Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Jul 25.
Published in final edited form as: Anesth Analg. 2008 Jul;107(1):309–317. doi: 10.1213/01.ane.0b013e31816ba437

Sex, Gender, and Pain: An Overview of a Complex Field

Robert W Hurley 1, Meredith C B Adams 1
PMCID: PMC2715547  NIHMSID: NIHMS126747  PMID: 18635502

Abstract

Traditionally, biomedical research in the field of pain has been conducted with male animals and subjects. Over the past 20–30 yr, it has been increasingly recognized that this narrow approach has missed an important variable: sex. An ever-increasing number of studies have established sex differences in response to pain and analgesics. These studies have demonstrated that the differences between the sexes appear to have a biological and psychological basis. We will provide brief review of the epidemiology, rodent, and human experimental findings. The controversies and widespread disagreement in the literature highlight the need for a progressive approach to the questions involving collaborative efforts between those trained in the basic and clinical biomedical sciences and those in the epidemiological and social sciences. In order for patients suffering from acute and/or chronic pain to benefit from this work, the approach has to involve the use or development of clinically relevant models of nociception or pain to answer the basic, but complex, question. The present state of the literature allows no translation of the work to our clinical decision-making.


The terms “sex” and “gender” are not synonymous. According to the Institute of Medicine definition, sex is “the classifications of living things, generally as male or female according to their reproductive organs and function assigned by the chromosomal complement.” Gender is “a person’s self-representation as male or female, or how that person is responded to by social institutions on the basis of the individual’s gender presentation.”1 The lack of semantic precision in the literature can and has led to confusion and faulty conclusions.

A large body of data has been collected in the past 20 years concerning differences between the sexes in response to pain, including pain thresholds, and in the tolerance and response to pain treatment. However, the exact differences, as well as their relevance, are far from clear. According to the International Association for the Study of Pain, “pain is an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience arising from actual or potential tissue damage or described in terms of such damage.” This definition does not differentiate between “pain” as a woman experiences it from “pain” as a man experiences it, and thus fundamental questions still remain. If there are differences, are they the result of nature or nurture or a combination of both? Or are the differences noted in the literature a result of a known or unknown testing bias? If there are differences, do they matter for our clinical management of acute and chronic pain?

This brief review is intended as an introduction to the history of the field of sex differences in pain medicine and as a discussion of the state of the literature. Our intent is to highlight some of the ambiguities that have developed in the field. It is hoped that this summary will spur future research into sex differences and pain but will also make clinical investigators aware that there are differences and they should be considered in the design of future studies.

The National Library of Medicine’s PubMed database, EMBASE, PsychInfo, OVID/Medline were searched for the time period from the databases inception to September 1, 2007. Terms for the primary search were (“gender” OR “sex”) AND (“pain” OR “analgesia” OR “antinociception”). “AND” and “OR” represent Boolean search terms. The search was furthered narrowed by limiting it to humans or animals in the English or translated literature where relevant. Attempts were made to restrict the manuscripts to those addressing “sex” or “gender” as a primary goal of the study not a secondary finding.2 The search was further narrowed to examine specific medications including drugs from the following classes: local anesthetics, N-methyl-D-aspartate modulators, opioids, α-adrenergics, anticonvulsants, and antidepressants (Table 1). These medications were queried by adding the generic or trade name of specific medications to the primary search; for example (“Neurontin” OR “gabap-entin”) AND (“gender” OR “sex”) AND (“pain” OR “analgesia” OR “antinociception”).

Table 1.

Medications Included in Search Strategy for Humans

Opioid Anticonvulsant Antidepressant N-methyl-D- aspartate modulator α-Adrenergic modulators Local anesthetic
Morphine Gabapentin Venlafaxine Dextromethorphan Clonidine Lidocaine
Hydromorphone Pregabalin Duloxetine Ketamine Dexmedetomidine Mexiletine
Fentanyl Lamotrigine Amitriptyline Amantadine
Pentazocine Carbamazepine Nortriptyline
Butorphanol Oxcarbazepine Imipramine
Buprenorphine Zonegran Desipramine
Nalbuphine Topiramate Bupropion

EPIDEMIOLOGY OF SEX DIFFERENCES IN PAIN MEDICINE

Females report more severe pain, more frequent bouts of pain, more anatomically diffuse and longer-lasting pain than males with similar disease processes, even when male and female specific disorders, including male urologic and female gynecologic pain, are excluded from the analysis. Females have a more frequent prevalence of pain related to musculoskeletal or to visceral origin, as well as pain related to auto-immune disease (Table 2).3 Substantial amounts of the accumulated data are from animal models, which rely heavily upon the objective but indirect signs of the pain or, more precisely, the nociceptive experience.4 However, human research models are highly influenced by sociocultural variables that have little to do with a biological difference of pain threshold or sensation between women and men. These sociocultural variables influence the social interaction between the human subjects and their experimenters, which can confound their reporting of the pain experience, resulting in biased or confounded data. There is an inherent reporting bias in the epidemiological research related to the incidence of pain in the sexes. Females are more likely to visit a physician and are more likely to report pain as a symptom than males5,6 [reviewed in Ref. 7], which can therefore lead to an over-estimation of the differences between the sexes. Furthermore, the incidence of specific pain syndromes or pain related to certain disease states is not absolute; it changes as people age8 and as their diseases progress.9 In an effort to reduce these biases, there has been substantial research in rodents, in which it is assumed many of these variables can be controlled and therefore removed from consideration.

Table 2.

