Abstract
Often, people are able to recall a message on a particular topic for a long period of time. These memorable messages have the ability to influence behavior when they are recalled from memory long after initial exposure. Knowing the topics and sources of the messages that are remembered about breast cancer can improve the efficacy of future breast cancer outreach. To this end, 359 women completed an online survey about memorable breast cancer messages. Most women (60%) recalled a memorable message, described it, identified its source, and noted whether it had resulted in prevention or detection behaviors. Four categories of message topics emerged: early detection (37.3%), awareness (30.9%), treatment (25.8%), and prevention (6%)). Furthermore, five categories of sources of these memorable messages were found: (media (35.5%), friends (22.2%), family (21.6%), medical professionals (15.2%), and others (5.5%)). The media were a major source of all four topics of messages although family members, friends, and the medical community were major sources for particular message topics, as well. Memorable messages originating from medical professionals were substantially more likely to motivate detection behaviors than prevention behaviors. This research demonstrates that message topic and source both play roles in determining message recall as well as in determining how memorable messages impacted behavior.
Breast cancer prevention, diagnosis, and treatment comprise some of the most compelling and ubiquitous health issues facing society. Within the United States, most women have exposure to the disease either directly or indirectly (Susan G. Komen for the Cure, 2007). Not surprisingly, messages about breast cancer are also prevalent in society. Messages that motivate women toward behaviors that enhance prevention and (or) increase the likelihood of detection are valuable because they can decrease the occurrence of the disease and save lives.
Protective behaviors of prevention and early detection play an important role in decreasing the incidence of, and mortality from, breast cancer (American Cancer Society, Inc., 2005). Studies demonstrate that exposure to breast cancer messages can influence the degree to which women perform protective breast cancer behaviors (Dalessandri, Cooper, & Rucker, 1998; Earp, Eng, O'Malley, Altpeter, Rauscher, Mayne, et al., 2002; Jones, Denham, & Springston, 2006; Powell, Carter, Bonsi, Johnson, Williams, Taylor-Smith, et al., 2005). However, a message must be recalled in order to influence women's health behaviors after initial exposure (Rimer & Glassman, 1984). Consequently, it is important to determine the characteristics of those breast cancer messages that become memorable.
Although research has examined the different media sources of cancer messages, such as newspaper articles (Moriarty & Stryker, 2007), popular magazines (Dobias, Moyer, McAchran, Katz, & Sonnad, 2001), pamphlets (Kline & Mattson, 2000), as well as interpersonal interaction (Jones et al., 2006), researchers have not examined breast cancer communication within the conceptual framework of memorable messages. Knapp, Stohl, and Reardon (1981) originally defined memorable messages as “verbal messages which may be remembered for extremely long periods of time and which people perceived as a major influence on the course of their lives” (p. 27). Because these messages potentially have long-term impact on the behaviors of receivers, it is important to determine the topics of memorable breast cancer messages and their sources so that future breast cancer messages can be designed and communicated effectively. This study examines the topics of breast cancer memorable messages and their sources and the relationship between those characteristics and women's performance of prevention and detection behaviors.
Memorable Messages
Memorable messages are pervasive. Smith and Ellis (2001) conducted an investigation of memorable messages as guides to behavior, focusing on messages recalled when self-assessing personal behavior that exceeded or fell below personal standards. Almost all of the participants (99%) were able to report messages that they recalled when self-assessing behavior. The behavioral impact of memorable messages has also been studied in final conversations about religious faith (Keeley, 2004), aging (Holladay, 2002), gendered socialization (Dallimore, 2003), and newcomer socialization in organizations (Stohl, 1986).
People receive hundreds, if not thousands, of messages each day, and most of these messages are processed and then released from the short-term memory's cache. Some of these messages, however, are perceived as important units of communication that potentially have a strong effect on behavior and sense-making processes (Holladay, 2002). These messages are consequently stored in long-term memory thus becoming memorable. Knapp et al. (1981) suggest that the reasons people remember these messages involve several characteristics of the message. Messages perceived as memorable are most often brief and prescribe rules of conduct for solving problems that could occur or have occurred.
Breast Cancer
Excluding skin cancer, breast cancer is currently the most commonly diagnosed cancer in women. Statistics suggest that one out of every nine women will develop the disease between the ages of 20 and 80. Approximately 2.3 million women were alive at the beginning of 2002 who had direct experience with breast cancer (American Cancer Society, 2005). An estimated 178,480 American women developed breast cancer and 40,460 women died from the disease in 2007 (American Cancer Society, Inc., 2007a). Breast cancer is second only to lung cancer in cancer deaths among women. Nevertheless, due to better treatment and earlier detection the death rate from breast cancer has decreased since 1990 (American Cancer Society, 2005).
