Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Jul 14.
Published in final edited form as: J Adolesc Health. 2007 Apr 30;41(1):102–104. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.02.013

Puberty and Gender Interact to Predict Social Anxiety Symptoms in Early Adolescence

Julianna Deardorff 1, Chris Hayward 2, Kimberly A Wilson 2, Susan Bryson 2, Lawrence D Hammer 3, Stewart Agras 2
PMCID: PMC2710300  NIHMSID: NIHMS26035  PMID: 17577541

Abstract

This study examines direct and interactive effects of puberty and gender on social anxiety symptoms in early adolescence. One hundred-six participants were assessed at ages 9.5 and 11. Results suggest that gender and puberty interact to predict social anxiety symptoms. Advanced puberty was associated with increased symptoms for girls only.

Keywords: Puberty, Anxiety, Women’s Health, Adolescent Development


The highest risk period for onset of social anxiety symptoms occurs during adolescence [1,2]. Social anxiety, in turn, predicts underachievement in school, fewer social networks and close friends, and poorer social skills [3]. As with depression, social anxiety is more common in adolescent girls than boys [4]. While rates of depression increase in girls relative to boys during puberty, no published studies have addressed whether puberty is also a time when gender differences in social anxiety symptoms emerge. The current study tested whether pubertal status and gender are associated with social anxiety symptoms among early adolescents. Puberty and gender were expected to interact to predict social anxiety symptoms, with advanced pubertal status leading to heightened symptoms of social anxiety for girls but not for boys.

Method

Participants

Institutional Review Board approval was obtained from the university. Participants constitute a cohort followed for 11 years from birth. The original cohort consisted of 260 infants enrolled in a study of risk factors for obesity [5]. Parents were recruited from well newborn nurseries. Eligible infants were born full term, had Apgar scores of 7 or above, and had no congenital abnormalities or illness during newborn hospitalization. At recruitment, 46% were girls. The majority (88%) was non-Hispanic White, with 9% Asian and 3% Hispanic. Ninety-nine percent of fathers and 75% of mothers were employed; 80% of parents had a college degree. Of the 146 children who participated at age 11, 106 (52% girls) had pubertal data at ages 9.5 and 11. These 106 youth comprise the present sample. Of note, the larger study officially ended at age 6 accounting for the reduced sample size; however, over half of the participants opted to continue. The only group difference between those who left and those who remained is mothers who remained in the study exhibited higher educational attainment.

Measures

Pubertal status was measured at ages 9.5 and 11 by self-reported Tanner staging using line drawings of the five pubertal stages, which compares favorably with pediatric examination [6]. Breast and pubic hair development were averaged for girls and genitalia and pubic hair for boys. Social anxiety was assessed at age 11 using the Social Anxiety subscale from the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC), which evidences good reliability and validity [7]. The coefficient alpha in the current sample was.86. Remaining subscales from the MASC, Physical Symptoms, Harm Avoidance, and Separation/Panic, were used in secondary analyses. Given the potential social implications of advanced pubertal development for young girls, we expected that a gender by puberty interactive effect would be limited to the Social Anxiety subscale.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Participants were dichotomized as nonpubertal (Tanner stage I) and pubertal (Tanner stage II or greater). At age 9.5, 40% of boys and 44% of girls were nonpubertal. By age 11, 21% of boys and 15% of girls were nonpubertal. To capture change, participants were categorized into 3 groups based on pubertal status at both assessments. Group 1 included children who were nonpubertal at ages 9.5 and 11. Ten participants (1 girl, 9 boys) reported being pubertal at 9.5 and nonpubertal at 11. These participants were classified as nonpubertal at both time points, assuming that youth report more accurately at older ages. Group 2 included those who transitioned from nonpubertal at age 9.5 to pubertal at age 11, and Group 3 those classified as pubertal at both ages. Table 1 shows sample size and mean social anxiety scores by group. Sample means and standard deviations were comparable to normative data reported in the MASC manual for boys and girls in this age group.

Table 1.

Mean Scores and Standard Deviations on the Social Anxiety Subscale of the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC) at Age 11 by Pubertal Status and Gender

Boys (n = 51) Girls (n = 55)
Non pubertal at 9 and 11 (n = 19) 8.9(4.7) (n = 11) 4.5(4.9) (n = 8)
In transition at 9 and 11 (n = 25) 7.8(5.1) (n = 10) 7.3(5.1) (n = 15)
Pubertal at 9 and 11 (n = 62) 6.7(5.2) (n = 30) 10.6(6.6) (n = 32)

Gender F(1,100) =.1, ns

Puberty F(2,100) = 1.0, ns

Gender by Puberty Interaction F(2,100) = 4.4, p < .05

Primary analyses

Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted. Pubertal status, gender and their interaction were entered as independent variables to predict social anxiety scores at age 11. There were no main effects for pubertal status [F(2,100) = 1.0, ns] or gender [F(1,100) = 0.1, ns]. There was, however, a significant interaction between gender and pubertal status [F(2,100) = 4.4, p < .05]. To probe this interaction, analyses were stratified by gender with pubertal status as the independent variable and social anxiety at age 11 as the dependent variable (Figure 1). As predicted, for girls, pubertal status was significantly related to social anxiety symptoms [F(2,52) = 3.9, p < .05] with more developed girls exhibiting greater symptoms. For boys, the relationship between pubertal status and social anxiety symptoms was not significant [F(2,48) = 0.8, ns].

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Interaction between pubertal status and gender in predicting social anxiety symptoms on the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC) at age 11.

Secondary Analyses

Secondary analyses were conducted using the Harm Avoidance, Physical Symptoms, and Separation/Panic subscales to determine whether findings were unique to social anxiety. Results were not significant.

Discussion

Findings suggest that the emergence of social anxiety symptoms in early adolescence may depend on gender and pubertal status. Only in girls was advanced pubertal development associated with higher levels of social anxiety. These results are consistent with those in the area of gender differences in depression [8]. To date, it is unknown why internalizing symptoms increase in girls relative to boys at puberty. Evidence suggests that hormones may play a critical role [9]. Future studies examining relations between morphological changes, hormones, and social anxiety are warranted.

In the psychosocial realm, puberty may be a vulnerable time for the development of social anxiety symptoms given gender-specific stressors girls face, including body dissatisfaction, restricted gender roles, and unwanted sexual attention [10]. Girls with advanced physical development may elicit negative responses from others associated with weight gain, emerging sexuality, and body image. Future studies should examine biological and psychosocial mechanisms related to puberty and social anxiety. At minimum, pubertal stage should be assessed in future studies of social anxiety given that gender differences may only be detectable when pubertal status is considered.

The current sample was predominantly European American and educated, therefore caution should be used when generalizing these findings to a broader population of adolescents. Small sample size may have prevented detecting main effects for pubertal status and gender but provided a statistically conservative test of the interaction. The current study is limited in that it does not address the issue of clinically significant social anxiety. Future studies would benefit from including clinical cut-points or assessing diagnostic criteria for Social Anxiety Disorder. Finally, although this study was prospective, it is difficult to determine the direction of effects. The possibility remains that elevated prepubertal anxiety symptoms contributed to advanced pubertal development.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by training grants awarded to the first author from the National Institute of Mental Health, T32MH019391, and National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, K12HD052163. Funding for the larger study was awarded to Dr. Agras from the National Institute of Child Health and Development, HD25492.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Schneier FR, Johnson J, Hornig CD, et al. Social phobia: comorbidity and morbidity in an epidemiological sample. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49:282–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820040034004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Wittchen HU, Stein MB, Kessler RC. Social fears and social phobia in a community sample of adolescents and young adults: prevalence, risk factors, and co-morbidity. Psychol Med. 1999;29:309–23. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798008174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kashdan TB, Herbert JD. Social anxiety disorder in childhood and adolescence: current status and future directions. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2001;4:37–61. doi: 10.1023/a:1009576610507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.La Greca AM, Lopez N. Social anxiety among adolescents: linkages with peer relations and friendships. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1998;26:83–94. doi: 10.1023/a:1022684520514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hammer LD, Bryson S, Agras S. Development of feeding practices during the first five years of life. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1999;153:189–194. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.153.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Dorn LD, Susman EJ, Nottleman ED, et al. Perceptions of puberty: adolescent, parent, and health care personnel. Dev Psychol. 1990;26:322–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.March JS, Parker JD, Sullivan K, Stallings P, Conners CK. The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC): Factor structure, reliability, and validity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:554–65. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199704000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hayward C, Sanborn K. Puberty and the emergence of gender differences in psychopathology. J Adolesc Health. 2002;30:49–58. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00336-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Angold A, Costello EJ, Erkanli A, Worthman CM. Pubertal changes in hormone levels and depression in girls. Psychol Med. 1999;29:1043–53. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799008946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Nolen-Hoeksema S, Girgus JS. The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psychol Bull. 1994;115:424–43. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES