Abstract
Nutrition is known to interact with genotype in human metabolic syndromes, obesity, and diabetes, and also in Drosophila metabolism. Plasticity in metabolic responses, such as changes in body fat or blood sugar in response to changes in dietary alterations, may also be affected by genotype. Here we show that variants of the foraging (for) gene in Drosophila melanogaster affect the response to food deprivation in a large suite of adult phenotypes by measuring gene by environment interactions (GEI) in a suite of food-related traits. for affects body fat, carbohydrates, food-leaving behavior, metabolite, and gene expression levels in response to food deprivation. This results in broad patterns of metabolic, genomic, and behavioral gene by environment interactions (GEI), in part by interaction with the insulin signaling pathway. Our results show that a single gene that varies in nature can have far reaching effects on behavior and metabolism by acting through multiple other genes and pathways.
Author Summary
Normal individual differences in the foraging (for) gene of the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster result in two behavioral types called rover and sitter. Larval rovers show a greater behavioral response to changes in their food environment than sitters. The for gene makes an enzyme called PKG, which is found in the head of the fly, as well as in most other organisms, including humans. Here, we demonstrate that adult rovers and sitters differ in their metabolic response to changes in their food environment. We measure metabolites in rovers and sitters and show that rovers store energy predominantly as lipids, whereas sitters store it as carbohydrates. We also examine expression levels of genes in rover and sitter heads when the flies are well-fed or food-deprived. We find that for affects levels of gene products involved in carbohydrate and fat metabolism and insulin signaling. We confirm an interaction between for and insulin signaling genes by using genetic mutants to measure their combined effects on fly food-leaving behavior. Our results show that natural variation in this single gene can affect plasticity of large numbers of traits.
Introduction
The question of how phenotypic plasticity evolves has been the subject of vigorous debate (reviewed in [1],[2],[3]), as has the related question of whether allelic variation in single genes can have large impacts on plasticity [3]. Phenotypic plasticity is defined as the degree to which the environment can change or modify the phenotype. Genotype-environment interaction (GEI) is genetic variation in phenotypic plasticity. The genetic variation in GEI is needed for the evolution of an adaptive level of phenotypic plasticity [4]. Here we abbreviate “the phenotypic plasticity of one genotype in different environments” to “plasticity”. Recent studies show that quantitative trait loci can have large effects on GEI [5] and that traits with GEI responses to nutrition can be correlated with GEI in the expression of a relatively small number of genes [6].
In this paper, we examine GEIs resulting from variation in a single gene, in response to food deprivation. We begin the process of determining the mechanism by which alleles of this gene affect plasticity. By quantifying the proportion of a large number of gene expression and metabolite traits in which the gene is involved in GEI, we also provide experimental data on the extent of the gene's pleiotropy and its allelic contributions to plasticity.
The foraging (for) gene of D. melanogaster encodes a cGMP dependent protein kinase [7],[8]. Naturally occurring for alleles give rise to the rover and sitter behavioural morphs. As larvae, rovers move more and feed less in the presence of food than sitters, but don't differ in locomotion in the absence of food [9],[10],[11]. Food deprivation causes larval rovers to behave more like sitters [12]. Like their larval counterparts, adult rovers and sitters also differ in food-related behaviours. The sucrose response of rovers in a proboscis extension assay is higher than in sitters and the patterns of walking after feeding on a sucrose drop in sitters exhibits higher turning rates than in rovers [13],[14],[15].
In the present study, we investigate the response of adult rovers and sitters to well-fed (Fed) and food deprived (FD) conditions. Through global profiling of gene expression and metabolites, we find that rovers have greater changes in gene expression profiles and metabolite levels in response to food deprivation than do sitters, and that the insulin pathway is required for this rover-sitter difference. Allelic variation in for also influences the allocation of energy stores to lipids as compared to carbohydrates in fed flies. We conclude that allelic variation in for has a major effect on multiple aspects of food-related plasticity and GEI.