There are four main phases of knowledge and experience with breast cancer. The first is awareness of the disease, followed by active prevention attempts. Next is an effort to detect the disease, which can involve a diagnosis. Finally, if needed, treatment of the disease follows. Although not all women will experience every phase, most will likely be aware of breast cancer due to national campaigns and its prominence in television news broadcasts (Pribble, Goldstien, Fowler, Greenberg, Noel, & Howell, 2006). Additionally, these phases do not necessarily occur in sequence. For example, a woman may be aware of breast cancer and make efforts to detect it, but never try to prevent it. Furthermore, research has identified the different topics of cancer messages women are exposed to: awareness (Papas, Logan, & Tomar, 2004), prevention, detection (Dillard & Nabi, 2006), and treatment (Liang, Burnett, Rowland, Meropol, Eggert, Hwang, et al., 2002), and these topics mirror the phases of breast cancer experience. It is unknown however, what topics of messages become memorable. Therefore, this study is interested in answering the following research question:
RQ1: What topics of breast cancer messages are reported as memorable?
Sources of Memorable Messages
Message source is an important aspect of any message because it affects persuasiveness (Heesacker, Petty, & Cacioppo, 1983). For example, remembered messages typically originate from sources that are higher in status and familiar to the receiver (Dallimore, 2003; Holladay, 2002; Stohl, 1986). Consequently, memorable breast cancer message source credibility differs from person to person based on the recipient's age, education, socioeconomic status (Jones et al., 2006; Rees & Bath, 2000), active or passive health attitudes (Carlsson, 2000), culture (Rajaram & Rashidi, 1999), and health status (Pecchioni & Sparks, 2007). Some research has taken this knowledge and applied it, examining the effectiveness of specific sources for breast cancer messages. For example, although messages about breast cancer awareness, prevention, detection, and treatment are offered frequently through the media, doctors are often claimed to be the best sources of breast cancer messages (Atkin, Smith, Ferguson, & McFeters, 2008). Additionally, a study of cancer patients found that the most common source of information was a doctor, followed by family, nurses, friends, the internet, other medical personnel, and other patients (Pecchioni & Sparks, 2007). Another study conducted to investigate the sources of breast cancer message sources women valued found that doctors were favored, followed by media, family, and friends (Meissner, Potosky, & Convissor, 1992). Family members of cancer patients on the other hand reported being most satisfied with information obtained from the internet (Pecchioni & Sparks, 2007).
Studies have also found that women are likely to seek screening for breast cancer if they are instructed to obtain a screening by a doctor (Clark et al., 2000; O'Malley, Earp, Hawley, Schell, Mathews, & Mitchell, 2001). When a doctor fails to recommend that their patients receive screening for breast cancer or work toward preventing an illness, however, patients turn towards media sources such as celebrities and journalists. For example, Meissner et al., (1992) found that breast cancer patients preferred to get their information from their health care providers at the time of their diagnosis but were often unsatisfied with the information they received. When dissatisfaction occurs, patients turn to media sources that were more readily available to meet their needs (Rees & Bath, 2000). A study of the general population found health care professionals were the most frequently used source of health information followed by the media (O'Malley, Kerner, & Johnson, 1999). Atkin et al. (2008) found that women who used media sources to retrieve information about breast cancer were significantly more likely to have heard of cancer screenings than those women who received most of their information from their doctor.
Patient information seeking tendencies in the face of aversive health information as a method of coping has also been examined to understand the preferred sources for breast cancer messages. Active patients, or monitors (Miller, 1980; 1987), are proactive in their search for breast cancer information whereas passive patients, or blunters (Miller, 1980; 1987), allow the information to come to them. One study asked cancer patients where they turned for medical advice and found that monitors sought information from medical books, narratives, the internet, and telephone hotlines (Pecchioni & Sparks, 2007). Blunters on the other hand, the more common of the two groups, received information about their cancer from the television, radio, newspapers, other patients and friends. Mothers' and daughters' preferred sources for breast cancer messages has also been studied. Jones et al. (2006) found that while mothers tended to turn most often to media sources, daughters tended to turn most often to interpersonal resources like family and friends.
Taken together, these studies suggest breast cancer messages are transmitted by three categories of sources, medical professionals, media sources, and interaction with people personally known by the receiver. Therefore, we offer the following research question:
RQ2: Will the main source categories of memorable breast cancer messages be medical professionals, media sources, and interpersonal relations, including both family and friends?
Behavioral Outcomes
Although it is likely that women receive exposure to several different topics of breast cancer messages from several different sources, it is unknown whether message topic or source is associated with particular behavioral outcomes. One meta-analysis found that mediated health communication campaigns are successful in changing the behavior of about 8% of the target population (Snyder, Hamilton, Mitchell, Kiwanuka-Tondo, Fleming-Milici, & Proctor, 2004). Mullen, Mains, and Valez (1992) reviewed studies of cardiac interventions in clinics and found that exposure to the intervention had a measurable effect on several different behaviors, but that the effect was variable depending on the type of behavior targeted. Furthermore, various studies have shown that mediated messages are effective at modifying abstinence, smoking, seat belt use, and condom use (Kim, Kols, Nyakauru, Marangwanda, & Chibatamoto, 2001; Egger, Fitzgerald, Frape, Monaem, Rubinstein, Tyler, et al.,1983; Wells, Malenfant, Williams, & Van Houten, 2000; Shapiro, Meekers, & Tambashe, 2003).