Sex Prevalence of Clinical Pain Syndromes or Diseases

Prevalence
Bodily area Female > male Female < male
Head
 Headache Chronic tension Cluster
Migraine with aura Migraine without aura
Postdural puncture Posttraumatic
Cervicogenic
Temporal arteritis
Occipital neuralgia
 Oral Odontalgia Paratrigeminal syndrome
Burning mouth Trigeminal postherpetic neuralgia
Temporomandibular disorder
Trigeminal neuralgia
Extremities
 Arms Carpal tunnel syndrome Brachial plexus neuropathy
Raynaud’s disease
CRPS type I
Scleroderma
 Legs Chronic venous insufficiency Meralgia paraesthetica
Peroneal muscular atrophy Gout
Piriformis syndrome Intermittent claudication
Raynaud’s disease
CRPS type I
Viscera
 Bowel Chronic constipation Duodenal ulcer
Irritable bowel syndrome
Proctalgia fugax
 Esophagus Esophagitis
 Pancreas Pancreatic disease
 Gall bladder Postcholecystectomy pain
Autoimmune Lupus erythematosus Reiter’s syndrome
Multiple sclerosis
Rheumatoid arthritis
Psychogenic Fibromyalgia

CRPS = complex regional pain syndrome.

Table modified from Wall and Melzack’s Textbook of Pain, 5th ed.3

ANIMAL MODELS

Baseline Differences

Differences in pain thresholds between male and female rodents have been explicitly studied during the last 20–30 yr, but it has only been over the past 15 yr that this issue has gained the prominence and the attention it deserves.10 It has become “well accepted” that female rodents have a lower pain threshold in experimental models of hot thermal,1115 chemical,1618 inflammatory,1921 and mechanical nociception.22,23 Mixed results among the animal models are most likely due to study design variation, but potentially also to the rodent population being tested. Studies with divergent results when examining thermal and inflammatory nociception considered additional variables in their study design, such as rodent population genotype.24

The nature of the animal injury either transient or persistent also has an impact on the sex differences in pain testing.25 In animal models of persistent nociception, such as neuropathic or inflammatory injuries, the results are mixed. In neuropathic pain models, some investigators have not found a difference between males and females,23 whereas others have found differences dependent on rat strain, with females having a more robust response to injury. Still other researchers have reported inconsistent findings.2628 Furthermore, the developmental rate of self-injurious behavior or autotomy after sciatic nerve destruction was lower in female rats.29 Others have found the response amplitude to neuropathic injury to be the same, but the resultant duration of hyperalgesia to be longer, in female rats.26,30 Female rodents appear to have a longer lasting response to inflammatory injuries in some experimental models20 but not in others.25,31 Interestingly, using the Brennan et al. model of postincisional pain,32 there were no sex differences in tests of mechanical allodynia or hyperalgesia.33,34 This model of nociception could be considered one of the more clinically applicable or valid pain models. It involves an incision to the hindpaw of a rodent under brief anesthesia and produces a mixed inflammatory/neuropathic and nociceptive “injury” similar to that in humans after minor surgical procedures.

In order for conclusions to be drawn regarding sex differences in experimental models of nociception, a large trial assessing all variables known to affect pain thresholds and tolerance including, but not limited to, strain, age, bedding, food, acute versus persistent nociception, and modality of noxious stimulus needs to be conducted. Until such a trial is conducted, the most clinically relevant animal models for extrapolation to humans, including the Brennan et al. postincisional model and the various models of complex regional pain syndrome type II (or causalgia) such as the sciatic nerve injury model, show either no difference or modest differences between male and female animals.

Nondrug Induced Antinociception

In addition to baseline differences in nociceptive threshold or in response to transient injury, female rodents respond differently than male rodents to nondrug induced antinociception. Stress-induced antinociception (SIA) is the most common category of environmentally induced antinociception. Male rodents appear to have greater SIA resulting from numerous techniques, including forced cold water swim,3537 mild electroshocks,38 restraint39,40 and predator exposure.41 The results with exercise-induced analgesia (EIA), which is considered a form of SIA, are somewhat different. EIA stimulates decreased sensitivity to morphine-induced analgesia in male and female rats, potentially due to the increased endogenous B-endorphin levels.42

Drug-Induced Antinociception

Opioids

Since 2000, more than 70 papers (excluding reviews) have been published on the topic of sex differences in the response to opioid antinociception in animals. Forty-three directly tested the hypothesis that male and female rodents respond differently to opioid-induced antinociception. Male rodents had a more robust response to opioids in 70% of these studies. However, 19% of these studies found that males and females had an equal response, and 11% showed that females had a greater antinociceptive response. Although most studies adjusted dosage for weight, plasma concentrations of opioids were not measured throughout the main body of the literature. This leaves the question of pharmacokinetic effects on sex differences unresolved.

Opioid analgesia is unlikely to be a uniform phenomenon because there are a minimum of three separate endogenous opioid receptors: μ, δ, and κ. These receptors are differentially located throughout the neuraxis and periphery and have disparate functions, depending on location as well as receptor subtype.4346 In more than 90% of the papers showing a male predominance of antinociception, a μ opioid receptor agonist was used. Interestingly, in five studies in which females had a greater antinociceptive response to opioid receptor agonists, four used a κ opioid receptor agonist.17,4749 The remaining study examined the effect of morphine antinociception after early maternal separation in rat pups, which alters endogenous steroid production and postnatal development.50 κ Opioid receptor agonists, similar to μ opioid receptor agonists, produce antinociception in some animal models of pain. The dissimilar response between males and females to these subtype agonists lends further credence to the notion that opioid-induced analgesia is not a uniform “opioid class” effect. However, this should not be interpreted to mean that μ opioid receptor agonists are antinociceptive in males and κ opioid receptor agonists are antinociceptive in females. Two recent studies51,52 examined κ receptor agonists in rats and mice and found that males had a greater response than females. This response contradicts earlier work17,4749 and is similar to the antinociceptive response of rodents to μ receptor agonists. Although the behavioral results are conflicting, genetic studies appear to support a biological basis for the μ/κ dichotomy between the male and female response to opioid agonists. Indeed, Mogil et al.52 discovered the gene for melanocortin-1 receptor (MC1r) mediated κ opioid agonist antinociception in female but not in male mice. In the absence of this gene, female mice had an enhanced antinociceptive response to κ opioids. This gene mutation has no known effect on μ opioid receptor-induced antinociception. Although most literature demonstrates a more robust response of male rodents, this may be the result of multifarious variables including receptor subtypes. Future studies should control potentially important baseline variables, such as rodent strain, and to explore the opioid receptor subtypes.