Results
We performed behavioural assays and gene expression profiling on flies from natural rover (forR) and sitter (fors) strains and from a sitter mutant strain (fors2) generated on a rover (forR) genetic background [9],[13]. We exposed these flies to well-fed (Fed) or food deprived conditions (FD) (see methods). To contain cost, metabolite profiling was limited to the forR and fors2 strains.
For behaviour, metabolite and gene expression measures we calculate I, the difference between rover and sitter plasticity, as I = (fed rovers – FD rovers)−(fed sitters – FD sitters); that is, I is the difference between rover response to food and sitter response (for cases when two sitter strains are used, see Statistical Methods). I can be measured for behavioural, metabolic, and gene expression traits, and gives useful information about GEI. I is proportional to the Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) interaction term used to calculate the significance of GEI. Thus, the “direction of GEI”, refers to the sign of I.
Behaviour
The food-leaving assay measures the proportion of flies that traverse a maze after leaving a vial containing sucrose in agar (see Methods and Figure S1). This “food-leaving” behaviour shows significant GEI interaction (Figure 1). To determine how general this pattern of GEI is we repeated these experiments rearing flies on a variety of different food media and in all cases we found significant and similar patterns in GEI (Table S1 for descriptions of food media and statistical tables). Specifically, rover scores increase more from FD to Fed environments than sitter scores. The direction of the GEI is positive (I>0) in all cases.
Metabolites
We examined Fed and FD forR and fors2 heads to determine compounds most strongly associated with the for response to feeding state, using Fourier Transform Ion Cyclotron Resonance Mass Spectroscopy (FTICR MS, Methods) to detect 750 putative metabolites. We found a significant influence of for (forR or fors2) and feeding state on compounds with the molecular weight (MW) and chemical properties of triacylglycerols (TAG) and polysaccharides (PS) (Figure 2A and 2B; Table S2A, S2B). There was a significant main effect of for in carbohydrates, and significant GEI in both carbohydrates and lipids, but in opposite directions (I>0 for lipids, I<0 for carbohydrates). The largest differences were found in smaller MW compounds. Incorporating MW into ANOVA of TAG compounds gave p(for×food) = 1.3·10−13 and p(for×MW) = 1.9·10−5; PS compounds had p(for×food×MW) = 0.03 (Table S2B). (Note that the term food in the ANOVA describes the feeding state of Fed vs. FD and the term for refers to the strains forR or fors2). Thus, GEI interactions are found for metabolites but their direction I depends on metabolite type. Rovers have a larger drop in lipids than sitters in the change from Fed to FD, while sitters have a greater drop in carbohydrates than rovers.
Whole-fly spectrophotometric measures of total carbohydrates, lipids, and proteins (Methods) showed that adult rovers had almost twice as much energy stored in whole-body lipid and about half the energy stored in carbohydrates compared to adult sitters, whereas protein levels normalized to dry weight were not significantly different between genotypes (Figure 3 gives full statistics). Thus, for genotype strongly affects energy storage strategies. A main effect of genotype in fed flies is consistent with an allocation shift between storage of energy as lipids and as carbohydrates.
GEI in gene expression depends on metabolic role
If for affects both behavioural and metabolic GEI and plasticity in a food-dependent manner, how is this reflected at the level of gene expression? To examine for's effect on transcript levels we performed whole-genome microarray analysis on heads of rovers and sitters and sitter mutants under Fed and FD conditions (Methods). Array results were verified using qRTPCR on two genes with strong rover-sitter differences and involved in carbohydrate metabolism [Treh, trehalase, and CG10924, human homolog is PCK1 phosphoenolpyruvate carboxykinase 1 (soluble)] (Figure S3).
Overall, the expression of genes involved in the breakdown of food to provide energy (catabolism) was significantly altered (had strong GEI), with rovers decreasing and sitters increasing their expression when food is present (I<0). For instance, glycogen phosphorylase (GlyP), which regulates glycogen breakdown, is highly significant (Figure 4A, q(for×food) = 0.000057). The differing genetic background between fors and fors2 has a main effect on GlyP (q(BG) = 1.64·10−7), but the response to food of each sitter genotype is similar (q(BG×food) = 0.33), so it is meaningful to speak of a GEI common to both types of sitters when compared to rovers. Conversely, expression of many genes in pathways for synthesis of proteins (anabolism) significantly increased in rovers in the Fed condition, exemplified by eukaryotic translation initiation factor 4A (eIF-4A; Figure 4B, q(for×food) = 0.0011). I is positive for eIF-4A and negative for GlyP.