Findings on the effectiveness of communication campaigns to bring about behavioral outcomes in breast cancer prevention or detection related behaviors are mixed. Although conclusive evidence declaring that certain sources or topics are more likely to change behaviors is elusive, the results of these studies provide a glimpse into this relationship. For example, medical professionals have been found to be the most frequent, valued, and consistent source of health information (O'Malley et al., 1999; Meissner et al., 1992; Pecchioni et al., 2007). The literature is also beginning to show that messages from medical professionals can increase women's prevention and detection behaviors. One study found that a phone call from a nurse increased mammography use 5-fold when compared to a control group who received only a pamphlet (Dalessandri et al., 1998). Additionally, Strickland, Feigl, Upchurch, King, Pierce, Grevstad, et al. (1997) reported on an intervention that tested the effects of receiving one, two, or all of the following messages: physician messages, a class on breast self-examination, and message reinforcement through a postcard and a phone call. Women who received all three messages reported engaging in the most breast self-examinations. There were reported increases of breast self-examination across all levels of the intervention, however.
Like messages originating from medical professionals, research indicates that some messages from media sources are more effective at increasing breast cancer prevention and detection behaviors than others. One study reviewed 15 media campaigns to promote physical activity, which aids breast cancer prevention, and found that although the majority were recalled, there was little or no influence on behaviors (Cavill & Bauman, 2004). Another study of a statewide media campaign revealed that the messages might have had adverse effects on women's breast cancer detection behaviors (McCaul, Jacobson, & Martinson, 1998). Other studies, however, have recorded significant increases in the desired behavioral outcomes. Media in China used two public service announcements to increase detection behaviors and found that they were effective in improving knowledge of breast health, knowledge of breast self-exams, and improving breast health practices (Sun, Zhang, Tsoh, Wong-Kim, & Chow, 2007). Additionally, a study of culturally tailored messages in magazines increased both breast cancer prevention (eating fruits and vegetables) and detection (mammography) behaviors in women (Kreuter, Sugg-Skinner, Holt, Clark, Haire-Joshu, Fu, et al., 2005).
Finally, interpersonal sources are known to affect breast cancer prevention and detection behaviors as well. One study found that dialogue with friends and relatives increased the chances that African American women would seek screening for breast cancer (Husaini, Sherkat, Bragg, Levine, Emerson, Mentes, et al., 2001). Another study using a telephone intervention strategy in which women contacted friends and encouraged them to have a mammogram showed that these interpersonal contacts increased mammography usage when compared to a control group (Calle, Miracle-McMahill, Moss, & Heath, 1994). Similarly, Jones, Denham, and Springston (2006) found that discussion with relatives about breast cancer led to higher numbers of breast self-exams performed by college-aged women.
These studies demonstrate that although the effect is not consistent, messages can influence the performance of prevention and detection behaviors. Therefore, we pose the following research question:
RQ3: What is the relationship between different topics and sources of memorable messages and the enactment of breast cancer prevention and detection behaviors?
Method
Participants
Participants were recruited from breast cancer awareness organizations, advocacy groups, a large Midwestern university and a medium sized Western university in the United States. In sum, 359 female respondents completed the survey. Participants ranged from 18 to 85 years old. Caucasians composed the majority of the sample, 85%. African Americans (4%), Latinos (3%), Asian Americans (2%), Native Americans (2%), Pacific Islanders (3%), and those choosing not to respond (1%) composed the remainder of the sample. When participants were asked about their highest level of education, 11% reported a high school degree or less, 35% participants completed some college, technical school, or earned an associates degree, 32% earned a bachelor's degree, and 22% had graduate schooling.
Instrumentation and Procedure
Participants completed a web-based survey questionnaire. The beginning of the survey gave the definition of a memorable message provided previously and asked participants if they could recall a memorable message about breast cancer. If the participant did have a memorable message (60%), a series of open-ended questions asked about personal experience with breast cancer and characteristics of, as well as responses to, their memorable message.
The topics of memorable messages were coded reliably (Cohen's Kappa = .93) by two trained coders into 16 subcategories that formed four second order categories also determined by the two coders. The first category, breast cancer awareness, included the subcategories of statistics/facts, breast cancer prevalence, and campaigns/organizations. The second category of prevention included the subcategories of health/be proactive, improve/change a health behavior, and family risk. Third, the category of detection included the subcategories of breast self-exam, mammograms, breast self-exam and mammograms, and early detection. The fourth category was treatment, which included the subcategories of appearance, choices, survival choices, social support, womanhood, fear/pain/negative, and appreciation for everyday life.
Message sources were coded into four main categories (family, friend, media, medical professional) from the 17 categories that participants identified (Cohen's Kappa = 1.0). Mother, sister, grandmother, grandfather, father, husband, aunt, cousin, and child composed the family category. Friend was classified into its own category. The media source category included celebrities, journalists, and other sources found in the media. Doctors and nurses were classified as medical professionals. Finally, an “other” category was formed by collapsing together teacher, other authority figure, and other responses that did not fit into one of the four main categories.
Results
Topics of Memorable Messages
The largest proportion of memorable breast cancer messages were categorized as detection messages, 37%. Exemplars from this category were, “Do a self breast examination every month,” “Don't put off having a mammogram,” “Early detection saves lives,” and “Don't forget to do self breast exams and get yearly mammograms.” The second most frequently recalled memorable message was the breast cancer awareness category, 31%. Messages in this category focused mainly on statistics such as “Over 40,000 women a year die from breast cancer,” or major breast cancer organizations such as “The pink ribbon.” Memorable messages with a treatment focus made up 26% of the messages. These messages focused on treatment outcomes such as “I would rather have lost a breast than my life, I am lucky I caught it early enough to not have it take the rest of me…if it comes down to the choice, it's just cosmetics.” An example of treatment choices was “The second time she was diagnosed it was even more severe than the first, but she refused to just die and continued all the treatments available.” Treatment messages also included experimental therapy such as “She refused to simply lie down and let cancer conquer her,” and fear of treatment “Before going into surgery, my mother in law said, 'I was so frightened that all I could do was cry.' Afterwards she often commented that her scar was so tender that it hurt to have the sheet touch where the incision was.” Finally, 6% of reported memorable messages fell under the category of prevention. Messages in the prevention category focused on the importance of being proactive about health. They included: “We must stop the silence that exists in the African American community because it's important that we know our family medical history in order to be proactive with our health,” and the importance of healthy behavior such as “What you eat, drink, put on your skin and breath[e] can cause cancer.” Therefore, the answer to RQ1 is that the topics of memorable messages most often reported were detection and awareness, followed by treatment and prevention of breast cancer.
Sources of Memorable Messages
Examination of data for the most frequent sources of memorable breast cancer messages reveal that the media were the primary source of these messages. Of all the memorable messages recalled by the participants, 36% were from the media. Memorable messages originating from friends (23%) and family members (21%) were the second and third most common, respectively. In a finding that might be surprising to some, health care professionals were the source of only 15% of the memorable messages. Six percent of the memorable breast cancer messages were from sources that did not fit into any of the other categories. Therefore, the answer to RQ2 is that media, and interpersonal sources such as friends and family members, and medical professionals, respectively, were the main sources of breast cancer memorable messages.
Relationship between Topic and Source
Of the breast cancer awareness messages, 55% were from the media, 23% were from a friend, 9% were from a health care professional, and 6% were from a family member (7% were from another source). Of those messages that were about prevention, 46% were from a family member, 31% were from the media, 15% were from a health care professional, and 8% was from a friend. Of the detection focused memorable messages, 30% were from a family member, 25% were from the media, 25% were from a health care professional, 15% were from a friend, and 5% were from an other source. Of the messages that were about treatment, 37% were from a friend, 30% were from the media, 20% were from a family member, 7% were from a health care professional, and 6% were from another source. Table 1 presents the topics of memorable messages and their sources both as a percent of all messages and in terms of the percentages within topic of message.
Table 1.
Percentages of Topics of Memorable Messages and Their Sources
Memorable Messages |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Source | BC Awareness | Prevention | Detection | Treatment | Total |
Family | 1.9 (6%) | 2.8 (46%) | 11.3 (30%) | 5.2 (20%) | 21.1 |
Media | 16.9 (55%) | 1.9 (31%) | 9.4 (25%) | 7.4 (29%) | 35.7 |
Health Care | 2.8 (9%) | 0.9 (15%) | 9.4 (25%) | 1.9 (8%) | 15 |
Friend | 7 (23%) | 0.5 (8%) | 5.6 (15%) | 9.4 (37%) | 22.5 |
Other | 2.3 (7%) | 0.0 (0%) | 1.9 (5%) | 1.4 (6%) | 5.6 |
Total | 31% | 6.1% | 37.6 | 25.4 | 100 |
Note: N = 213, BC = Breast Cancer, Health Care = Health Care Professional, Percentages that come first are percentage of total and percentages in parentheses are percent of message topic category.
Relationship between Memorable Messages and Behaviors
In order to assess the relationship between memorable messages and behaviors, respondents indicated whether their memorable message motivated them to engage in breast cancer detection behaviors and (or) breast cancer prevention behaviors. Responses to the questions, “Did you engage in breast self exams?” and “Did you engage in mammograms?” had a Pearson correlation of .52 (n = 214, p < .05) and were summed to form a measure of detection behaviors that ranged from zero to two. Responses to the questions, “Did you engage in eating healthy food?” and “Did you engage in exercise?” had a Pearson correlation of .71 (n = 210, p < .05). Summing responses to these two questions formed a measure of prevention behaviors that ranged from zero to two. Although the data are categorical, this procedure is meaningful because the categories differ in magnitude rather than quality. Consequently, the Pearson correlation coefficient as employed here can be interpreted in the usual manner.
Analyses focusing on the relationship between memorable message source and reported behavioral outcomes revealed that memorable messages sent by medical professionals were associated with the topics of behaviors reported by participants. Specifically, messages sent by medical professionals were less likely to be associated with prevention behaviors and more likely to be associated with detection behaviors. Using Pearson's r as a measure of effect size revealed a positive but statistically non-significant difference indicating that there was a trend for messages sent by medical professionals to result in more detection behaviors than messages sent by other message sources, r = .11, n = 212, p = .11, P(-.02 ≤ ρ ≤ .24). On the other hand, memorable messages sent by a medical professional resulted in significantly less prevention behavior than messages sent by other message sources (r = -.16, n = 210, p < .05, P(-.29 ≤ ρ ≤ -.03). Further examination of the .95 confidence intervals of these two correlations indicated that their difference is statistically significant. Thus, memorable messages originating from medical professionals were substantially more likely to be associated with subsequent detection behaviors rather than prevention behaviors. Figure 1 illustrates this effect. Simply put, the answer to RQ3 is that all topics of memorable messages about breast cancer were equally likely to motivate prevention and detection behaviors, as were all sources except medical professionals. Messages sent by medical professionals were less likely to promote prevention behaviors.