Other Drugs

Unlike the relative abundance of literature around the question of sex differences in the response to opioid administration in animals, there is a paucity of research regarding the use of any of the more common analgesic medications (Table 1). The most commonly investigated class of non-opioid agonists is the indirect and the direct-acting cholinergic agonists.53 These drugs show male predominant antinociception in most studies,53 but not all studies.34 This will not be further discussed here as cholinergic agonists are not currently in common use for analgesia in pain medicine.

The next most commonly studied class of drugs is the α-adrenergic medications. Systemically administered clonidine has consistently been found to be more effective in producing antinociception in male rodents in models of acute thermal nociception.5459 However, Kroin et al.,34 using the clinically applicable Brennan et al. model of postincisional nociception, reported the absence of a sex difference after intrathecal administration of clonidine. The other non-opioid drugs unfortunately have little supporting data and have each only been examined by a single laboratory. Baclofen was found to have similar results to μ opioid receptor agonists and clonidine, demonstrating male predominance of antinociception.54 Gabapentin,34 paroxetine,60 cannabinoids,58,59 and ketamine54 were found to have no sex difference. The newly developed anticonvulsant, lacosamide, produced a greater amount of antihyperalgesia in females in the clinically relevant spinal cord injury model of nociception.61 The dearth of information makes it difficult to infer whether sex determines efficacy of non-opioid analgesics.

Unfortunately, the testing of clinically applicable analgesics on animals of either sex has not provided any greater clarity. The greatest amount of data supports the current belief that, among the opioid compounds, μ opioid agonists produce significantly greater antinociception in males and κ agonists produce greater antinociception in females than in males. Unfortunately, recently performed, methodologically rigorous studies have brought the previously accepted notions into doubt.34

HUMAN MODELS

In 1994, the United States National Institutes of Health mandated that all human clinical trials supported by National Institutes of Health funds have a representative sample of females. This decision was based on the accepted and assumed biological differences between females and males, including the differences in pain thresholds and analgesic response to pain medications.

Superficially, the determination of pain thresholds in human subjects appears to be a simpler experimental paradigm. Humans, unlike rodents, can verbally express the sensory and affective components of a painful stimulus. The investigator is therefore not forced to use indirect reflexive responses to determine the point at which the stimulus crosses the threshold from innocuous to noxious. Experimentation with humans has obvious external validity that is lacking in animal research. However, human research presents manifold challenges. The straightforward challenge is that the human experimentation is expensive, and that subjecting people to painful stimuli for research can be difficult to defend to the Institutional Research Review Board. More importantly, the painful stimuli that can be used are transient (e.g., thermal—cold or heat, chemical—capsaicin, and mechanical—algometry) and may represent only a subset of clinical pain, thus reducing its validity. Although some of the testing regimens simulate acute pain, there are not sufficient data to extrapolate to persistent and/or neuropathic pain states. The intricate challenges to objective data collection in humans include the fact that human response to pain is influenced by numerous social, cultural, and psychological variables that are not uniform to one sex or the other.

In a meta-analysis of studies examining sex differences in pain response in healthy subjects <60-yr-old, the authors found that women report higher pain severity at lower thresholds and have less tolerance to noxious stimulation than males.62 The greatest sex differences were noted in tests examining mechanical pain via a pressure algometer or calibrated calipers. The second largest effect size was in electrical noxious stimulation, followed by thermal and ischemic stimuli. Interestingly, the most common stimuli used to test sex differences in animals has traditionally been thermal stimulation vide supra. In a large multicenter trial, Rolke et al.63 used quantitative sensory testing to determine sensory detection thresholds and pain thresholds for thermal and mechanical noxious stimuli. The results demonstrated a similar trend: females had lower pain thresholds, but the modalities were not in the same rank order. The greatest disparities for sex differences were found for heat pain threshold, followed by cold pain and pain to blunt pressure.

Many other physiological, sociocultural, and psychological variables have been identified as contributing to the differences between the two sexes with regard to pain. One set of factors includes the reproductive status and menstrual cycle of female subjects.64 A number of pain states, such as temporomandibular joint disorder, are more commonly diagnosed in women but only after puberty.65 Furthermore, pain thresholds have been found to be lower in females during their menses.66 Stening et al.67 found that women in low progesterone and high estradiol states had pain thresholds that were not different from men. During the low estradiol phase of the menstrual cycle, females were found to have higher pain scores to persistent noxious stimulation.68 This has been attributed to a reduction in endogenous opioid receptor activation in brain regions associated with analgesia when compared with the high estradiol state. The age of the subject also modifies the pain threshold. Advancing age is positively associated with pain threshold.63,69 Pickering et al.70 found that the difference in score between males and females decreased with advancing age. The significant sex difference seen in thermal and mechanical threshold and in tolerance in younger volunteers became nonsignificant in volunteers older than 40-yr-old.