We investigated whether functionally related genes show common modes of regulation as defined by the sign of I (see Text S1, Supplementary Methods, Group Level ANOVA), and found effects consistent with the catabolism-anabolism pattern described above. Catabolic groups such as carbohydrate (Figure 4C), glycogen, chitin, and amino acid breakdown genes, and mitochondrial oxidative phosphorylation complexes I–V had significant negative I values (Table S3). Anabolic groups associated with protein biosynthesis (Figure 4D), such as gene splicing, translation initiation, ribosomal proteins, and post-translational protein modification complex had significant positive I values (Figure 4D, Table S3). In Table S3, we list Gene Ontology groups with significant GEI, ordered from most positive to most negative I [groups with greater GO term specificity as per DAVID [16] GO levels 3–5 were used]. The most inclusive GO groups relating to catabolism/anabolism are group 9056 “catabolic process” which has significant negative for×food interaction I (p = 0.009, n = 140 genes) while the “biosynthetic process” group 9058 has positive I (p = 0.008, n = 300 genes; as these two groups were specifically singled out for testing we report p rather than FDR-adjusted q).
Some gene groups with significant for GEI go beyond a simple anabolism - catabolism dichotomy. In particular, functional groups involved in neural and muscle function – neurotransmitter secretion groups, synaptic transmission, postsynaptic membrane, ion channels, GABA and calcium-binding EGF domains – all have negative I values (Table S3).
Importance of genetic background in gene expression
How does the genetic background (BG) difference between fors and the other two strains (forR and fors2) affect GEI of functional groups? Most groups in Table S3 have highly significant FDR-corrected q values for the main effect of background. However, only a few groups have significant BG×food interactions, and these are all groups with positive for×food I values, associated with transcription, splicing, translation, or post-translational modification of proteins. Thus, at the functional group level, gene groups with significant positive for GEI I often have significant interactions with the genetic background but those with negative I values do not.
We used two methods to quantify the contributions of for, food, and BG to gene expression over all genes above cut off, not just those with interactions. First, we performed a principal components analysis (PCA) on log2 gene expression levels. Five PCA components were identified. The first component (explaining the largest amount of variance, see below) correlates strongly with main effect of BG, followed by components correlating with main effects of food and then for, with components 4 and 5 (explaining similar amounts of variance) correlating with the interactions of BG×food and for×food. Thus, genetic effects rank in the order BG>for>BG×food>for×food when measured by variance explained.
Our second method uses the Storey-Tibshirani false discovery rate (FDR) analysis to estimate the proportion of genes with significant effects [17]. This method uses a mixture-model approach to estimate the proportion π0 of genes matching the null hypothesis of no effect. Then πalt = 1−π0 estimates the proportion of genes matching the alternative hypothesis of an effect. One interpretation of πalt is that is the proportion of genes that would show significant effects, after FDR, if we had large numbers of replicates. π0 and hence πalt depend not only on the true rate of differential expression of genes (DEG) but also on the signal to noise ratio of the array technology. Thus, for low expression genes with poorer signal to noise ratio π0 will be higher even if the true proportion of DEG is unchanged. We calculated π0 both for the top-1000 highest expressing genes and for increasingly large groups of genes based on minimum expression levels. If signal to noise is the only factor affecting π0 then the intercept of the curve of π0 values based on expression is an estimator of 1-DEG. In practice we found good agreement between the latter method and π0 for the top 1000 genes, so we report the top-1000 figure.