Figure 1.
Medical professional's memorable message source relationship with detection and prevention behaviors.
Discussion
Breast cancer is a major health issue in the United States, and most women are exposed to the disease directly or indirectly. Breast cancer messages are also prevalent in society. Messages that motivate women toward prevention and detection behaviors are valuable because they have the power to decrease the occurrence of the disease and save lives. The data from this study indicates that memorable messages about breast cancer are associated with women's engagement in both breast cancer prevention and detection behaviors. This study also found that memorable breast cancer messages have four main message topic and sources respectively.
Categories of memorable breast cancer messages included detection, awareness, treatment, and prevention of the disease. The most frequently recalled memorable messages related to detection and the least recalled were messages about prevention. The relative paucity of prevention messages is consistent with several facts. Although about seventy percent of women in the United States in 2005 reported in engaging in the detection behavior of getting a mammogram within the previous two years (American Cancer Society, Inc., 2007b), 52% of adult females in the United States are overweight or obese (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2006). This is a largely preventable condition associated with increased risk for breast cancer (American Cancer Society, Inc., 2006). In other words, women are both less likely to recall messages associated with prevention, and they are less likely to enact certain prevention behaviors. Ironically, prevention efforts have been found to have a wider array of benefits compared to early detection behaviors (Kaplan, 2000).
Perhaps features of preventative messages are not as memorable as other topics. Possibly selective attention plays a role because preventative behaviors are not as readily performed as detection behaviors. For example, although inconvenient, breast self-exams and mammograms are simple and can be performed at specific time intervals. On the other hand, engaging in a regular exercise and healthy diet regimen requires more time and resources. Consequently, it is possible that less cognitive capital is allocated to prevention messages than detection messages and women do not store prevention messages in long-term memory as readily. Additionally, these acts apply to many diseases besides breast cancer and therefore may not be recalled for this specific disease. More research is necessary in order to investigate these possibilities and design messages to overcome these potential barriers to prevention behaviors.
Four primary sources of breast cancer memorable messages that are similar to previous research on typical messages that are sent to women were also found: health care professionals, media sources, friends, and family. Media sources like celebrities and journalists were sources of the most memorable breast cancer messages, yet as numerous studies suggest media sourced messages often present incorrect or incomplete information about health topics (Dornbusch, 1998; MacKenzie, Chapman, Barratt & Holding, 2007; Moynihan, Bero, Ross-Degnan, Henry, Lee, Watkins, et al., 2000; Nakahara, Ichikawa, & Wakai, 2007; Pruitta & Mullen, 2005). For example, media sources have been found to be responsible for over-publicizing the role of genetic factors in women's risk for breast cancer, when they account for 5 to 10 percent of a woman's risk (Jones et al., 2006; Henderson & Kitzinger, 1999; Sagi, Kadur, & Zlotogora, & Peretz, 1998; American Cancer Society, Inc., 2006). The inflation of genetic factors has been observed particularly with certain ethnic minorities resulting in minority “ownership” of diseases like breast cancer (Donelle, Hoffman-Goetz, & Clarke, 2005). Moreover, other work has found that although media sources do an excellent job of disseminating information, the influence they have on behavior is much weaker (Chaffee, 1982); this finding should concern the health communicator who is interested in activating behavior.
In contrast to messages from media sources, interpersonal communication has been found to have a stronger effect on actual health behavior change than the media (Dutta-Bergman, 2004). Importantly, these data indicate that when considered together, messages from family and friends account for more breast cancer memorable messages than media sources, representing a great opportunity to transmit messages that increase both preventative and detection behaviors.
Surprisingly, memorable messages originating from medical professionals were the least frequently recalled. Previous research indicated that doctors are the primary source of medical information (Pecchioni & Sparks, 2007; O'Malley et al., 2001; Rees & Bath, 2000; Meissner et al., 1992). Possibly the high stress, anxiety-provoking nature of the physicians' office visit creates a situation that inhibits storage of the medical professional's message into memory. However, we cannot discount the need for improved doctor-patient communication identified in several studies (Meissner et al., 1992). Clearly, despite the fact the memorable messages originating from medical professionals were the third most frequently recalled, messages from medical professionals are exceedingly important.
Indeed, messages from medical professionals were found to have the largest impact on women's reported behaviors, specifically increased engagement in detection behaviors and decreased engagement in prevention behaviors. These findings suggest that the medical profession needs to encourage patients pervasively and persuasively to perform prevention behaviors. The observed effect is possibly due to a side effect of a health system focused primarily on detecting and treating diseases rather than working to prevent the disease from occurring. Perhaps this effect can be counteracted by regular visits with a primary care physician. Research has shown that participating in regular visits with a consistent primary care physician can triple a patient's likelihood of receiving preventative care information (Ettner, 1999). Most women do regularly visit a primary care physician and these visits tend to increase as they get older (U.S Department of Health and Human Services, 2001). This solution may not be possible for many Americans, however, as over 40 million (nearly 15 %) do not have health insurance according to 2004 estimates by the U.S Census Bureau (2004). People in these low resource groups may only visit a medical professional when a problem is manifest.