Another influential factor includes the “pain” endpoint being examined, such as pain threshold versus pain tolerance.62,70,71 Pool et al.72 showed that pain tolerance is highly malleable and strongly influenced by the subject’s “gender” norms. Males who are highly identified with the “male” role tolerate higher levels of noxious stimuli, but those males who do not have this belief tolerate noxious stimuli at the same level as females. Stereotypical social gender roles also influence pain threshold differences. In two very revealing studies, the investigators showed that the gender of the experimenter influences the response and pain threshold of the subjects. Male subjects reported less pain and had higher thresholds when tested by a female examiner.73 This effect was accentuated when the examiner of the opposite sex was attractive. Male subjects, again, had lower reports of pain and higher thresholds with the attractive female examiner; interestingly, females reported more pain and had lower thresholds with attractive male examiners.74 Therefore, the examiner introduces a substantial testing bias and entirely different results are produced if a male subject is tested by a male examiner (no sex difference) versus a female examiner (large sex difference).

Subject coping strategies can also be important in pain threshold modulation. Maladaptive pain coping strategies, such as catastrophizing, have been associated with poorer adjustment to clinical pain and higher sensitivity to experimental pain.7578 Importantly, higher catastrophizing among women than men has been reported, and can account for the sex difference.7981 Edwards et al.82 further examined this issue and found that controlling for negative affect and catastrophizing did not fully explain the sex differences in threshold or tolerance but these factors have large effect sizes on their own.

As discussed above, the sex difference in humans is neither a universal nor a large effect. Furthermore, no difference between sexes is found in at least one-third of the published studies, and effect sizes are often in the small to moderate range.62 Other investigators have sought objective measures of pain as a result of the numerous factors that have been shown to influence, and in some cases abolish the pain threshold and tolerance differences between the sexes. Paulson et al.71 used positron emission tomography (PET) to investigate regional brain activation after a painful somatic thermal stimulus in healthy normal volunteer male and female subjects. They reported that females had significantly greater activation of the contralateral prefrontal cortex, the contralateral insula and the thalamus compared with males, suggesting sexual dimorphism in response to pain. Unfortunately, the investigators used a standardized thermal stimulus at 40°C or 50°C and therefore did not match the thermal stimulus subjective pain intensity between males and females. Thus, this study reflected a difference in brain activation that was more likely the result of different perceived pain intensities rather than a true sex difference. In a later study, also using PET technology, the pain intensity was matched between the sexes and the results were the opposite of the earlier study, males had greater activation than females.83 Finally, a study using matched pain intensity and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) showed no sex-based difference in brain activation.84 Other PET studies have described sex differences in responses to visceral pain from rectal balloon distension, although principally with chronic visceral pain patients.85,86 However, the differences reported were predominantly in the direction of greater activation in men. These studies were replicated using fMRI with similar results of a male predominance of neuronal activation in pain-related areas of the brain.85

In summary, pain thresholds in humans vary by internal factors such as sex, gender, age, female menstrual phase, and psychological variables, including catastrophizing, anxiety, and depression. External factors also affect the outcome including testing environment, sex, and gender of the examiner, and the modality of the noxious stimuli. In the largest study assessing the role of sex on pain thresholds using multiple modalities, it was found that females had lower pain thresholds in thermal and mechanical pain testing.63 Unfortunately, this subjective difference has not been consistently supported by other confirmatory techniques including PET or fMRI brain imaging. Awareness of the possible differences between males and females in response to pain is the only clinical application of the present data with no guidance for particular situations.

Nondrug Induced Analgesia

SIA has very different effects on humans than on rodents. For instance, stress produces a pain threshold increase in male rodents, whereas stress in humans results in either no difference between the sexes or female predominance.8789 EIA in humans produces similarly disparate findings. In two studies in which the participants were required to do isometric exercises90 or run on a treadmill,89 females had the greatest increase in pain thresholds and tolerance.

Drug-Induced Analgesia

Unlike the abundance of literature addressing the question of drug-induced sex differences in experimental pain in rodents, the human literature is not as voluminous. Most literature addresses the response to μ opioid receptor agonists and the remainder addresses κ opioid receptor agonists. As far as we know, there has not been testing of other clinically available medications on humans in models of experimental pain. With respect to μ opioid receptor agonists, primarily morphine, human females have a very different response than their rodent counterparts. In multiple studies, either no difference9193 was noted between sexes or females had a significantly greater response to the medication.9496 Although initial studies attributed the difference to a pharmacokinetic difference,97 the metabolism of morphine to morphine-6-glucuronide, more recent work by Romberg et al.92 demonstrates that there is no sex difference in morphine-6-glucuronide concentrations. To further exclude pharmacokinetic explanations for the sex differences, the same investigative group subsequently published findings using the potent synthetic μ opioid receptor agonist alfentanil. The subjects’ responses to alfentanil did not differ based on their sex.98

The human response to κ opioid receptor agonists is not fully elucidated. In a postsurgical model of pain, females had a greater analgesic response to κ agonist-antagonist medications, including pentazocine, nalbuphine, and butorphanol.99101 However, in experimental models of pain either no difference between males and females was noted102 or males had an increased sensitivity to the medications.52 Currently, there is no cohesive theory that would explain these conflicting results. Mogil et al.52 suggest that the increased responsiveness of males to κ opioid receptors agonists may be related to the absence of the MC1r gene. In an elegant study, this group studied women with fair skin and red hair who often have a functional reduction in MC1r. The women with a genetically proven loss of function polymorphisms experienced an accentuated analgesic response to pentazocine similar to the mice without the MC1r gene. Their analgesic response was indistinguishable from the male subjects in the study.52 This finding has led to the proposal that the MC1r gene product acts as an anti-opioid and, therefore, when removed, unmasks the full κ opioid analgesic effect. Thus females with a subpopulation of MC1r deficient members would have an enhanced “population” κ effect in comparison to males with a full MC1r complement. However, this does not explain the lack of difference or the greater analgesic response of females to μ opioids in clinical and experimental models. Unfortunately, the data with regard to opioid analgesics garnered from human trials are not sufficient to guide clinical practice. The studies do not justify the conclusion that males or females have a greater responsiveness to μ or κ opioid receptor agonists and therefore they should continue to receive similar pain management until more definitive studies are conducted.