This analysis was done for each ANOVA p-value (for all main and interaction effects). The πalt = 1−π0 for a given set of p-values (e.g. for main effect p values) estimates the true DEG rate for that effect. Our top-1000 πalt values were BG = 0.84, food = 0.73, BG×food = 0.63, for = 0.59, and for×food = 0.57. That is, among the top-1000 genes by mean expression level, 84% had a main effect of BG, 73% of food, 63% showed BG×food, 59% had a main effect of for and 57% had for×food GEI. Thus, although for×food GEI affects the smallest proportion of top-expressing genes, it still has an effect on 57% of these genes.
A quantitative measure of plasticity is higher in rovers
The interaction between for and feeding state could be due either to the genotypes responding in equal amounts but opposite directions to feeding (same plasticity of each genotype), or to one genotype responding more than the other (differences in magnitude of plasticity). To quantify differences in plasticity between rovers and sitters, we calculated an index of plasticity called Relative Nutrient Sensitivity (RNS) for any given trait as the difference between the size of the trait's rover response to food and the size of the sitter response: RNS = (|rover change|−|sitter change|)/C (where C = 1 for log2 transformed data, otherwise C = mean level of trait). In other words, for each trait compared (e.g., behaviour, metabolite, gene expression), RNS compares the absolute magnitude of for-dependent changes in response to food rather than their direction (see Methods).
When RNS>0, rovers show a larger response; when RNS<0, sitters respond more. When we tabulated RNS for behaviour, metabolites, gene expression, and functional gene categories, we found RNS>0 (rovers change more) in 8 of 9 behaviour cases (89%, Figure 5A, p = 0.03). For metabolites, gene expression, and functional gene categories, a significant majority of traits had RNS>0 (Figure 5B–5D, p<10−15 and Table S4). Thus, for a large majority of behavioural, metabolic, and gene expression traits, rovers exhibit greater food-related plasticity than sitters. This is true whether fors2 or fors are used in comparison to forR. The rover-natural sitter RNS comparison is more rover-biased (has more cases with rovers changing more than sitters) than the rover-mutant sitter comparison, but the correlation between the two is good (for genes, r = 0.83, p<10−15), so we show the conservative, common genetic background, rover-sitter mutant distributions in Figure 5.
The direction of for-dependent response to feeding state (GEI I) and plasticity (RNS) are different measures, as shown in Table S3. Most groups with significantly negative GEI I have RNS>0. Of the traits with RNS<0, most are mitochondrial groups involved in fatty acid beta oxidation or oxidative phosphorylation complexes I–V. Among metabolites (Table S2A, Table S4) only the group containing PS polysaccharides has significant RNS<0. So, sitters are more plastic than rovers (have a higher magnitude of change in response to food) for a small subset of traits having to do with sugars (among metabolites) or mitochondrial catabolic pathways (functional groups), but rovers are more plastic for the majority of gene and metabolite groups.
Hypothesis: plasticity differences and the insulin pathway
We hypothesized that the for-dependent metabolic plasticity might be mediated by the insulin signaling pathway. This is because rovers exhibit a higher plasticity in response to feeding state and the insulin signaling pathway is a key regulator of the response to food [18].
In the cell, binding of DILPS (Drosophila insulin-like peptides) to the insulin receptor (InR) triggers a signaling cascade with major effects on gene expression [19]. Protein translation is increased via phosphorylation of key members of the TOR pathway by the kinases Akt1 (dPKB) and Pk61C (dPDK) [20],[21]. Negative homeostatic control of insulin signaling occurs on the transcriptional level – when signaling is high, foxo is phosphorylated by Akt1 and sequestered in the cytoplasm, but when signaling drops foxo translocates to the nucleus where it stimulates transcription of genes such as InR and the negative regulator of translation Thor (d4EBP) [22],[23],[24]. At the transcript level, then, many insulin pathway genes have an inverse relationship to the level of insulin signaling. Our results show that transcription of positive regulators decreased more in fed rovers than sitters, resulting in a negative GEI interaction coefficient I for the group of positive regulators as a whole (Figure S2). This normal inverse relationship between transcription and insulin signaling is more evident in rovers than sitters. As with RNS, this is true whether mutant or natural sitters are compared, but again as for RNS, the difference between rovers and natural sitters is larger than the difference between rovers and mutant sitters, suggesting that the genetic background of natural sitters may intensify this difference.
Genetic test of interaction of foraging and insulin genes
The finding that rovers show larger responses to food, and the known role of insulin signaling in the response to food, suggests that rovers might also show a larger impact of changes to insulin signaling. We therefore tested whether for interacts with the insulin signaling pathway by means of quantitative complementation crosses for epistasis between mutant insulin pathway genes and alleles of for [25],[26],[27]. We crossed each of the three for genotypes to loss of function mutants of the fly insulin receptor InR, phosphatidylinositol-3-kinase Pi3K92E (or Dp110), and foxo (Methods). InR and Dp110 are positive regulators of insulin signaling; foxo is a negative regulator. Based on our gene expression data, we hypothesized that rovers had higher insulin signaling than sitters, so we expected crosses of rovers with loss of function insulin mutants to be more sitter-like than their controls.
We tested food-deprived adults of the resulting 18 trans heterozygote genotypes and compared food-leaving scores of for;mutant to the for;Balancer which controlled for genetic background effects (see Methods). Recall that food-deprived homozygous rovers show low levels of food leaving behaviour (Figure 1) while sitters have higher levels. As expected, the control for;Balancer flies show the previously found lower level of behavioural response for rovers compared to sitters (Figure 6A and 6B, solid lines), indicating no direct effects of, or interactions with, the balancer chromosome background. There is, however, a significant epistatic interaction in the for;InR and for;dp110 flies (Figure 6A and 6B dashed lines; Table S5). Rovers crossed to these insulin pathway mutants become more sitter-like. In contrast, the interaction with negative regulator foxo is not significant (Table S5). Mutants of positive regulators of insulin signaling make rover food-leaving behaviour more like sitters (reduces RNS), while a mutant of negative regulator foxo trended towards making rovers less like sitters (increases RNS). This suggests that there is a significant (epistatic) interaction between for and the two positive regulators of the insulin signaling pathway tested here. This is consistent with rovers experiencing greater shifts in insulin signaling effects between Fed and FD states than sitters. There are also differences between natural and mutant sitters in the interaction with InR (Table S5) suggesting that the difference in genetic backgrounds between these strains may also affect this interaction. For this behavioural measure, natural sitters are intermediate between rovers and mutant sitters, a difference from the trend found in gene expression overall (via RNS, Figure 5) or in regulators of insulin signaling (Figure S2). Thus the effect of the background difference between natural sitters and the other strains varies between gene expression and behavioural measures.
Meta-analysis of insulin and foraging effects on gene expression
We performed a bioinformatic meta-analysis comparing our array results to those from three published microarray studies which manipulated insulin/Tor signaling [28],[29],[30]. This provides additional evidence for transcriptional parallels between for and insulin. We use these studies to identify sets of genes which were up- or down-regulated by the manipulation of insulin/Tor signaling, and which had high enough expression levels in our data for reliable comparison. To ensure independence of the three analyses we used sets of genes which did not overlap between studies (see Table S6 for gene selection criteria).
For each up- or down-regulated set of genes identified from a study we calculated the mean log2 fold change between rovers and mutant sitters when Fed or FD. This gave four comparisons per study (up/down regulated in study 1, 2 or 3×Fed/FD in our data). In other words, we used the 3 independent studies to tell us which genes may be transcriptionally regulated by insulin signaling. We then used our data to ask, for the same genes, what the rover-sitter difference in expression is under the two food conditions. Our hypothesis is that rovers have higher insulin signaling when Fed than sitters, but not necessarily when FD. Hence we predict that genes requiring insulin signaling for their expression should have higher expression levels in Fed rovers than in Fed sitters, but that this difference may not exist in FD rovers and sitters. Similarly, if genes are shown in the independent study to have lower expression when insulin signaling is high (or equivalently, higher expression when insulin expression is reduced), then we predict those genes should have lower expression in Fed rovers than in Fed sitters.