Although this study produced some interesting findings, it is not without limitations. The method by which participants listed the source of their memorable message allowed participants to select more than one source per message. To prevent double counting, the first source that respondents reported was considered the source of the memorable message. This decision forced respondents to report one source even though some messages might be memorable because they were heard from multiple sources. Additionally, participants did not receive direction to recall their memorable messages verbatim. Finally, this study was based on a snowball sample comprised mostly of Caucasian females with a bachelor's degree or higher level of education. Although the demographic of this sample does include diverse characteristics, it is restricted particularly on the education variable; therefore, the results may not be generalizeable to less educated members of the general population.
Conclusion
This study was designed to determine the sources and topics of memorable breast cancer messages. The results reveal that although messages regarding breast cancer awareness, treatment, detection and prevention that are sent by the media as well as friends and family are the most likely to be remembered, messages sent by medical professionals had the most impact on behaviors. This study leaves future researchers with at least two immediate directions for future research. First, this study identifies several common topics and sources of memorable breast cancer messages. Research exists that demonstrates the power of memorable messages to influence behavior. What the previous literature did not reveal, however, is how the different topics of memorable breast cancer messages identified in this study work to influence the behaviors of those who recall the message. By understanding how and why the messages influence behavior the second direction of future research can be followed. Specifically, by knowing the processes behind the relationship between memorable breast cancer message recall and behavior change, it might become evident that some messages influence behavior more strongly than others depending on situational features such as the source of the message. Health communicators could use this information to intertwine messages topics and sources into breast cancer messages in an attempt to influence at-risk populations effectively. Additionally, future research could perform laboratory experiments using detection and prevention messages to see which messages are most readily recalled to attempt to explain further the paucity of prevention memorable breast cancer messages reported.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by the Breast Cancer and the Environment Research Centers grant number U01 ES012800 from the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (NIEHS), and the National Cancer Institute (NCI), NIH, DHHS. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the NIEHS or NCI, NIH.
References
- American Cancer Society, Inc. Breast Cancer Facts & Figures 2005-2006. 2005 Retrieved June 8, 2007 from http://www.cancer.org/downloads/STT/CAFF2005BrF.pdf.
- American Cancer Society, Inc. What are the risk factors for breast cancer? 2006 Retrieved June 21, 2007 from http://www.cancer.org/docroot/CRI/content/CRI_2_4_2X_What_are_the_risk_factors_fo r_breast_cancer_5.asp?
- American Cancer Society, Inc. Cancer Facts & Figures 2007. 2007a Retrieved August 4, 2007 from http://www.cancer.org/downloads/STT/CAFF2007PWSecured.pdf.
- American Cancer Society, Inc. Mammography rates inch downward. 2007b Retrieved June 21, 2007 from http://www.cancer.org/docroot/NWS/content/NWS_1_1x_Mammography_Rates_Inch_D ownward.asp.
- Atkin CK, Smith SW, Ferguson V, McFeters C. A comprehensive analysis of breast cancer news coverage in leading media outlets. Journal of Health Communication. 2008 doi: 10.1080/10810730701806912. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calle EE, Miracle-McMahill HL, Moss RE, Heath CW., Jr Personal contact from friends to increase mammography usage. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 1994;10:361–366. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlsson M. Cancer patients seeking information from sources outside the health care system. Support Care Cancer. 2000;8:453–457. doi: 10.1007/s005200000166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavill N, Bauman A. Changing the way people think about health-enhancing physical activity: Do mass media campaigns have a role? Journal of Sports Sciences. 2004;22:771–790. doi: 10.1080/02640410410001712467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaffee SH. Mass media and interpersonal channels: Competitive, convergent, or complimentary? In: Gumpert G, Cathart R, editors. Inter/Media: Interpersonal communication in a media world. Oxford University Press; New York, NY: 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Clark S, et al. Patient motivation, satisfaction, and coping in genetic counseling and testing for BRCA 1 and BRCA2. Journal of Genetic Counseling. 2000;9:219–235. doi: 10.1023/A:1009463905057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalessandri KM, Cooper M, Rucker T. Mammography outreach in women veterans. Western Journal of Medicine. 1998;169:150–152. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dallimore EJ. Memorable messages as discursive formations: The gendered socialization of new university faculty. Women's Studies in Communication. 2003;26:214–265. [Google Scholar]
- Dillard JP, Nabi RL. The persuasive influence of emotion in cancer prevention and detection messages. Journal of Communication. 2006;56:S123–S139. [Google Scholar]
- Dobias KS, Moyer CA, McAchran SE, Katz SJ, Sonnad SS. Mammography messages in popular media: Implications for patient expectations and shared clinical decision-making. Health Expectations. 2001;4:131–139. doi: 10.1046/j.1369-6513.2001.00120.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donelle L, Hoffman-Goetz L, Clarke JN. Ethnicity, genetics, and breast cancer: Media portrayal of disease identities. Ethnicity and Health. 2005;10:185–197. doi: 10.1080/13557850500120751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dornbusch D. An analysis of media coverage of the BSE crisis in Britain. In: Ratzan SC, editor. The mad cow crisis: Health and the public good. Routledge; London: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Dutta-Bergman MJ. Primary sources of health information: Comparisons in the domain of health attitudes, health cognitions, and health behaviors. Health Communication. 2004;16:273–288. doi: 10.1207/S15327027HC1603_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Earp J, Eng E, O'Malley MS, Altpeter M, Rauscher G, Mayne L, et al. Increasing use of mammography among older, rural African American women: Results from a community trial. American Journal of Public Health. 2002;92:646–654. doi: 10.2105/ajph.92.4.646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egger G, Fitzgerald W, Frape G, Monaem A, Rubinstein P, Tyler C, et al. Results of large scale media antismoking campaign in Australia: North Coast “Quit for Life” programme. British Medical Journal. 1983;287:1125–1128. doi: 10.1136/bmj.287.6399.1125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ettner S, Ettner L. The relationship between continuity of care and the health behavivors of patients: Does having a usual physician make a difference? Medical Care. 1999;37:547–555. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199906000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heesacker M, Petty RE, Cacioppo JT. Field dependence and attitude change: source credibility can alter persuasion by affecting message relevant thinking. Journal of Personality. 1983;51:653–666. [Google Scholar]
- Henderson L, Kitzinger J. The human drama of genetics. “Hard” and “soft” media representations of inherited breast cancer. Sociology of Health and Illness. 1999;21:560–578. [Google Scholar]
- Holladay SJ. “Have fun while you can, “You're only as old as you feel,” and “Don't ever get old!:” An examination of memorable messages about aging. Journal of Communication. 2002;52:681–697. [Google Scholar]
- Husaini BA, Sherkat DE, Bragg R, Levine R, Emerson JS, Mentes CM, et al. Predictors of breast cancer screening in a panel study of African American women. Women & Health. 2001;34:35–51. doi: 10.1300/J013v34n03_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones KO, Denham BE, Springston JK. Effects of mass and interpersonal communication on breast cancer screening: Advancing agenda-setting theory in health contexts. Journal of Applied Communication Research. 2006;34:94–113. [Google Scholar]
- Jones LW, Sinclair RC, Courneya KS. The effects of source credibility and message framing on exercise intentions, behaviors, and attitudes: An integration of the Elaboration Likelihood Model and Prospect Theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2003;33:179–196. [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan RM. Two pathways to prevention. American Psychologist. 2000;55:382–386. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim YM, Kols A, Nyakauru R, Marangwanda C, Chibatamoto P. Promoting sexual responsibility among young people in Zimbabwe. International Family Planning Perspectives. 2001;27:11–19. [Google Scholar]
- Keeley M. Final conversations: survivors' memorable messages concerning Religious faith and spirituality. Health Communication. 2004;16:87–104. doi: 10.1207/S15327027HC1601_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kline KM, Mattson M. Breast self-examination pamphlets: A content analysis grounded in fear appeal research. Health Communication. 2000;12:1–21. doi: 10.1207/S15327027HC1201_01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knapp ML, Stohl C, Reardon KK. “Memorable” messages. Journal of Communication. 1981;31:27–41. [Google Scholar]
- Kreuter MW, Sugg-Skinner C, Holt CL, Clark EM, Haire-Joshu D, Fu O, et al. Cultural tailoring for mammography and fruit and vegetable intake among low-income African-American women in urban public health centers. Preventive Medicine. 2005;41:53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liang W, Burnett CB, Rowland JH, Meropol NJ, Eggert L, Hwang Y, et al. Communication between physicians and older women with localized breast cancer: Implications for treatment and patient satisfaction. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2002;20:1008–1016. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2002.20.4.1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKenzie R, Chapman S, Barratt A, Holding S. “The news is [not] all good”: misrepresentations and inaccuracies in Australian news media reports on prostate cancer screening. The Medical Journal of Australia. 2007;187:507–510. doi: 10.5694/j.1326-5377.2007.tb01391.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCaul KD, Jacobson K, Martinson B. The effects of a state-wide media campaign on mammography screening. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1998;28:504–515. [Google Scholar]
- Miller SM. When is a little information a dangerous thing?: Coping with stressful life-events by monitoring vs. blunting. In: Levine S, Ursin H, editors. Coping and health. Plenum Press; New York, NY: 1980. pp. 145–169. [Google Scholar]
- Miller SM. Monitoring and blunting: Validation of a questionnaire to assess styles of information seeking under threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;52:345–353. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.52.2.345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meissner HI, Potosky AL, Convissor R. How sources of health information relate to knowledge and use of cancer screening exams. Journal of Community Health. 1992;17:153–165. doi: 10.1007/BF01324404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyerowitz BE, Chaiken S. The effect of message framing on breast self exam attitudes, intentions, and behavior. Journal of Personality Social Psychology. 1982;52:500–510. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.52.3.500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moriarty CM, Stryker JE. Prevention and screening efficacy messages in newspaper accounts of cancer. Health Education Research. 2007 doi: 10.1093/her/cyl163. Retrieved June 6, 2007 from http://her.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/reprint/cyl163v1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moynihan R, Bero L, Ross-Degnan D, Henry D, Lee K, Watkins J, et al. Coverage by the news media of the benefits and risks of medications. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2000;342:1645–1650. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200006013422206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullen PD, Mains DA, Velez R. A meta-analysis of controlled trials of cardiac patient education. Patient Education and Counseling. 1992;19:143–162. doi: 10.1016/0738-3991(92)90194-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakahara S, Ichikawa M, Wakai S. Magazine information on safety belt use for pregnant women and young children. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2007;39:356–363. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2006.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Malley AS, Kerner JF, Johnson L. Are we getting the message out to all? Health information sources and ethnicity. American Journal of Preventative Medicine. 1999;17:198–202. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(99)00067-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Malley MS, Earp JAL, Hawley ST, Schell MJ, Mathews HF, Mitchell J. The association of race/ethnicity, socio-economic status, and physician recommendation of mammogram: Who gets the message about breast cancer screening. American Journal of Public Health. 2001;91:49–54. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.1.49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papas RK, Logan HL, Tomar SL. Effectiveness of a community-based oral cancer awareness campaign. Cancer Causes and Control. 2004;15:121–131. doi: 10.1023/B:CACO.0000019474.98238.35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pecchioni LL, Sparks L. Health information sources of individuals with cancer and their family members. Health Communication. 2007;21:143–151. doi: 10.1080/10410230701307162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitts V. Illness and internet empowerment: Writing and reading breast cancer in cyberspace. Health. 2004;8:33–59. doi: 10.1177/1363459304038794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powell ME, Carter V, Bonsi E, Johnson G, Williams L, Taylor-Smith L, et al. Increasing mammography screening among African American women in rural areas. Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved. 2005;16:11–21. doi: 10.1353/hpu.2005.0129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pribble JM, Goldstien KM, Fowler EF, Greenberg MJ, Noel SK, Howell JD. Medical news for the public to use? What's on local TV News. American Journal of Managed Care. 2006;12:170–176. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pruitta SL, Mullen PD. Contraception or abortion?: Inaccurate descriptions of emergency contraception in newspaper articles, 1992-2002. Contraception. 2005;71:14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.contraception.2004.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rajaram S, Rashidi A. Asian-Islamic women and breast cancer screening: A socio-cultural analysis. Women and Health. 1999;28:45–58. doi: 10.1300/J013v28n03_04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rees CE, Bath PA. The information needs and source preferences of women with breast cancer and their family members: A review of the literature published between 1988 and 1989. Journal of Advanced Nursing. 2000;31:833–841. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2648.2000.01341.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rimer B, Glassman B. How do persuasive health messages work?: A health education field study. Health Education Behavior. 1984;11:313–321. doi: 10.1177/109019818401100307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sagi M, Kadur L, Zlotogora J, Peretz T. The effect of genetic counseling on knowledge and perceptions regarding risk for breast cancer. Journal of Genetic Counseling. 1998;7:417–434. doi: 10.1023/A:1022880831996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro D, Meekers D, Tambashe B. Exposure to the 'SIDA dans la Cité' AIDS prevention television series in Côte' d'Ivoire, sexual risk behaviour and condom use. AIDS Care. 2003;15:303–314. doi: 10.1080/0954012031000105360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SW, Ellis JB. Memorable messages as guides to self-assessment of behavior: An initial investigation. Communication Monographs. 2001;68:154–168. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder LB, Hamilton MA, Mitchell EW, Kiwanuka-Tondo J, Fleming-Milici F, Proctor D. A meta-analysis of the effect of mediated health communication campaigns on behavior change in the United States. Journal of Health Communication. 2004;9:71–96. doi: 10.1080/10810730490271548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stohl C. The role of memorable messages in the process of organizational socialization. Communication Quarterly. 1986;34:231–249. [Google Scholar]
- Strickland CJ, Feigl P, Upchurch C, King DK, Pierce I, Grevstad PK, et al. Improving breast self-examination compliance: A Southwest Oncology Group randomized trial of three interventions. Preventive Medicine. 1997;26:320–332. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1997.0147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun A, Zhang J, Tsoh J, Wong-Kim E, Chow E. The effectiveness in utilizing Chinese media to promote breast health among Chinese women. Journal of Health Communication. 2007;12:157–171. doi: 10.1080/10810730601150106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Susan G. Komen for the Cure 2007. Retrieved September 27, 2007 from http://www.komen.org/stellent/groups/public/
- U.S. Census Bureau 2004. Table 1: Published and revised estimates of person without health insurance: 2004. Current population survey, 2005. Annual social and economic supplement. U.S Census Bureau. Retrieved August 18, 2007 from http://www.census.gov/hhes/www/hlthins/usernote/usernote3-21rev.html.
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Women's health USA 2006. 2006 Retrieved June 11, 2007 from http://mchb.hrsa.gov/whusa_06/pages/introduction.htm.
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Utilization of ambulatory medical care by women: United States, 1997-1998. 2001 Vital and health statistics. Series 13, Number 149. Data from the National Health Care Survey Retrieved August 18, 2007 from http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/series/sr_13/sr13_149.pdf: Brett, K. M., & Burt, C. W.
- Wells JK, Malenfant JEL, Williams AF, Houten R. Use of a community program to increase seat belt use among shopping center patrons in Charlotte, North Carolina. Journal of Safety Research. 2000;31:93–99. [Google Scholar]