CONCLUSION

Although this review is, by necessity, an overview of an enormous, active and growing field, it is intended to provide the history of the field from, and the rationale for, the basic science work in rodents progressing to the clinical experimental work in humans and the mandatory inclusion of females in all clinical trials of medications that are intend for either sex. Both the animal and human literature are rife with disagreement and conflicting findings within each species and, importantly, between each species. This supports the thesis that more work needs to be done to elucidate the common/basic biological and psychological factors that produce the differences that are described. These are important questions not just for the satisfaction of simple intellectual curiosity, but also to allow the pain practitioner to offer more focused or empirically guided therapy to his or her patients.

Acknowledgments

Supported by NIH grant MH075884.

References

  • 1.Wizeman TM, Pardue M-L. Does sex matter? Washington: National Academy Press; 2001. Exploring the biological contributions to human health. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Mariano ER, Ilfeld BM, Neal JM. “Going fishing”–the practice of reporting secondary outcomes as separate studies. Reg Anesth Pain Med. 2007;32:183–5. doi: 10.1016/j.rapm.2007.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Holdcroft A, Berkley KJ. Wall and Melzack’s textbook of pain. 5. Philadelphia: Elsevier; 2005. Sex and gender difference in pain and its relief. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Wu CL, Hurley RW, Stauffer JW. Analgesics. In: Lee CJ, editor. Clinical trials of drugs and biopharmaceuticals. New York: CRC Press; 2005. pp. 417–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bingefors K, Isacson D. Epidemiology, co-morbidity, and impact on health-related quality of life of self-reported headache and musculoskeletal pain—a gender perspective. Eur J Pain. 2004;8:435–50. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Isacson D, Bingefors K. Epidemiology of analgesic use: a gender perspective. Eur J Anaesthesiol Suppl. 2002;26:5–15. doi: 10.1097/00003643-200219261-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Myers CD, Riley JL, III, Robinson ME. Psychosocial contributions to sex-correlated differences in pain. Clin J Pain. 2003;19:225–32. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200307000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.LeResche L. Epidemiologic perspectives on sex differences in pain. In: Fillingim RB, editor. Sex, gender, and pain. Seattle: IASP Press; 2000. pp. 233–49. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Turk DC, Okifuji A. Does sex make a difference in the prescription of treatments and the adaptation to chronic pain by cancer and non-cancer patients? Pain. 1999;82:139–48. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00041-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ruda MA. Gender and pain. Pain. 1993;53:1–2. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(93)90048-T. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Chesler EJ, Wilson SG, Lariviere WR, Rodriguez-Zas SL, Mogil JS. Identification and ranking of genetic and laboratory environment factors influencing a behavioral trait, thermal nociception, via computational analysis of a large data archive. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002;26:907–23. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(02)00103-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sternberg WF, Smith L, Scorr L. Nociception and antinociception during the first week of life in mice: sex differences and test dependence. J Pain. 2004;5:420–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2004.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Terner JM, Lomas LM, Smith ES, Barrett AC, Picker MJ. Pharmacogenetic analysis of sex differences in opioid antinociception in rats. Pain. 2003;106:381–91. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2003.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wala EP, Sloan JW, Jing X, Holtman JR. The effects of diazepam dependence and withdrawal on morphine-induced antinociception and changes in locomotion in male and female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2001;69:475–84. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00570-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Terner JM, Barrett AC, Cook CD, Picker MJ. Sex differences in (−)-pentazocine antinociception: comparison to morphine and spiradoline in four rat strains using a thermal nociceptive assay. Behav Pharmacol. 2003;14:77–85. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200302000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Aloisi AM, Albonetti ME, Carli G. Sex differences in the behavioural response to persistent pain in rats. Neurosci Lett. 1994;179:79–82. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(94)90939-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Barrett AC, Smith ES, Picker MJ. Capsaicin-induced hyperalgesia and mu-opioid-induced antihyperalgesia in male and female Fischer 344 rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2003;307:237–45. doi: 10.1124/jpet.103.054478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gaumond I, Arsenault P, Marchand S. The role of sex hormones on formalin-induced nociceptive responses. Brain Res. 2002;958:139–45. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)03661-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bradshaw H, Miller J, Ling Q, Malsnee K, Ruda MA. Sex differences and phases of the estrous cycle alter the response of spinal cord dynorphin neurons to peripheral inflammation and hyperalgesia. Pain. 2000;85:93–9. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(99)00253-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Cook CD, Nickerson MD. Nociceptive sensitivity and opioid antinociception and antihyperalgesia in Freund’s adjuvant-induced arthritic male and female rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2005;313:449–59. doi: 10.1124/jpet.104.077792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Dina OA, Aley KO, Isenberg W, Messing RO, Levine JD. Sex hormones regulate the contribution of PKCepsilon and PKA signalling in inflammatory pain in the rat. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;13:2227–33. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Barrett AC, Smith ES, Picker MJ. Sex-related differences in mechanical nociception and antinociception produced by mu-and kappa-opioid receptor agonists in rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 2002;452:163–73. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(02)02274-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bourquin AF, Suveges M, Pertin M, Gilliard N, Sardy S, Davison AC, Spahn DR, Decosterd I. Assessment and analysis of mechanical allodynia-like behavior induced by spared nerve injury (SNI) in the mouse. Pain. 2006;122:14 e1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.10.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Mogil JS, Chesler EJ, Wilson SG, Juraska JM, Sternberg WF. Sex differences in thermal nociception and morphine antinociception in rodents depend on genotype. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24:375–89. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00015-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wang X, Traub RJ, Murphy AZ. Persistent pain model reveals sex difference in morphine potency. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2006;291:R300–6. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00022.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.DeLeo JA, Rutkowski MD. Gender differences in rat neuropathic pain sensitivity is dependent on strain. Neurosci Lett. 2000;282:197–9. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)00880-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.LaCroix-Fralish ML, Rutkowski MD, Weinstein JN, Mogil JS, Deleo JA. The magnitude of mechanical allodynia in a rodent model of lumbar radiculopathy is dependent on strain and sex. Spine. 2005;30:1821–7. doi: 10.1097/01.brs.0000174122.63291.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Tall JM, Stuesse SL, Cruce WL, Crisp T. Gender and the behavioral manifestations of neuropathic pain. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2001;68:99–104. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(00)00461-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wagner R, DeLeo JA, Coombs DW, Myers RR. Gender differences in autotomy following sciatic cryoneurolysis in the rat. Physiol Behav. 1995;58:37–41. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)00037-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Coyle DE, Sehlhorst CS, Mascari C. Female rats are more susceptible to the development of neuropathic pain using the partial sciatic nerve ligation (PSNL) model. Neurosci Lett. 1995;186:135–8. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(95)11304-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bellinger LL, Spears R, King CM, Dahm F, Hutchins B, Kerins CA, Kramer PR. Capsaicin sensitive neurons role in the inflamed TMJ acute nociceptive response of female and male rats. Physiol Behav. 2007;90:782–9. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Brennan TJ, Vandermeulen EP, Gebhart GF. Characterization of a rat model of incisional pain. Pain. 1996;64:493–501. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(95)01441-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Banik RK, Woo YC, Park SS, Brennan TJ. Strain and sex influence on pain sensitivity after plantar incision in the mouse. Anesthesiology. 2006;105:1246–53. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200612000-00025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kroin JS, Buvanendran A, Nagalla SK, Tuman KJ. Postoperative pain and analgesic responses are similar in male and female Sprague-Dawley rats. Can J Anaesth. 2003;50:904–8. doi: 10.1007/BF03018737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kavaliers M, Colwell DD. Decreased predator avoidance in parasitized mice: neuromodulatory correlates. Parasitology. 1995;111(Pt 3):257–63. doi: 10.1017/s0031182000081816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Romero MT, Bodnar RJ. Gender differences in two forms of cold-water swim analgesia. Physiol Behav. 1986;37:893–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Romero MT, Kepler KL, Cooper ML, Komisaruk BR, Bodnar RJ. Modulation of gender-specific effects upon swim analgesia in gonadectomized rats. Physiol Behav. 1987;40:39–45. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90183-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Menendez L. Gender and test dependence of a type of kappa mediated stress induced anlagesia in mice. Gen Pharmacol. 1994;25:903–6. doi: 10.1016/0306-3623(94)90094-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Aloisi AM, Steenbergen HL, van de Poll NE, Farabollini F. Sex-dependent effects of restraint on nociception and pituitary-adrenal hormones in the rat. Physiol Behav. 1994;55:789–93. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90061-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kavaliers M, Innes D. Sex differences in the effects of neuropeptide FF and IgG from neuropeptide FF on morphine- and stress-induced analgesia. Peptides. 1992;13:603–7. doi: 10.1016/0196-9781(92)90096-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kavaliers M, Colwell DD. Sex differences in opioid and non-opioid mediated predator-induced analgesia in mice. Brain Res. 1991;568:173–7. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)91394-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kanarek RB, Gerstein AV, Wildman RP, Mathes WF, D’Anci KE. Chronic running-wheel activity decreases sensitivity to morphine-induced analgesia in male and female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1998;61:19–27. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(98)00059-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Mansour A, Fox CA, Burke S, Meng F, Thompson RD, Akil H, Watson SJ. Mu, delta and kappa opioid receptor mRNA expression in the rat CNS: an in situ hybridization study. J Comp Neurol. 1994;350:412–38. doi: 10.1002/cne.903500307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Mansour A, Fox CA, Thompson Rc, Akil H, Watson SJ. μ-opioid receptor mRNA expression in the rat CNS: comparison to μ-receptor binding. Brain Res. 1994;643:245–65. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90031-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Mansour A, Khachaturian H, Lewis ME, Akil H, Watson SJ. Anatomy of CNS opioid receptors. Trends Neurol Sci. 1988;11:308–14. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(88)90093-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Mansour A, Watson SJ. Anatomical distribution of opioid receptors in mammalians: an overview. In: Herz A, editor. Handbook of experimental pharmacology. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 1993. pp. 79–105. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Binder W, Carmody J, Walker J. Effect of gender on anti-inflammatory and analgesic actions of two kappa-opioids. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2000;292:303–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Holtman JR, Jr, Wala EP. Characterization of the antinociceptive effect of oxycodone in male and female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2006;83:100–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Tershner SA, Mitchell JM, Fields HL. Brainstem pain modulating circuitry is sexually dimorphic with respect to mu and kappa opioid receptor function. Pain. 2000;85:153–9. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(99)00257-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Kalinichev M, Easterling KW, Holtzman SG. Repeated neonatal maternal separation alters morphine-induced antinociception in male rats. Brain Res Bull. 2001;54:649–54. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00485-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lomas LM, Barrett AC, Terner JM, Lysle DT, Picker MJ. Sex differences in the potency of kappa opioids and mixed-action opioids administered systemically and at the site of inflammation against capsaicin-induced hyperalgesia in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;191:273–85. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0663-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Mogil JS, Wilson SG, Chesler EJ, Rankin AL, Nemmani KV, Lariviere WR, Groce MK, Wallace MR, Kaplan L, Staud R, Ness TJ, Glover TL, Stankova M, Mayorov A, Hruby VJ, Grisel JE, Fillingim RB. The melanocortin-1 receptor gene mediates female-specific mechanisms of analgesia in mice and humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:4867–72. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0730053100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Craft RM. Sex differences in opioid analgesia: “from mouse to man. Clin J Pain. 2003;19:175–86. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200305000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Blednov YA, Stoffel M, Alva H, Harris RA. A pervasive mechanism for analgesia: activation of GIRK2 channels. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:277–82. doi: 10.1073/pnas.012682399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Kiefel JM, Cooper ML, Bodnar RJ. Inhibition of mesencephalic morphine analgesia by methysergide in the medial ventral medulla of rats. Physiol Behav. 1992;51:201–5. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(92)90224-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Mitrovic I, Margeta-Mitrovic M, Bader S, Stoffel M, Jan LY, Basbaum AI. Contribution of GIRK2-mediated postsynaptic signaling to opiate and alpha 2-adrenergic analgesia and analgesic sex differences. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:271–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0136822100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Nag S, Mokha SS. Activation of alpha2-adrenoceptors in the trigeminal region produces sex-specific modulation of nociception in the rat. Neuroscience. 2006;142:1255–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Wilson SG, Bryant CD, Lariviere WR, Olsen MS, Giles BE, Chesler EJ, Mogil JS. The heritability of antinociception II: pharmacogenetic mediation of three over-the-counter analgesics in mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2003;305:755–64. doi: 10.1124/jpet.102.047902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Wilson SG, Smith SB, Chesler EJ, Melton KA, Haas JJ, Mitton B, Strasburg K, Hubert L, Rodriguez-Zas SL, Mogil JS. The heritability of antinociception: common pharmacogenetic mediation of five neurochemically distinct analgesics. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2003;304:547–59. doi: 10.1124/jpet.102.041889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Duman EN, Kesim M, Kadioglu M, Ulku C, Kalyoncu NI, Yaris E. Effect of gender on antinociceptive effect of paroxetine in hot plate test in mice. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2006;30:292–6. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Hao JX, Stohr T, Selve N, Wiesenfeld-Hallin Z, Xu XJ. Lacosamide, a new anti-epileptic, alleviates neuropathic pain-like behaviors in rat models of spinal cord or trigeminal nerve injury. Eur J Pharmacol. 2006;553:135–40. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2006.09.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Riley JL, III, Robinson ME, Wise EA, Myers CD, Fillingim RB. Sex differences in the perception of noxious experimental stimuli: a meta-analysis. Pain. 1998;74:181–7. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(97)00199-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, Tolle TR, Treede RD, Beyer A, Binder A, Birbaumer N, Birklein F, Botefur IC, Braune S, Flor H, Huge V, Klug R, Landwehrmeyer GB, Magerl W, Maihofner C, Rolko C, Schaub C, Scherens A, Sprenger T, Valet M, Wasserka B. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and reference values. Pain. 2006;123:231–43. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.01.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Aloisi AM, Bonifazi M. Sex hormones, central nervous system and pain. Horm Behav. 2006;50:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Unruh AM. Gender variations in clinical pain experience. Pain. 1996;65:123–67. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(95)00214-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Giamberardino MA, Berkley KJ, Iezzi S, de Bigontina P, Vecchiet L. Pain threshold variations in somatic wall tissues as a function of menstrual cycle, segmental site and tissue depth in non-dysmenorrheic women, dysmenorrheic women and men. Pain. 1997;71:187–97. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(97)03362-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Stening K, Eriksson O, Wahren L, Berg G, Hammar M, Blomqvist A. Pain sensations to the cold pressor test in normally menstruating women: comparison with men and relation to menstrual phase and serum sex steroid levels. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2007;293:1711–6. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00127.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Smith YR, Stohler CS, Nichols TE, Bueller JA, Koeppe RA, Zubieta JK. Pronociceptive and antinociceptive effects of estradiol through endogenous opioid neurotransmission in women. J Neurosci. 2006;26:5777–85. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5223-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Lariviere M, Goffaux P, Marchand S, Julien N. Changes in pain perception and descending inhibitory controls start at middle age in healthy adults. Clin J Pain. 2007;23:506–10. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e31806a23e8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Pickering G, Jourdan D, Eschalier A, Dubray C. Impact of age, gender and cognitive functioning on pain perception. Gerontology. 2002;48:112–8. doi: 10.1159/000048937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Paulson PE, Minoshima S, Morrow TJ, Casey KL. Gender differences in pain perception and patterns of cerebral activation during noxious heat stimulation in humans. Pain. 1998;76:223–9. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(98)00048-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Pool GJ, Schwegler AF, Theodore BR, Fuchs PN. Role of gender norms and group identification on hypothetical and experimental pain tolerance. Pain. 2007;129:122–9. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Levine FM, De Simone LL. The effects of experimenter gender on pain report in male and female subjects. Pain. 1991;44:69–72. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(91)90149-R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Gijsbers K, Nicholson F. Experimental pain thresholds influenced by sex of experimenter. Percept Mot Skills. 2005;101:803–7. doi: 10.2466/pms.101.3.803-807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Sullivan AF, Kalso EA, McQuay HJ, Dickenson AH. Evidence for the involvement of the mu but not delta opioid receptor subtype in the synergistic interaction between opioid and alpha 2 adrenergic antinociception in the rat spinal cord. Neurosci Lett. 1992;139:65–8. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(92)90859-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Sullivan MJ, Rodgers WM, Kirsch I. Catastrophizing, depression and expectancies for pain and emotional distress. Pain. 2001;91:147–54. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(00)00430-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Swimmer GI, Robinson ME, Geisser ME. Relationship of MMPI cluster type, pain coping strategy, and treatment outcome. Clin J Pain. 1992;8:131–7. doi: 10.1097/00002508-199206000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Thorn BE, Ward LC, Sullivan MJ, Boothby JL. Communal coping model of catastrophizing: conceptual model building. Pain. 2003;106:1–2. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(03)00228-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Fillingim RB, Maddux V, Shackelford JA. Sex differences in heat pain thresholds as a function of assessment method and rate of rise. Somatosens Mot Res. 1999;16:57–62. doi: 10.1080/08990229970654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Keefe FJ, Lefebvre JC, Egert JR, Affleck G, Sullivan MJ, Caldwell DS. The relationship of gender to pain, pain behavior, and disability in osteoarthritis patients: the role of catastrophizing. Pain. 2000;87:325–34. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00296-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Osman A, Barrios FX, Gutierrez PM, Kopper BA, Merrifield T, Grittmann L. The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: further psychometric evaluation with adult samples. J Behav Med. 2000;23:351–65. doi: 10.1023/a:1005548801037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Edwards RR, Haythornthwaite JA, Sullivan MJ, Fillingim RB. Catastrophizing as a mediator of sex differences in pain: differential effects for daily pain versus laboratory-induced pain. Pain. 2004;111:335–41. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Derbyshire SW, Nichols TE, Firestone L, Townsend DW, Jones AK. Gender differences in patterns of cerebral activation during equal experience of painful laser stimulation. J Pain. 2002;3:401–11. doi: 10.1054/jpai.2002.126788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Moulton EA, Keaser ML, Gullapalli RP, Maitra R, Greenspan JD. Sex differences in the cerebral BOLD signal response to painful heat stimuli. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2006;291:R257–67. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00084.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Berman SM, Naliboff BD, Suyenobu B, Labus JS, Stains J, Bueller JA, Ruby K, Mayer EA. Sex differences in regional brain response to aversive pelvic visceral stimuli. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2006;291:R268–76. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00065.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Naliboff BD, Berman S, Chang L, Derbyshire SW, Suyenobu B, Vogt BA, Mandelkern M, Mayer EA. Sex-related differences in IBS patients: central processing of visceral stimuli. Gastroenterology. 2003;124:1738–47. doi: 10.1016/s0016-5085(03)00400-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Rhudy JL, Grimes JS, Meagher MW. Fear-induced hypoalgesia in humans: effects on low intensity thermal stimulation and finger temperature. J Pain. 2004;5:458–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2004.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Sternberg WF, Bailin D, Grant M, Gracely RH. Competition alters the perception of noxious stimuli in male and female athletes. Pain. 1998;76:231–8. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(98)00050-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Sternberg WF, Bokat C, Kass L, Alboyadjian A, Gracely RH. Sex-dependent components of the analgesia produced by athletic competition. J Pain. 2001;2:65–74. doi: 10.1054/jpai.2001.18236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Koltyn KF, Trine MR, Stegner AJ, Tobar DA. Effect of isometric exercise on pain perception and blood pressure in men and women. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2001;33:282–90. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200102000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Fillingim RB, Ness TJ, Glover TL, Campbell CM, Hastie BA, Price DD, Staud R. Morphine responses and experimental pain: sex differences in side effects and cardiovascular responses but not analgesia. J Pain. 2005;6:116–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2004.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Romberg R, Olofsen E, Sarton E, den Hartigh J, Taschner PE, Dahan A. Pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic modeling of morphine-6-glucuronide-induced analgesia in healthy volunteers: absence of sex differences. Anesthesiology. 2004;100:120–33. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200401000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Wasan AD, Davar G, Jamison R. The association between negative affect and opioid analgesia in patients with discogenic low back pain. Pain. 2005;117:450–61. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Pud D, Yarnitsky D, Sprecher E, Rogowski Z, Adler R, Eisenberg E. Can personality traits and gender predict the response to morphine? An experimental cold pain study. Eur J Pain. 2006;10:103–12. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Sarton E, Olofsen E, Romberg R, den Hartigh J, Kest B, Nieuwenhuijs D, Burm A, Teppema L, Dahan A. Sex differences in morphine analgesia: an experimental study in healthy volunteers. Anesthesiology. 2000;93:1245–54. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200011000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Zacny JP. Characterizing the subjective, psychomotor, and physiological effects of a hydrocodone combination product (Hycodan) in non-drug-abusing volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;165:146–56. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1245-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Murthy BR, Pollack GM, Brouwer KL. Contribution of morphine-6-glucuronide to antinociception following intravenous administration of morphine to healthy volunteers. J Clin Pharmacol. 2002;42:569–76. doi: 10.1177/00912700222011508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Olofsen E, Romberg R, Bijl H, Mooren R, Englbers F, Kest B, Dahan A. Alfentanil and placebo analgesia: no sex differences detected in models of experimental pain. Anesthesiology. 2005;103:130–9. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200507000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Gear RW, Gordon NC, Heller PH, Paul S, Miaskowski C, Levine JD. Gender difference in analgesic response to the kappa-opioid pentazocine. Neurosci Lett. 1996;205:207–9. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(96)12402-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Gear RW, Miaskowski C, Gordon NC, Paul SM, Heller PH, Levine JD. Kappa-opioids produce significantly greater analgesia in women than in men. Nat Med. 1996;2:1248–50. doi: 10.1038/nm1196-1248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Gordon NC, Gear RW, Heller PH, Paul S, Miaskowski C, Levine JD. Enhancement of morphine analgesia by the GABAB agonist baclofen. Neuroscience. 1995;69:345–9. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00335-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Fillingim RB, Gear RW. Sex differences in opioid analgesia: clinical and experimental findings. Eur J Pain. 2004;8:413–25. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES