be appropriate for use with adults (Kline, Wood, & Moore, 2003) and they had high internal consistency in the present study (α = .92 for mothers and .91 for fathers). Participants also indicated whether their parents were divorced (yes or no). Family-of-origin information was collected only once during the study.
Premarital cohabitation history
Participants reported the dates that they began living together and the dates that they were married. Based on this information, participants were coded as having lived together premaritally or not.
Measures of Nature of Stressful Event
Child’s gender and birth date
Participants indicated at each assessment if they (or their partners) were pregnant or if they had given birth to a child since their last visit. The birth date and gender of each child was recorded.
Timing of pregnancy
To compute how long couples had been married before giving birth, the birth date was subtracted from couples’ wedding date; this variable was then used to investigate whether having children shortly after marriage predicted changes after birth.
Planned or unplanned pregnancy
When couples reported their child’s birth date, both partners separately reported whether the pregnancy was planned (yes or no). Individual, rather than couples’, reports were used in analyses below to permit partners to disagree about whether the pregnancy was planned.
Financial stress
Financial stress was assessed using two items: “During the past 12 months, how much difficulty have you had paying your bills?” and “Generally, at the end of each month did you end up with money left over?” (reversed) scored on a 5-point Likert scale. Internal consistency was adequate for this two-item scale (α = .73 for fathers and .77 for mothers).
Theoretical Models
Twelve potential models were fit to each individual’s data (Figure 1) to identify the best fitting model for six constructs: marital satisfaction, negative observed communication, poor conflict management, problem intensity, relationship confidence, and dedication. These models were based on Huston and Holmes’ (2004) suggestion that post-birth change could occur gradually or suddenly and be maintained or not over time.
The first three models tested here, if identified as the best-fitting model, would indicate no effect of the transition to parenthood because there were no changes at or after birth that could be attributed to the birth of the child. Specifically, Model 1a suggests there is no change in the construct either before, immediately following, or after the birth of the baby (the vertical dotted line). Model 2a suggests linear changes in the construct both before and after birth while Model 3a depicts initial steep decreases in the construct with a gradual slowing of those decreases; however, this slowing is not attributable to birth.
The remaining nine models expand upon the three base models and indicate a potential effect of the transition to parenthood on the construct. Models 1b, 2b, and 3b indicate a sudden vertical shift (either increase or decrease) in the construct in the year following the birth of the baby. Such changes would be consistent with the fairly sudden declines in relationship functioning in the year after birth observed in the previous literature. Models 1c, 2c, and 3c indicate a more gradual shift (either increase or decrease) in the construct over several years following birth that differs from the rate of change in that construct before birth. Because the effect of the transition to parenthood has typically been examined in studies of limited duration, these gradual changes have not generally been examined. Finally, Models 1d, 2d, and 3d combine models b and c to include both a sudden vertical shift in the year after birth as well as more gradual changes in the construct in the following years. Models 1d, 2d, or 3d would be indicated if the initial changes that have been repeatedly documented after birth in the previous literature gradually improve (or even further deteriorate) after birth.
Statistical Models
Multi-level analyses were conducted within the Hierarchical Linear Modeling program (HLM 6.03; Raudenbush, Bryk, Cheong, & Congdon, 2004) and run separately by gender to permit changes over time to differ by gender.2 In the multi-level approach, estimates of change over time within an individual are generated at level one; level two accounts for any systematic variability due to the nesting of repeated assessments within individuals. Prior to analysis, the time (in years) of each parent’s assessment was centered on the birth of the couple’s child so that the intercept represented the estimated mean level of the relationship construct at the time of birth. This strategy maximizes the ability to examine changes in levels and trajectories of the measured constructs before and after having a baby.
Model estimation
Models 1a, 2a, and 3a were tested using parameterizations of an intercept-only model (Model 1a), an intercept + linear time model (Model 2a) and an intercept + linear time + quadratic time (Model 3a). Models 1b, 2b, and 3b were tested by adding a single variable (“level”) to the base models that had a value of zero for all assessments that occurred before the birth of a couple’s child and a value of 1 for all the assessments that occurred after the birth. For example, Model 3b was tested by fitting Equation 1 to the data:
(1) |
Fixed effects at level one were allowed to vary randomly at level two if the difference in log likelihoods between models with and without the random effect was significant. Models 1c, 2c, 3c as well as 1d, 2d, and 3d were fit to the data using a piecewise model (e.g., Raudenbush and Bryk, 2002) that estimated a linear rate of change before birth and a separate linear rate of change after birth that captured deviations from the pre-birth rate of change.3
Model fit
To determine the best-fitting model of the eight potential models for each dependent variable, values of the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978) were calculated from the log-likelihood statistic obtained for each model using Full Maximum Likelihood estimation; these values were examined separately for each gender.4 As part of this model-fitting procedure, level two random effects were tested by comparing the deviance of models with and without random effects; these random effects were retained when a chi-square test of the difference in model deviance was significant. This process selected the model that best captured both the mean trajectory of change as well as the between-individual variability of trajectories over time, satisfying both the descriptive and predictive goals of Study 1. As a result, some of the best-fitting models include fixed effects with non-significant estimates because there was important between-individual variability in these effects. To ensure that the above process selected the best-fitting model, the model was also compared with its nested models using the deviance value of the full maximum likelihood solution; in all cases, the chi-square difference test supported the model selected by the BIC criterion as the best fit. Log-likelihood and BIC statistics for each model are available from the first author.
Testing gender differences
Once the best fitting models were identified for each gender, these models were run for both partners simultaneously following the guidelines presented by Raudenbush, Brennan, and Barnett (1995) for couple data. For example, Model 3b was fit using Equation 2:
(2) |
By constraining the estimates of both partners to be equal and testing the deviance of the constrained and unconstrained models, it was possible to test whether the estimates for fathers and mothers were significantly different from each other. When models differed by gender, all relevant parameters were included in both models to permit testing across spouses.
Prediction of change after birth
For those variables that demonstrated significant variability in post-birth changes attributable to the transition to parenthood, those changes were first correlated with the level of that variable at birth. Examination of these correlations revealed whether couples who were high or low in a variable before birth (e.g., high or low marital satisfaction) tended to show more or less change after birth.
Next, changes attributable to birth were predicted from variables hypothesized by the VSA model to be central to change in relationship functioning — enduring vulnerabilities, aspects of the stressful event (birth), and the couples’ adaptive processes. Prediction analyses followed the recommendations by Raudenbush et al. (1995) for couple data, with gender-specific predictors entered in level two. Using the Latent Variable Regression procedure in HLM 6.03, an individual’s estimated amount of change attributable to birth (e.g., βh2i or βw2i in Equation 2) was predicted simultaneously from that individual’s estimated level of the dependent variable before birth (i.e., βh0i or βw0i in Equation 2) and from the level-two predictor of interest. The value of the dependent variable before birth was included as a control variable because pre-birth levels and post-birth changes were correlated for several of the dependent variables. Because we were interested in whether the predictor added information above and beyond that contained in the dependent variable itself, the estimated level of the dependent variable at birth was entered in all prediction equations. For predictors whose values varied over time, the value of the predictor during the year preceding birth was used. To reduce the total number of analyses run, cross-partner predictions were not conducted.
Influence of Premarital Education
As the couples in the present study received differing forms of premarital education, it was important to determine whether the type of education impacted the results. To this end, we compared, in two ways, couples who received the typical premarital education through the religious organization where they were getting married with couples who received the empirically-based premarital education program (PREP). First, the sample was split into couples that did and did not receive PREP, and the best fitting models in each of the four samples was determined using the BIC procedures above. The results revealed no systematic differences based on the type of education received. In a second approach, receipt of PREP was entered in each best-fitting model as a predictor of parents’ changes attributable to the birth of their baby. In all cases, premarital education group did not significantly predict parents’ changes following birth (all ps > .25). Therefore, the results of the present study indicate that the impact of birth on relationship functioning does not depend on the type of premarital education received.
Results
Mean Change in Relationship Functioning After Birth
Descriptive data for the eight periods of interest are detailed in Table 1. The primary question addressed in our first analyses was whether there was evidence of a change in relationship constructs associated with the birth of the baby in the parent sample. For mothers, all six measures of relationship functioning indicated a potential effect of the transition to parenthood (Figure 2). However, although fathers reported significant post-birth changes in five of six domains of relationship functioning, changes in poor conflict management after birth did not differ from what was expected given changes already occurring before birth.
Table 1.
Satisfaction | Negative Observed Commun. |
Poor Conflict Manage. |
Problem Intensity |
Confidence | Dedication | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Time Period | Fathers | |||||
-3 to -4 years | 29.50 (5.71) |
2.72 (1.01) |
1.29 (0.27) |
11.53 (6.91) |
6.68 (0.48) |
6.44 (0.43) |
-2 to -3 years | 26.71 (7.60) |
2.56 (0.91) |
1.38 (0.38) |
12.19 (7.40) |
6.60 (0.57) |
6.33 (0.51) |
-1 to -2 years | 26.43 (6.28) |
2.47 (1.03) |
1.36 (0.33) |
13.37 (7.32) |
6.70 (0.41) |
6.34 (0.51) |
-0 to -1 years | 27.72 (5.84) |
2.34 (1.00) |
1.26 (0.25) |
11.90 (8.56) |
6.81 (0.31) |
6.49 (0.39) |
+0 to +1 years | 25.35 (5.46) |
2.53 (1.12) |
1.33 (0.32) |
11.90 (7.08) |
6.69 (0.41) |
6.37 (0.51) |
+1 to +2 years | 24.67 (7.68) |
2.59 (1.12) |
1.36 (0.42) |
13.77 (8.88) |
6.60 (0.63) |
6.29 (0.62) |
+2 to +3 years | 22.90 (5.91) |
2.06 (0.85) |
1.37 (0.37) |
16.17 (10.26) |
6.58 (0.63) |
6.34 (0.50) |
+3 to +4 years | 23.05 (6.82) |
2.35 (0.93) |
1.39 (0.37) |
17.06 (10.31) |
6.60 (0.48) |
6.33 (0.60) |
Time Period | Mothers | |||||
-3 to -4 years | 30.71 (5.73) |
2.65 (0.97) |
1.30 (0.27) |
11.05 (6.18) |
6.82 (0.38) |
6.51 (0.38) |
-2 to -3 years | 26.51 (8.17) |
2.57 (0.87) |
1.41 (0.39) |
13.41 (9.38) |
6.63 (0.77) |
6.43 (0.48) |
-1 to -2 years | 25.19 (7.16) |
2.40 (1.06) |
1.38 (0.31) |
13.60 (7.94) |
6.70 (0.61) |
6.42 (0.42) |
-0 to -1 years | 26.38 (6.86) |
2.41 (1.09) |
1.29 (0.28) |
11.17 (7.93) |
6.73 (0.46) |
6.44 (0.48) |
+0 to +1 years | 24.29 (7.00) |
2.58 (1.07) |
1.42 (0.35) |
15.45 (9.30) |
6.48 (0.58) |
6.40 (0.51) |
+1 to +2 years | 21.85 (6.10) |
2.57 (1.05) |
1.47 (0.39) |
17.70 (11.11) |
6.42 (0.97) |
6.23 (0.55) |
+2 to +3 years | 21.47 (2.47) |
2.20 (0.98) |
1.48 (0.37) |
17.67 (10.64) |
6.45 (0.70) |
6.21 (0.60) |
+3 to +4 years | 21.51 (2.51) |
2.36 (1.03) |
1.45 (0.36) |
17.92 (11.23) |
6.49 (0.72) |
6.23 (0.57) |
Regarding the nature of the changes that occurred, all of the relationship constructs that demonstrated effects of the transition to parenthood showed significant and sudden worsening of the relationship in either fathers or mothers (either declines in positives or increases in negatives; Table 2). Specifically, marital satisfaction showed significant drops following the transition to parenthood of 2.81 points for mothers (d = -0.71, p < .01)5 and 1.81 points for fathers (d = -0.45, p < .05).6 Following the transition to parenthood, both fathers (d = 0.61, p < .01) and mothers (d = 0.57, p < .05) showed sudden increases in negative observed communication. Additionally, mothers, but not fathers, also reported sudden increases in poor conflict management (d = 0.54, p < .001) and problem intensity (d = 0.77, p < .001) as well as sudden decreases in relationship confidence (d = -0.61, p < .05) after birth. Finally, fathers but not mothers demonstrated significant sudden declines in relationship dedication after birth (d = -0.47, p < .05).
Table 2.
Fathers |
Mothers |
|||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | SE B | SD of Level 2 Random Effect |
d |
r with level at birth1 |
B | SE B | SD of Level 2 Random Effect |
d |
r with level at birth1 |
|
Relationship Satisfaction |
||||||||||
Intercept | 26.71 | 0.54 | 3.98*** | 26.04 | 0.57 | 3.97*** | ||||
Linear Δ | -0.46*** | 0.12 | 0.41* | -0.47*** | 0.13 | 0.12 | ||||
Level change | -1.81* | 0.76 | 2.70* | -0.45 | -.21* | -2.81** | 0.85 | 3.68** | -0.71 | -.55*** |
Negative Observed Communication |
||||||||||
Intercept | 2.35 | 0.09 | 0.62*** | 2.36 | 0.09 | 0.65*** | ||||
Linear Δ | -0.15*** | 0.02 | 0.08 | -0.13*** | 0.02 | 0.10 | ||||
Level change | 0.38** | 0.14 | 0.38 | 0.61 | — | 0.37* | 0.14 | 0.42 | 0.57 | — |
Poor Conflict Management |
||||||||||
Intercept | 1.36 | 0.02 | 0.24*** | 1.36 | 0.03 | 0.26*** | ||||
Linear Δ | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.03*** | -.01 | .01 | 0.04*** | ||||
Level change | — | — | — | — | 0.14*** | 0.04 | 0.17** | 0.54 | .55*** | |
Problem Intensity | ||||||||||
Intercept | 12.73 | 0.60 | 5.77*** | 13.03 | 0.62 | 5.57*** | ||||
Linear Δ After | 0.60** | 0.20 | 0.52 | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Level change | 1.10 | 0.75 | 5.20*** | 0.19 | .03 | 4.29*** | 0.74 | 5.54* ** | 0.77 | .24** |
Confidence | ||||||||||
Intercept | 6.70 | 0.04 | 0.32*** | 6.73 | 0.04 | 0.36*** | ||||
Linear Δ Before | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.06*** | — | — | — | ||||
Linear Δ After | -0.04* | 0.02 | 0.02 | — | -0.01 | 0.03 | 0.18*** | -.13 | ||
Level change | — | — | — | — | — | -0.22* | 0.09 | 0.75*** | -0.61 | -.11 |
Dedication | ||||||||||
Intercept | 6.41 | 0.04 | 0.33*** | 6.39 | 0.04 | 0.36*** | ||||
Linear Δ | 0.02* | 0.01 | 0.02 | -0.02+ | 0.01 | 0.04** | ||||
Level change | -0.13* | 0.05 | 0.28*** | -0.47 | -.13 | -0.08 | 0.06 | 0.33** | -0.25 | -.47*** |
Correlations of post-birth change with level at birth (intercept) only when there was significant variability in both variables.
Additionally, for fathers only, two variables evidenced modifications to the pre-birth rate of change following the birth of the baby. Compared to their non-significant increases in relationship confidence before birth, fathers reported significant gradual decreases after birth (p < .05). Additionally, although fathers did not report any mean changes in problem intensity immediately following birth (in contrast to their wives), fathers reported a steady increase in problem intensity following birth (p < .01) compared to their pre-birth reports.
Gender Differences in Change in Relationship Functioning After Birth
Given that the model selection criteria suggested gender differences in the best-fitting models for half of the relationship variables examined in the present study, it was important to determine whether mothers and fathers showed significant differences in relationship functioning after birth. To this end, regression coefficients for the sudden and gradual changes were constrained to be equal across gender and differences in model deviance between the constrained and unconstrained models were tested. When the same parameters were not included in both fathers’ and mothers’ best-fitting model, the parameter being tested was included for both partners and constrained to be equal. Results revealed that, of the six relationship variables, only two showed significant gender differences in sudden gains (Table 3). Specifically, mothers, compared to fathers, showed significantly larger sudden increases in problem intensity (χ2(1) = 13.41, p < .001) and poor conflict management (χ2(1) = 4.18, p < .05). There were no significant gender differences in post-birth gradual changes.
Table 3.
Sudden Changes | Gradual Changes | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Deviance | Df | χ2(1) | Deviance | Df | χ2(1) | |
Relationship Satisfaction | ||||||
Unconstrained | 9861.43 | 22 | — | — | — | |
Constrained | 9862.15 | 21 | 0.72 | — | — | — |
Negative Observed Communication |
||||||
Unconstrained | 3172.21 | 17 | — | — | — | |
Constrained | 3172.22 | 16 | 0.01 | — | — | — |
Poor Conflict Management | ||||||
Unconstrained | 324.43 | 22 | — | — | — | |
Constrained | 328.62 | 21 | 4.18* | — | — | — |
Problem Intensity | ||||||
Unconstrained | 10819.82 | 16 | 10819.32 | 17 | ||
Constrained | 10833.23 | 15 | 13.41*** | 10822.97 | 16 | 3.65+ |
Confidence | ||||||
Unconstrained | 1795.93 | 23 | 1797.29 | 22 | ||
Constrained | 1799.17 | 22 | 3.24+ | 1797.65 | 21 | 0.36 |
Dedication | ||||||
Unconstrained | 1533.62 | 22 | — | — | — | |
Constrained | 1534.01 | 21 | 0.39 | — | — | — |
Note. When the best-fitting model for only one gender included a sudden or gradual change, these changes were added to the model for both spouses so that they could be constrained to be equal.
Between-Individual Variability in Change in Relationship Functioning After Birth
The third question posed by the present study was whether or not there was significant variability in individuals’ reactions to the transition to parenthood. Results indicated that fathers’ and mothers’ sudden changes in relationship satisfaction, problem intensity, and relationship dedication varied significantly between individuals (Table 2). In other words, there was significant variability in individual trajectories around the average trajectories for changes over time. Additionally, mothers’ sudden changes in poor conflict management and relationship confidence showed significant, between-individual variability; fathers did not show sudden changes in these variables. However, individuals did not significantly differ in the amount of sudden changes in negative observed communication. Additionally, mothers showed significant variability in gradual change in relationship confidence but not relationship dedication; fathers did not show significant variability in gradual changes in any variable. To examine this variability in more detail, Empirical Bayes (EB) estimates of fathers’ and mothers’ sudden changes were obtained from the best fitting model for each individual. Table 4 presents the percent of individuals who reported sudden changes of various sizes (according to the estimated standard deviation of the measure before birth). Results revealed that, for all variables with significant between-individual variability, some individuals reported increases while other reported decreases over the transition to parenthood. For example, although both fathers and mothers in general showed significant sudden decreases in relationship satisfaction after birth, approximately 7% of mothers and 15% of fathers reported sudden gains in relationship satisfaction over the transition to parenthood.
Table 4.
< -2 SD | -2 to -1 SD |
-1 to 0 SD |
0 to +1 SD |
+1 to +2 SD |
> +2 SD | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mothers | ||||||
Relationship Satisfaction | 0.8% | 28.0% | 64.4% | 6.8% | 0.0% | 0.0% |
Observed Negative Communication |
0.0% | 0.0% | 0.0% | 100.0 | 0.0% | 0.0% |
Poor conflict management |
0.0% | 0.8% | 3.0% | 90.9% | 5.3% | 0.0% |
Problem intensity | 0.0% | 0.8% | 9.0% | 54.6% | 28.8% | 6.8% |
Confidence | 9.8% | 21.2% | 36.4% | 40.1% | 1.5% | 0.8% |
Dedication | 0.8% | 6.1% | 64.9% | 27.4% | 0.8% | 0.0% |
Fathers | ||||||
Relationship Satisfaction | 0.8% | 9.1% | 75.6% | 14.5% | 0.0% | 0.0% |
Observed Negative Communication |
0.0% | 0.0% | 0.0% | 100.0% | 0.0% | 0.0% |
Poor conflict management |
— | — | — | — | — | — |
Problem intensity | 0.0% | 3.1% | 43.5% | 44.3% | 10.7% | 1.5% |
Confidence | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Dedication | 0.8% | 10.7% | 70.9% | 15.3% | 2.3% | 0.0% |
Prediction of Change in Relationship Functioning After Birth
Given the variability of change in relationship functioning after birth, predictors of this variability were explored as a fourth question in this study. Constructs that did not demonstrate significant variability in sudden or gradual changes after birth were not examined further.
Prediction from level of variable at birth
To investigate whether couples’ level of relationship functioning at birth was related to changes in that same variable after birth, correlations between the intercept and post-birth changes (in the tau matrix) were examined. The level of relationship satisfaction at birth was significantly negatively correlated with sudden changes in relationship satisfaction for fathers (r = -.21, p < .05) and mothers (r = -.55, p < .001), indicating that couples who were more satisfied at birth showed larger decreases in relationship satisfaction after birth. Similar patterns were seen for dedication for mothers (r = -.47, p < .001). However, for mothers but not fathers, the opposite relation was observed for relationship conflict. Specifically, for mothers, higher levels of pre-birth poor conflict management and problem intensity were significantly associated with post-birth increases in poor conflict management (r = .55, p < .001) and intensity (r = .24; p < .01), respectively (Table 3)7.
Prediction from enduring vulnerabilities
After controlling for levels of relationship functioning at birth, several types of enduring vulnerabilities were entered at level two as predictors of variability in post-birth functioning. Family of origin variables did not significantly predict changes after birth for fathers. However, for mothers, greater declines in relationship satisfaction were predicted by parental divorce (b = -3.28; t(112) = -2.30; p < .05) as well as higher levels of parental conflict (b = -0.82; t(112) = -1.99; p < .05).
Fathers who cohabited before marriage, compared to those who did not, reported significantly larger sudden decreases in relationship dedication (b = -0.31; t(90) = -2.15; p < .05) and larger increases in observed negative communication (b = 0.71; t(84) = 2.19; p < .05) after birth. Similarly, mothers who cohabited before marriage reported higher observed negative communication after birth (b = 0.65; t(84) = 2.03; p < .05). Neither ethnicity nor level of religiosity were significantly predictive of changes in relationship functioning after birth.
Prediction from nature of stressful event
Characteristics of the birth also significantly predicted post-birth changes in relationship functioning after controlling for levels of functioning at birth. Mothers who gave birth to girls, compared to those who gave birth to boys, experienced significantly larger drops in marital satisfaction after birth (b = -2.87; t(101) = -1.99; p < .05). Moreover, being married longer before birth predicted significantly smaller decreases in marital satisfaction for fathers (b = 0.58; t(124) = 2.24; p < .05) but not mothers. Results revealed that whether or not the pregnancy was planned was not significantly related to changes in marital satisfaction after birth.
Higher income level at birth was predictive of significantly smaller drops in relationship satisfaction (b = 0.73; t(101) = 2.36; p < .05) for fathers but not mothers. Additionally, higher income level was predictive of smaller increases in problem intensity for mothers (b = -0.73; t(101) = -2.12; p < .05). In contrast, self-reported financial stress before birth was not predictive of any relationship changes after birth.
Prediction from relationship adaptive processes
Finally, levels of relationship adaptive processes at birth were used to predict post-birth changes in other domains of relationship functioning (after controlling for pre-birth levels in the dependent variable). Results indicated that higher levels of relationship confidence at birth predicted significantly larger increases in problem intensity following birth for both fathers (b = 5.66; t(110) = 2.19; p < .05) and mothers (b = 5.30; t(110) = 2.83; p < .01). Additionally, mothers’ higher levels of relationship confidence predicted significantly larger sudden increases in poor conflict management after birth (b = 0.20; t(110) = 2.22; p < .05). In contrast, higher levels of relationship confidence predicted smaller decreases in relationship satisfaction for fathers after birth (b = 4.19; t(110) = 2.21; p < .05).
Additionally, higher levels of reported poor conflict management before birth predicted larger increases in problem intensity after birth for mothers (b = 7.10; t(110) = 2.59; p < .05) and fathers (b = 8.93; t(110) = 2.80; p < .01). Mothers’ observed negative communication also predicted significantly larger increases in their reported poor conflict management (b = 0.06; t(110) = 2.05; p < .05).
Discussion
Of the six relationship constructs examined in the present study, the majority demonstrated patterns of change consistent with an immediate or delayed impact of the transition to parenthood. For mothers, five of six areas of relationship functioning showed sudden deteriorations from their trajectories of relationship functioning before birth. That is, relationship satisfaction, self-reported problem intensity and poor conflict management, observed negative communication, and relationship confidence all showed sudden deteriorations. Fathers showed significant sudden deterioration in marital satisfaction, dedication, and negative observed communication; additionally, fathers showed significant gradual increases in problem intensity. When tested directly, mothers reported significantly larger, sudden increases than fathers in problem intensity and poor conflict management. For the average couple, no aspect of relationship functioning showed a positive effect of the transition to parenthood.
In addition to mean changes in relationship functioning after birth, there was also significant between-individual variability in many of the variables examined. Variability in changes in some aspects of relationship functioning after birth was related to the levels at birth. For both genders, higher levels of marital satisfaction were related to greater decreases in these variables after birth; additionally, for mothers, higher levels of relationship dedication before birth were related to greater decreases in dedication after birth. However, for mothers, higher levels of relationship conflict (poor management and severity) at birth were related to greater increases in conflict after birth. Additionally, post-birth changes were predicted from several other variables. In particular, the most consistent risk factors for post-birth declines included high levels of observed negative communication, difficulties in the mother’s family of origin, shorter duration of the marriage at birth, and giving birth to a girl. However, it is important to note that there were numerous non-significant predictions in the present study, consistent with the existing literature on change over the transition to parenthood. Possible reasons for the generally poor predictive ability are explored in more detail in the general discussion.
Study 2: Non-Parent Sample
Before the results in the parent sample can be confidently attributed to the transition to parenthood, it is important to rule out common alternative explanations to which the ITS design used in Study 1 is particularly susceptible (Shadish et al., 2002). The primary threat to interpretation is the potential effect of history — that another variable or event acted at the time of birth to produce the effect that we attributed to parenthood. A second alternative explanation is instrumentation — a change in how data are collected, kept, or scored. For example, a systematic data entry or scoring error occurring for the assessment approximately four years into the study could be masquerading as the effect of birth in Study 1. In Study 2, a sample of non-parents is used to examine whether threats to internal validity (i.e., history and instrumentation) can explain the changes after birth observed in the parent sample. If similar changes are not observed, we can be more confident that the sudden and gradual changes in relationship functioning observed in Study 1 can be attributed to the transition to parenthood. On the other hand, if similar changes are observed, then one of those alternative possibilities becomes the most likely explanation.
Method
Procedure
Participants in Study 2 participated in the same larger study as the participants in Study 1 and followed the same procedure described above. As with the parent sample, the type of premarital education received did not affect the selection of the best-fitting model.
Participants
In Study 2, data from 86 couples who did not have children before or during the first eight years of the study (“non-parents”) were used as a non-equivalent control group. When they began participation, non-parents were 26.34 years old (SD = 4.91), on average, with 15.08 years of education (SD = 1.77). The median personal income level was $20,000-29,999 and they were 90.1% White, 5.2% Latino, and 2.3% African American; 2.4% were of some other race/ethnicity. Seventy five percent of non-parents cohabited before marriage. Twenty seven percent of couples reported attending church at least once a week and, on average, couples described themselves as “somewhat” religious. During the course of the present study, 18 couples divorced and 22 withdrew from participation. This divorce rate is consistent with previous longitudinal research of non-parents that begins just after marriage (e.g., Shapiro et al., 2000). All available data from the couples that divorced and withdrew were included in the analyses below, allowing for estimates of change to be fit for the majority of these couples who did not provide full data.
As expected based on previous studies, there were a few differences between the parents in Study 1 and the non-parents in Study 2 at the initial assessment. Parents were more confident about their relationships (F(1, 208) = 4.93, p < .05) and had more years of education (F(1, 216) = 6.93, p < .01) than non-parents. There were no significant differences on any other relationship or demographic variables. Parents and non-parents did not significantly differ in terms of what type of premarital education they received, χ2(2, N = 217) = 2.53, p > .25.
Measures
The same measures described in Study 1 were used in Study 2.
Data Analysis
To determine if non-parents experienced similar to changes to parents at the same point in their relationships, the time of each non-parent’s assessment was centered around mean time elapsed from the first assessment to birth for the parent sample utilized in study 1 (3.51 years). This centering allowed us to examine whether relationship events other than birth of child (i.e., history or instrumentation effects) were viable explanations for the pattern of change in relationship functioning observed in parents. Other than this difference in centering, the same analytical and model-fitting procedure used in Study 1 was applied in Study 2.
Results
Change Within the Non-Parent Sample
When the twelve models in Figure 1 were fit to the non-parent sample, all of the relationship constructs showed only linear or linear and quadratic change; there was no evidence of change in intercepts or slopes at the time when parents were showing such changes for these relationship variables (Table 5). As presented in Table 5, marital satisfaction showed significant linear declines over time (p < .001). Men and women also demonstrated significant decreases in negative observed communication over the course of the study (all p < .001). Men, but not women, also reported a significant linear decline in relationship dedication (p < .01). In contrast, neither men nor women demonstrated significant changes during the course of the study in either problem intensity or poor conflict management.
Table 5.
Men |
Women |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | SE B | SD of Level 2 Random Effect |
B | SE B | SD of Level 2 Random Effect |
|
Satisfaction | ||||||
Intercept | 24.15 | 0.71 | 3.88*** | 22.49 | 1.00 | 5.34*** |
Linear Δ | -0.94*** | 0.19 | 0.40* | -0.79*** | 0.19 | 0.73 |
Quadratic Δ | — | — | 0.15* | 0.07 | 0.24 | |
Negative Observed Communication |
||||||
Intercept | 2.55 | 0.08 | 0.41* | 2.56 | 0.12 | 0.58** |
Linear Δ | -0.18*** | 0.03 | 0.20* | -0.15*** | 0.03 | 0.07+ |
Poor Conflict Management |
||||||
Intercept | 1.50 | 0.05 | 0.33*** | 1.48 | 0.05 | 0.34*** |
Linear Δ | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.04*** | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.03** |
Problem Intensity | ||||||
Intercept | 15.74 | 1.20 | 8.56*** | 15.75 | 1.07 | 7.46*** |
Linear Δ | 0.50+ | 0.27 | 0.88** | 0.37 | 0.24 | 0.65** |
Confidence | ||||||
Intercept | 6.37 | 0.18 | 1.06*** | 6.25 | 0.17 | 1.00*** |
Linear Δ | -0.03 | 0.02 | 0.09 | -0.31+ | 0.12 | 0.08 |
Quadratic Δ | 0.02+ | 0.01 | 0.06 | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.06 |
Dedication | ||||||
Intercept | 6.11 | 0.07 | 0.49*** | 6.18 | 0.12 | 0.82*** |
Linear Δ | -0.05** | 0.02 | 0.06*** | -0.02 | 0.01 | 0.02 |
Quadratic Δ | — | — | — | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.06*** |
Discussion
In contrast to the results for parents in Study 1, non-parents in Study 2 showed no evidence of sudden changes. Over the first eight years of marriage, couples generally evidenced deterioration in relationship satisfaction, consistent with other longitudinal studies over the early years of marriage (e.g., Huston, Caughlin, Houts, Smith, & George, 2001; Johnson et al., 2005). Men, but not women, also reported significant decreases in relationship dedication over time. In contrast, self-reported problem intensity and poor conflict management did not significantly change for non-parents over time; additionally, negative observed communication significantly improved over the course of the study.
Overall, the results of Study 2 suggest that the most serious threats to the validity of an ITS design cannot explain the post-birth changes seen in Study 1. First, the most serious potential confound — history — was ruled out because none of the changes attributable to birth in the parent sample were evidenced in Study 2, reducing the possibility that these changes in levels or slopes are attributable to a common event that occurred around the same time as birth. The second threat to validity explored in Study 2 was instrumentation. Results indicated that non-parents, who completed the same measures and procedures as parents, did not show changes similar to those in Study 1 that we have attributed to birth. Therefore, instrumentation is an unlikely explanation of the results in the parent sample. In addition to history and instrumentation, Shadish and colleagues (2002) identify a third threat to ITS designs — selection. Selection effects could potentially explain the results observed in Study 1 if different subsamples of couples completed measures before and after the birth of the baby. However, examinations of the data from both Study 1 and Study 2 did not reveal differential study dropout immediately before or following these time points. Therefore, based on these results, the most likely cause of the sudden deterioration in the parent sample remains the birth of the first baby.
General Discussion
Sudden and Gradual Changes after Birth
The primary aim of the present study was to determine the effect of the birth of the first child on marital functioning by using an ITS design. This approach addressed several of the most pressing concerns about the previous literature and was able to: 1) provide an estimate of change in relationship functioning attributable to birth by separating change after birth from change that was occurring in the couple before birth; 2) determine whether couples generally recover from the effects of birth or if instead these effects are stable or even worsen with time; 3) examine gender differences in the effect of the transition to parenthood; and 4) compare parents and non-parents in appropriate ways.
Across positive and negative aspects of the relationship and across both self-report and observed variables, the birth of the first child has a small to medium negative effect on both fathers’ and mothers’ relationship functioning. The magnitude of this deterioration was smaller than many previous studies of the transition to parenthood (e.g., Cowan & Cowan, 1995, 2000; Gottman et al., 2002) but consistent with others (e.g., Kurdek, 1995; Simpson et al., 2003). The smaller effects of birth revealed in the present study support criticisms of previous studies of the transition to parenthood. Specifically, as noted by Houston and Holmes (2004), previous studies may have inadvertently captured a “honeymoon” period for the couples during pregnancy and confounded typical relationship deterioration occurring even before birth with problems attributable to the transition to parenthood.
The present study also answered previous calls (e.g., Huston & Vangelisti, 1995) to investigate the stability of the effect of the transition to parenthood. Results indicated that the negative effects on relationship functioning tended to persist through at least the first four years after birth. Indeed, there was not a single variable that showed evidence of even non-significant recovery after birth; parenthood tended to be experienced as sudden changes that persisted over time. In contrast to the general pattern of sudden and persistent change, fathers’ relationship confidence and problem intensity continued to deteriorate in the years following birth.
Generally, mothers and fathers evidenced statistically similar patterns in post-birth relationship functioning, though there were two areas in which mothers reported significantly larger sudden declines than fathers. Consistent with previous research (e.g., Belsky & Hsieh, 1998; Grote & Clark, 2001), mothers reported significantly larger sudden increases in poor conflict management and problem intensity than fathers, indicating that they may be more sensitive to the impact of having a first baby on these negative aspects of relationship functioning than their husbands are. In other areas of relationship functioning, however, the patterns tended to be similar.
Finally, the statistical approach in the present study also provided novel information about the similarities and differences between parents and non-parents in changes in relationship functioning over the first eight years of marriage. Had we instead fit only linear and quadratic slopes over time (omitting the possibility of sudden or gradual changes occurring at birth) and compared the amount of change between parents and non-parents, we would have concluded, along with previous studies (e.g., MacDermid et al., 1990; McHale & Huston, 1985), that parents and non-parents showed similar types of change over the course of the first few years of marriage. Instead, the results of the present study suggest that parents and non-parents generally show similar amounts of decline in overall relationship functioning over the first eight years of marriage, but that these changes tend to occur suddenly following the birth of the baby for parents and more gradually over time for non-parents. Additionally, parents showed clear increases in negativity, conflict, and problem intensity following the birth of a child whereas non-parents did not show such changes at the same point in time nor did they show such declines over time more generally. Given the sudden nature of the negative relationship changes following birth for parents, the most likely explanation is that they are caused by stressors encountered during the transition to parenthood that are not handled well.
Variability and Prediction of Change after Birth
In addition to examining change over the transition to parenthood for the average couple, we were interested in exploring variability in couples’ adjustment to birth. In the present study, although the average individual’s relationship functioning deteriorated, some individuals reported improvements over the transition to parenthood, consistent with previous studies (e.g., Belsky & Hsieh, 1998; Belsky & Rovine, 1990). The variability in individuals’ reactions and adjustments to childbirth is important because it suggests avenues for future research on risk and resiliency during this typically stressful time. To understand why some couples deteriorate while others improve after birth, Karney and Bradbury’s (1995) VSA model is especially useful. Consistent with that model, the present study focused on three constructs that are understood to impact changes in relationship satisfaction over time: 1) enduring vulnerabilities; 2) the nature of the stressful event (birth), and 3) the couples’ quality of adaptive processes.
First, several types of enduring vulnerabilities predicted relationship deterioration after birth. For mothers, a history of parental divorce or conflict was predictive of larger decreases in relationship satisfaction after birth. In previous studies, relationship functioning in the family of origin, particularly in that of the female partner, has been found to impact couples’ current relationship functioning (e.g., D’Onofrio et al., 2007; Sanders, Halford, & Behrens, 1999; Story et al., 2004). Additionally, in the present study, both fathers and mothers who cohabited premaritally had more difficulty over the transition to parenthood than couples who had not cohabited before marriage. This difference is perhaps due to a combination of prior risk factors and the nature of the way their relationships initially formed (Stanley, Rhoades, & Markman, 2006). The increased risk of cohabiting couples is consistent with previous research suggesting that those who cohabit before marriage (e.g., Kamp Dush, Cohan, & Amato, 2003; Stanley et al., 2004) are more at risk for marital problems and divorce.
Second, several characteristics of the stressful event (birth) predicted changes in couples’ relationship functioning after birth. Specifically, fathers who had a child more quickly following marriage evidenced more declines in relationship satisfaction after birth than parents who waited longer after getting married. This finding is consistent with some (e.g., Belsky & Rovine, 1990) but not all (O’Brien & Peyton, 2002) research. It may be that couples that have been married longer have more time to develop a shared understanding of relationship responsibilities and goals that help to buffer them from the stressors of increased childcare and general disorganization after birth. Interestingly, whether the baby was planned was generally unrelated to post-birth functioning in this sample. In interpreting this finding, it should be noted that it was not possible in the present study to differentiate couples who did not plan to get pregnant but were happy about it from couples who had an unplanned and undesired pregnancy. In previous research, the post-birth relationship outcomes for these two types of couples have been shown to be very different (Cowan & Cowan, 2000); therefore, combining these groups in the present study may cause the overall prediction to be non-significant.
Additionally, the gender of the baby affected parents’ relationship functioning after birth, with female children leading to larger decreases in mothers’ relationship satisfaction and larger increases in fathers’ reports of problem intensity. These findings are consistent with previous studies that have shown male children are associated with lower rates of divorce and higher marital satisfaction (see Raley & Bianchi, 2006), possibly because fathers of girls are less active in childcare than fathers of boys. Pregnant couples may need specific help in understanding and communicating their expectations about the gender of their babies and childcare so that they can stop declines in relationship functioning from occurring. Finally, consistent with previous research (e.g., Belsky & Rovine, 1990), lower individual incomes (but not more financial stress) tended to predict more deterioration in fathers’ relationship functioning after birth. Therefore, it appears that the additional resources, supports, and alternative sources of self-esteem afforded by a higher income, rather than reduced financial stress, serves to buffer the relationship from declines in relationship quality. In future studies, it will be important to identify these protective resources and supports.
Third, several aspects of couples’ adaptive processes before birth predicted declines in relationship functioning after birth. Consistent with previous research (e.g., Cox et al., 1999; Crohan, 1996; Kluwer & Johnson, 2007), individuals who reported more problems with poor conflict management and problem intensity before birth showed significantly larger increases in these problems after birth. Additionally, more observed negative communication in mothers before birth predicted their reports of increases in post-birth poor conflict management, also consistent with previous research (e.g., Shapiro et al., 2000). Thus, as in previous studies, high levels of negative communication before birth placed individuals at risk for greater post-birth increases in problematic communication.
Interestingly, the present results also suggest that individuals with high pre-birth levels of positive relationship functioning were at risk for more post-birth deterioration in the positive aspects of their relationships. Specifically, the sudden declines in fathers’ and mothers’ relationship satisfaction as well as mothers’ dedication were larger when individuals showed higher levels of these constructs before birth; these findings are consistent with some (Belsky & Rovine, 1990) but not all (O’Brien & Peyton, 2002) previous literature. This pattern could indicate that couples who have the highest romantic connections find the transition to the tasks of parenting the most challenging, at least to the positive aspects of their relationships.
Additionally, reporting more relationship confidence before birth was associated with greater increases in negative relationship constructs (problem intensity and poor conflict management) after birth. Relationship confidence has not been previously examined over the transition to parenthood, but the risks of having unrealistically high relationship expectations have been demonstrated in newlywed couples, especially those with poor communication (McNulty & Karney, 2004). Taken together, these results suggest that the impact of the transition to parenthood may be especially potent when couples are at the extremes of both positive and negative relationship constructs. However, it should be noted that regression to the mean could also explain the associations of the positive (but not negative) relationship behaviors with changes after birth. Therefore, these results should be interpreted with caution until they are replicated in future research.
Despite the predictive ability of these variables, we should note that many other variables were not predictive of change in relationship functioning after birth. Given that the present study defines the “effect” of birth differently than previous studies, we wanted to include many of the predictors that have been used in previous studies. Many to most of these predictors had received equivocal support in previous research; the results in the present study were similar. There are likely several reasons for the difficulty in differentiating couples who will and will not have relationship difficulties after the transition to parenthood; we focus on three here. First, predicting change after birth requires predicting the development of some important aspects of the relationship that simply do not exist before birth. For example, the most commonly reported conflict after birth is division of childcare (e.g., Cowan & Cowan, 2000), something that couples have not previously dealt with. A second reason for the difficulty in predicting change after birth is that factors that cannot be known before birth may be some of the strongest determinants of change in relationship functioning over the transition to parenthood (e.g., child temperament; Kochanska, Friesenborg, Lange, & Martel, 2004). Third, in the present study, we purposely tested a restrictive definition of a predictor. Consistent with our desire to separate change in the relationship attributable to changes before birth from changes attributable solely to the transition to parenthood, we tested whether variables would be able to predict changes following birth after controlling for the couples’ functioning at the time of birth. In other words, the only predictors identified in the present study were those that had an additional effect on relationship functioning following birth above and beyond the effect they had already had on relationship functioning up to that point. Future research may wish to expand the list of predictors.
Limitations and Future Directions
The results of the present study should be considered in the context of its limitations. Most importantly, generalizability may be somewhat limited to due to several factors. While the sample is reasonably representative of the metro area from which it was drawn, and much effort was expended not to obtain a convenience sample (Stanley et al., 2001), participants were mainly well educated and Caucasian. Additionally, all couples received some form of premarital education and were married within a religious organization. The potential impact of these factors on generalizability is tempered for three reasons. First, analyses demonstrated that the type of education received did not systematically affect the shape of change in the relationship variables; furthermore, type of intervention did not significantly predict the amount of change following the birth of the baby. Second, because of the way participants were recruited, the selection effects are expected to be minimal. Each participating couple was required to receive premarital education through the religious organization that would perform their wedding (as a condition of being married there); therefore, this was not a sample that purposely sought out premarital education. Furthermore, over 75% (Johnson et al., 2002; Stanley, Amato, Johnson, & Markman, 2006; Sullivan & Bradbury, 1997) of couples that receive premarital education do so through a religious organization. Third, although all couples in the present study were married by an official associated with a religious organization, this is true for most couples in the United States (Stanley et al., 2006). Additionally, the average couple described themselves as only “somewhat religious” and only 27 percent of couples attend church once a week or more. Nevertheless, it could be that the findings here most specifically generalize to those couples who seek to marry in a religious organization.
A second limitation of the present study was that we were unable to separate the impact of the first child on changes in relationship functioning from the impact of later children. Assuming that later children have a negative impact on relationship functioning, this impact may be reflected in the gradual changes (and, to a lesser extent, sudden changes) seen after the birth of the first child. Future studies with more frequent assessments could address the issue of multiple children more directly.
We should also note that the larger study was originally designed to track changes in relationship functioning over time, rather than the impact of the transition to parenthood. While these types of designs have some advantages over studies that specifically target future parents (for a review of these advantages, see Huston & Holmes, 2004), it meant that the timing of the assessment before birth varied between couples. For some couples, the final pre-birth assessment (from which the values of the time-varying predictors were obtained) occurred before the couple was pregnant while it occurred in the third trimester for other couples. As a result, additional variability was added to the predictor, reducing power to find an effect of those predictors. In future studies, it would be useful to combine an ITS design with measures that are more specific to changes and challenges faced by couples over the transition to parenthood. For example, it may be that that relationship constructs other than those measured in the present study (e.g., prenatal expectations; Lawrence, Nylen, & Cobb, 2007) may be more predictive of relationship change after the birth of the baby. Additionally, to expand our understanding of the transition to parenthood, examination of the role the couple relationship plays in the developing coparenting relationship is critical. The data set used here contains an unusually broad assessment of relationship constructs, allowing for tests of various relationship effects over time. However, since child variables such as temperament are likely to have important impacts on relationship functioning after birth (Kochanska et al., 2004), our ability to understand this process would be enhanced by inclusion of these variables in future studies. Finally, the internal reliability of some of our measures (e.g., dedication) was low at some of the initial assessment points, likely due to a restriction of range in newlywed couples.
Conclusion
In sum, using an extensive database of 218 couples spanning the first eight years of marriage, the present study answers a long-standing and central question in the field; this investigation demonstrated that the transition to parenthood has a significant impact on marital functioning. For the average couple, these effects were negative, small to medium in magnitude, and consistent across a number of relationship domains. Moreover, these effects tended to be sudden and persist over time. Additionally, results revealed significant variability in changes after birth. This variability was systematically related to a number of factors associated with the individual, the marriage, and characteristics of the birth itself.
Acknowledgments
Support for preparation of this article was provided in part by a grants from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development to the first author (R03 HD051695) and the third and fourth authors (5R01HD047564 & R01HD053314). Data collection was made possible through grants to the third and fourth authors from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01 HD053314-20A1) and the National Institute of Mental Health, Division of Services and Intervention Research, Adult and Geriatric Treatment and Prevention Branch (5-RO1-MH35525-12). The contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NIH. We wish to thank the couples who participated in this research for their dedication to this project.
Footnotes
Previous studies have indirectly examined the shape of change in relationship variables beginning in either pregnancy or shortly following birth as part of examining assumptions of their statistical tests (e.g., O’Brien & Peyton, 2002; Schulz, Cowan, & Cowan, 2006). Additionally, a recent study (Lawrence, Nylen, & Cobb, 2007) fit separate slopes of change before and after birth. However, no studies have examined the statistical fit of various models of change in relationship functioning before and after birth; it is this addition that is the central contribution of the present study.
Running spouses separately was necessary because the model fitting procedure, described next, used the deviance of the entire model which would capture fit of the model to both fathers’ and mothers’ data if run together. Once models were selected, spouses were run simultaneously which permitted direct comparison of spouses’ data.
Typically, piecewise models are used to estimate two separate rates of change. However, given that our interest was in modifications to pre-existing rates of change, an alternate parameterization presented by Bryk and Raudenbush (2002) was used. To obtain an estimate of the slope after birth, the two estimates of time can be summed.
BIC values, rather than the log-likelihood values, were used to select the best-fitting models because not all of the models were nested within the same statistical model. BIC values (Schwarz, 1978) adjust the log-likelihood the account for both the number of parameters and the sample size. When comparing models, Raftery (1995) suggest that BIC differences of 2-6 to be “positive” evidence, 6-10 to be “strong”, and over 10 to be “very strong”.
In all cases, Cohen’s d was computed by dividing the estimate of the sudden change by the standard deviation of the intercept (the estimated level of the construct at birth) obtained from the HLM program.
The full results for the MAT are not reported here to save space. However, the one-item measure of satisfaction and the full MAT were highly correlated (r = .82 for fathers and r = .57 for mothers) and showed similar changes over time. Specifically, the full MAT demonstrated a sudden drop of 6.31 points for mothers (d = -.66, p < .001) and 5.63 points for fathers (d = -.56, p < .01) following the birth of the first baby. Full results are available from the first author.
To further understand these relations, we conducted three post-hoc analyses. First, individual’ scores in the year preceding and following birth were correlated with each other; in all cases, these scores were significantly positively correlated. Thus, the rank order of individuals after birth remains similar for measures of both positive and negative relationship functioning. Second, we divided individuals into three groups based on their scores in the year preceding birth: those scoring 1 standard deviation or more above the mean, within 1 standard deviation of the mean, and 1 standard deviation or more below the mean. For all three measures of positive relationship functioning (fathers’ and mothers’ relationship satisfaction and mothers’ dedication), plots revealed that it was the high-functioning individuals who declined most after birth (rather than the lowest-functioning individuals declining the least). However, for two measures of negative relationship functioning (mothers’ reports of poor conflict management and problem intensity), the opposite pattern held; specifically, the lowest-functioning group showed the largest post-birth increases in negative relationship functioning. Finally, we examined the possibility that the complex modeling procedures used in the present study were artificially creating these relations. To equate our analyses with those of previous studies, we eliminated data from all assessments that occurred before pregnancy and fit an intercept and linear slope to the pregnancy and post-birth data. In all cases, the signs of the correlations between the intercepts and post-birth change were consistent with the analyses presented above (i.e., higher levels of both positive and negative relationship functioning before birth predicted greater relationship deterioration after birth).
Publisher's Disclaimer: The following manuscript is the final accepted manuscript. It has not been subjected to the final copyediting, fact-checking, and proofreading required for formal publication. It is not the definitive, publisher-authenticated version. The American Psychological Association and its Council of Editors disclaim any responsibility or liabilities for errors or omissions of this manuscript version, any version derived from this manuscript by NIH, or other third parties. The published version is available at http://www.apa.org/journals/psp/.
References
- Adams JM, Jones WH. The conceptualization of marital commitment: An integrative analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;72(5):1177–1196. [Google Scholar]
- Amato PR. Explaining the intergenerational transmission of divorce. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1996;58:628–640. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amato PR. Children of divorce in the 1990s: An update of the Amato and Keith (1991) meta-analysis. Journal of Family Psychology. 2001;15:355–370. doi: 10.1037//0893-3200.15.3.355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Hsieh K. Patterns of marital change during the early childhood years: Parent personality, coparenting, and division-of-labor correlates. Journal of Family Psychology. 1998;12:511–528. [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Lang M, Huston TL. Sex typing and division of labor as determinants of marital change across the transition to parenthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;50:517–522. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.50.3.517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Rovine M. Patterns of marital change across the transition to parenthood: Pregnancy to three years postpartum. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1990;52:5–19. [Google Scholar]
- Biglan A, Metzler CW, Ary DV. Increasing the prevalence of successful children: The case for community intervention research. Behavior Analyst. 1994;17:335–351. doi: 10.1007/BF03392680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bramlett MD, Mosher WD. Advance data from vital and health statistics; no. 323. National Center for Health Statistics; Hyattsville, MD: 2001. First marriage dissolution, divorce, and remarriage: United States. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciarlo JA, Shern DL, Tweed DL, Kirkpatrick LA. II: The Colorado Social Health Survey of Mental Health Service Needs: Sampling, instrumentation, and major findings. Evaluation and Program Planning. 1992;15(2):133–147. [Google Scholar]
- Clements M, Markman HJ. The transition to parenthood: Is having children hazardous to marriage? In: Vanzetti N, Duck S, editors. A lifetime of relationships. Brooks/Cole; Pacific Grove, CA: 1996. pp. 290–310. [Google Scholar]
- Clulow CF. To have and to hold: Marriage, the first baby and parenting during the transition to parenthood. Aberdeen University Press; Aberdeen, Scotland: 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Cohan CL, Kleinbaum S. Toward a greater understanding of the cohabitation effect: Premarital cohabitation and marital communication. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2002;64:180–192. [Google Scholar]
- Collins NL, Dunkel-Schetter C, Lobel M, Scrinshaw SCM. Social support in pregnancy: Psychosocial correlates of birth outcomes and postpartum depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1993;65:1243–1258. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.65.6.1243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan CP, Cowan PA. Interventions to ease the transition to parenthood: Why they are needed and what they can do. Family Relations: Journal of Applied Family & Child Studies. 1995;44:412–423. [Google Scholar]
- Cowan CP, Cowan PA. When partners become parents: The big life change for couples. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, NJ, US: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Cox MJ, Owen MT, Lewis JM, Henderson VK. Marriage, adult adjustment, and early parenting. Child Development. 1989;60:1015–1024. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1989.tb03532.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crane DR, Allgood SM, Larson JH, Griffin W. Assessing marital quality with distressed and nondistressed couples: A comparison and equivalency table for three frequently used measures. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1990;52:87–93. [Google Scholar]
- Crowan SE. Marital quality and conflict across the transition to parenthood in African American and white couples. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1996;58:933–944. [Google Scholar]
- Cummings EM, Davies PT. Children and marital conflict: The impact of family dispute and resolution. Guilford Press; New York: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, Harden KP, Slutske WS, Heath AC, et al. A genetically informed study of the intergenerational transmission of marital instability. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2007;69:793–809. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00406.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Wolff MS, Van Ijzdendoorn MH. Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development. 1997;68:571–591. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dion MR, Avellar SA, Zaveri HH, Hershey AM. Implementing healthy marriage programs for unmarried couples with children: Early lessons from the Building Strong Families Project. Mathematica Policy Research, Inc; Washington, DC: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Feeney JA, Hohaus L, Noller P, Alexander RP. Becoming parents: Exploring the bonds between mothers, fathers, and their infants. Cambridge University Press; New York: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Fincham FD, Bradbury TN. The assessment of marital quality: A reevaluation. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1987;49:797–809. [Google Scholar]
- Funk JL, Rogge RD. Testing the ruler with item response theory: Increasing precision of measurement for relationship satisfaction with the couples satisfaction index. Journal of Family Psychology. 2007;21:572–583. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.4.572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg AE, Sayer A. Lesbian couples’ relationship quality across the transition to parenthood. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2006;68:87–100. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg LR. An alternative “description of personality”: The Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;59:1216–1229. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.59.6.1216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg LR. The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure. Psychological Assessment. 1992;4:26–42. [Google Scholar]
- Gottman JM, Driver J, Tabares A. Building the sound marital house: An empirically-derived couple therapy. In: Gurman AS, Jacobson NS, editors. Clinical Handbook of Couple Therapy. Third Edition Guilford Press; New York: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Grote NK, Clark MS. Perceiving unfairness in the family: Cause or consequence of marital distress ? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80:281–293. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grych JH, Seid M, Fincham FD. Assessing marital conflict from the child’'s perspective: The Children’s Perception of Interparental Conflict Scale. Child Development. 1992;63(3):558–571. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01646.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horwitz SM, Irwin JR, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Heenan JMB, Mendoza J, Carter AS. Language delay in a community cohort of young children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:932–940. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046889.27264.5E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huston TL, Caughlin JP, Houts RM, Smith SE, George LJ. The connubial crucible: Newlywed years as predictors of marital delight, distress, and divorce. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80:237–252. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huston TL, Holmes EK. Becoming parents. In: Vangelisti A, editor. Handbook of Family Communication. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, NJ: 2004. pp. 105–133. [Google Scholar]
- Huston TL, Vangelisti AL. How parenthood affects marriage. In: Fitzpatrick MA, Vangelisti AL, editors. Explaining Family Interactions. Sage Publications; Thousand Oaks, CA: 1995. pp. 147–176. [Google Scholar]
- Impett EA, Beals KP, Peplau LA. Testing the investment model of relationship commitment and stability in a longitudinal study of married couples. Current Psychology. 2001;20(4):312–326. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson CA, Stanley SM, Glenn ND, Amato PR, Nock SL, Author HJ, et al. Marriage in Oklahoma: 2001 baseline statewide survey on marriage and divorce (S02096 OKDHS) Oklahoma Department of Human Services; Oklahoma City, OK: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Julien D, Markman HJ, Lindahl KM. A comparison of a global and a microanalytic coding system: Implications for future trends in studying interactions. Behavioral Assessment. 1989;11:81–100. [Google Scholar]
- Dush C. M. Kamp, Cohan CL, Amato PR. The relationship between cohabitation and marital quality and stability: Changes across cohorts? Journal of Marriage and Family. 2003;65:539–549. [Google Scholar]
- Karney BR, Bradbury TN. The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;118:3–34. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karney BR, Bradbury TN. Neuroticism, marital interaction, and the trajectory of marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;72:1075–1092. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.72.5.1075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kline GH, Julien D, Baucom B, Hartman SG, Gilbert K, Gonzales T, et al. The Interactional Dimensions Coding System: A global system for couple interactions. In: Kerig PK, Baucom DH, editors. Couple observational coding systems. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, NJ: 2004. pp. 113–127. [Google Scholar]
- Kline GH, Stanley SM, Markman HJ, Olmos-Gallo PA, Peters M, Whitton SW, Prado L. Timing is everything: Pre-engagement cohabitation and increased risk for poor marital outcomes. Journal of Family Psychology. 2004;18:311–318. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.18.2.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kline GH, Wood LF, Moore S. Validation of revised measures of family and interparental conflict for use with young adults. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage. 2003;39(34):125–142. [Google Scholar]
- Kluwer ES, Johnson MD. Conflict frequency and relationship quality across the transition to parenthood. Journal of Marriage and Family. 69:1089–1106. [Google Scholar]
- Kochanska G, Friesenborg AE, Lange LA, Martel MM. Parents’ personality and infants’ temperament as contributors to their emerging relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2004;86:744–759. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurdek L. Nature and prediction of changes in marital quality for first-time parent and nonparent husbands and wives. Journal of Family Psychology. 1993;3:255–265. [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence E, Nylen K, Cobb RJ. Prenatal expectations and marital satisfaction over the transition to parenthood. Journal of Family Psychology. 2007;21:155–164. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.2.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence E, Rothman AD, Cobb RJ, Rothman MT, Bradbury TN. Marital satisfaction across the transition to parenthood. Journal of Family Psychology. 2008;22:41–50. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.22.1.41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemasters EE. Parenthood as a crisis. Marriage and Family Living. 1957;19:352–355. [Google Scholar]
- Locke HJ, Wallace KM. Short marital-adjustment and prediction tests: Their reliability and validity. Marriage and Family Living. 1959;21:251–255. [Google Scholar]
- Lorensen M, Wilson ME, White MA. Norwegian families: Transition to parenthood. Health Care for Women International. 2004;25:334–348. doi: 10.1080/07399330490278394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu L. The transition to parenthood: Stress, resources, and gender differences in a Chinese society. Journal of Community Psychology. 2006;34:471–488. [Google Scholar]
- MacDermid SM, Huston TL, McHale SM. Changes in marriage associated with the transition to parenthood: Individual differences as a function of sex-role attitudes and changes in the division of household labor. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1990;52:475–486. [Google Scholar]
- Markman HJ, Stanley SM, Blumberg SL. Fighting for your marriage. Jossey-Bass, Inc; San Francisco: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Markman HJ, Whitton SW, Kline GH, Thompson H, Peters M, Stanley SM, et al. Use of an empirically-based marriage education program by religious organizations: Results of a dissemination trial. Family Relations. 2004;53:504–512. [Google Scholar]
- McHale S, Huston TL. The effect of the transition to parenthood on the marriage relationships. Journal of Family Issues. 1985;6:409–435. doi: 10.1177/019251385006004002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nock SL, Sanchez LA, Wright JD. Covenant marriage and the movement to reclaim tradition. Rutgers University Press; New Brunswick, NJ: (in press) [Google Scholar]
- Notarius CI, Vanzetti N. Marital Agendas Protocol. In: Filsinger EE, editor. Marriage and family assessment: A sourcebook for family therapy. Sage; Beverly Hills, CA: 1983. pp. 209–227. [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien M, Peyton V. Parenting attitudes and marital intimacy: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Family Psychology. 2002;16:118–127. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perren S, Von Wyl A, Bürgin D, Simoni H, Von Klitzing K. Intergenerational transmission of marital quality across the transition to parenthood. Family Process. 2005;44:441–459. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2005.00071.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1(3):385–401. [Google Scholar]
- Raftery AE. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology. 1995;25:111–163. [Google Scholar]
- Raley S, Bianchi S. Sons, daughters, and family processes: Does gender of children matter? Annual Review of Sociology. 2006;32:401–421. [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush SW, Brennan RT, Barnett RC. A multivariate hierarchical model for studying psychological change within married couples. Journal of Family Psychology. 1995;9:161–174. [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. Sage; Thousand Oaks, CA: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS, Cheong YF, Congdon RT. HLM 6: Hierarchcial linear and nonlinear modeling. Scientific Software International, Inc; Lincolnwood, IL: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Rholes WS, Simpson JA, Campbell L, Grich J. Adult attachment and the transition to parenthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;81:421–435. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.81.3.421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salmela-Aro K, Aunola K, Saisto T, Halmesmaki E, Nurmi J. Couples share similar changes in depressive symptoms and marital satisfaction anticipating the birth of a child. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2006;23:781–803. [Google Scholar]
- Sanders MR, Halford WK, Behrens BC. Parental divorce and premarital couple communication. Journal of Family Psychology. 1999;13(1):60–74. [Google Scholar]
- Schultz MS, Cowan CP, Cowan PA. Promoting healthy beginnings: A randomized controlled trial of a preventive intervention to preserve marital quality during the transition to parenthood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006;74:20–31. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.1.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarz G. Estimating the dimensions of a model. Annals of Statistics. 1978;6:461–464. [Google Scholar]
- Shadish WR, Cook TD, Campbell DT. Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Houghton Mifflin Company; Boston: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro AF, Gottman JM, Carrère S. The baby and the marriage: Identifying factors that buffer against decline in marital satisfaction after the first baby arrives. Journal of Family Psychology. 2000;14:59–70. doi: 10.1037//0893-3200.14.1.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simpson JA, Rholes WS, Campbell L, Tran S, Wilson CL. Adult attachment, the transition to parenthood, and depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84:1172–1187. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanley SM, Amato PR, Johnson CA, Markman HJ. Premarital education, marital quality, and marital stability: Findings from a large, random, household survey. Journal of Family Psychology. 2006;20:117–126. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.1.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanley SM, Blumberg SL, Markman HJ. Helping couples fight for their marriages: The PREP approach. In: Berger R, Hannah MT, editors. Preventive approaches in couples therapy. Brunner/Mazel, Inc; Philadelphia, PA, US: 1999. pp. 279–303. [Google Scholar]
- Stanley SM, Hoyer L, Trathen DW. The Confidence Scale. University of Denver; Denver, CO: 1994. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Stanley SM, Markman HJ. Assessing commitment in personal relationships. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1992;54:595–608. [Google Scholar]
- Stanley SM, Markman HJ. Marriage in the 90s: A nationwide random phone survey. PREP; Denver, CO: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Stanley SM, Markman HJ, Whitton S. Communication, conflict, and commitment: Insights on the foundations of relationship success from a national survey. Family Process. 2002;41:659–675. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2002.00659.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanley SM, Markman HJ, Prado LM, Olmos-Gallo PA, Tonelli L, Peters M, et al. Community-based premarital prevention: Clergy and lay leaders on the front lines. Family Relations. 2001;50(1):67–76. [Google Scholar]
- Stanley SM, Rhoades GK, Markman HJ. Sliding vs. Deciding: Inertia and the premarital cohabitation effect. Family Relations. 2006;55:499–509. [Google Scholar]
- Stanley SM, Whitton SW, Markman HJ. Maybe I do: Interpersonal commitment and premarital or nonmarital cohabitation. Journal of Family Issues. 2004;25:496–519. [Google Scholar]
- Story LB, Karney BR, Lawrence E, Bradbury TN. Interpersonal mediators in the intergenerational transition of marital dysfunction. Journal of Family Psychology. 2004;18:519–529. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.18.3.519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan KT, Bradbury T. Are premarital prevention programs reaching couples at risk for marital dysfunction? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1997;65:24–30. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.65.1.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tilden VP, Shepherd P. Increasing the rate of identification of battered women in an emergency department: Use of a nursing protocol. Research in Nursing and Health. 1987;10:209–215. doi: 10.1002/nur.4770100403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Twenge JM, Campbell WK, Foster CA. Parenthood and marital satisfaction: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2003;65:574–583. [Google Scholar]
- Velicer WF. Time series models of individual substance abusers. In: Collins LM, Seitz LA, editors. Advances in data analyses for prevention intervention research. NIDA; Rockville, MD: 1994. pp. 264–299. NIDA Research Monograph No. 142. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West SG, Biesanz J, Pitts SC. Causal inference and generalization in field settings: Experimental and quasi-experimental designs. In: Reis HT, Judd CM, editors. Handbook of research methods in social psychology. Cambridge University Press; New York: 2000. pp. 40–84. [Google Scholar]
- Whisman MA, McClelland GH. Designing, testing, and interpreting interactions and moderator effects in family research. Journal of Family Psychology. 2005;19:111–120. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.1.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White LK, Booth A. The transition to parenthood and marital quality. Journal of Family Issues. 1985;6:435–449. doi: 10.1177/019251385006004003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitton SW, Olmos-Gallo PA, Stanley SM, Prado LM, Kline GH, Peters M, Markman HJ. Depressive symptoms in early marriage: Predictions from relationship confidence and negative marital interaction. Journal of Family Psychology. 2007;21:297–306. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.2.297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]