In the first study, Buch et al. [28] ablated dilp3 secreting cells in adults and used microarrays to compare ablation lines which had reduced insulin signaling to that of controls. Figure 7A shows a summary of the four rover-sitter comparisons for this study. Bars on the right labelled “expression down” are for genes whose expression was reduced by dilp3 ablation (i.e. insulin signaling increases expression of these) and bars on the left (“expression up”) are for genes whose expression was increased by dilp3 ablation (genes repressed by insulin signaling). Genes with expression reduced by dilp3 ablation (Figure 7A, left) show a negative GEI interaction sign I (rovers higher when food deprived, mutant sitters higher when fed). Conversely genes increased by dilp3 ablation (Figure 7A, right) show a positive GEI interaction sign I (no difference when food deprived, rovers higher when fed), in accordance with our predictions.
Figure 7B gives the four comparisons for genes whose expression was changed by foxo overexpression [29]; Figure 7C is for genes changed by rapamycin treatment [30]. In each case the pattern is similar to dilp3 ablation: genes with expression increased by a manipulation equivalent to lowering insulin/Tor signaling (genes reduced by insulin) show the negative I GEI interaction, while genes whose expression is reduced by the manipulation (genes increased by insulin) show positive I. Full statistics are given in Table S6. This table also shows that the pattern in I is more significant when natural sitters are used in the analysis than when only mutant sitters are used, so the trends shown in Figure 7 apply to both mutant and natural sitters.
In summary, the patterns of GEI interaction strength I in rover-sitter gene expression of genes affected by three different manipulations of insulin/Tor signaling in three independent studies [28],[29],[30] are consistent with our hypothesis in each study. Genes requiring insulin signaling for expression show positive rover-sitter I and genes inhibited by insulin signaling show negative rover-sitter I.
Discussion
The foraging gene in Drosophila which encodes PKG is known for its importance as a natural variant affecting behavioural and neural plasticity [11],[14],[31],[32],[33],[34],[35]. We now demonstrate that it also affects metabolic, gene expression, and behavioural plasticity in adult flies. Specifically, rovers show a greater response to changes in their food environment than either mutant or natural sitters for the majority of behavioural, metabolite, and gene expression traits studied here. The pattern of such changes is matched by the pattern of expression of positive regulators of insulin signaling. Combining for alleles with mutants of positive insulin signaling regulators makes rover responses sitter-like, but does not change sitter responses. Collectively these findings suggest that the effect on metabolic, gene expression, and behavioural plasticity of foraging works in part through the insulin signaling pathway.
The for product PKG and the insulin pathway are conserved across many animals, from worms to flies and mammals [36]. PKG has been found to produce behavioural responses to food in flies [14],[33], nematodes [37],[38], honeybees [39], and ants [40],[41]. In particular, PKG interacts with insulin and TGF-beta signaling in worms to regulate quiescence, a state possibly related to satiety [37]. In this study we have focused on the insulin pathway, but potential interactions between for and TGF-beta may be a fruitful area for future study in flies.
GEI, foraging, insulin, and energy store allocation
Expression of insulin pathway genes such as InR and Pi3k92E (Dp110) is inversely related to the strength of insulin signaling via the foxo transcription factor: foxo is retained in the cytoplasm when signaling is high, but translocates to the nucleus and stimulates transcription of pathway genes when signaling is low [23],[24]. Since insulin gene expression is opposite to insulin signaling strength, our finding of greater negative transcriptional effects on insulin pathway genes in rovers (Figure S2) suggests the presence of greater positive insulin signaling in rovers. Since insulin signaling upregulates anabolism and reduces catabolism [19],[42], this is consistent with the patterns we find in genes involved in anabolic (fed rover-biased) and catabolic (fed sitter-biased) processes. And genes which require insulin for expression are higher in fed rovers, while genes repressed by insulin tend to be higher in fed sitters (Figure 7).
The finding of genetic interactions between for and genes in the insulin signaling pathway raises questions for future investigation. Insulin signaling in flies can reduce flow through tricarboxylic acid cycle and oxidative phosphorylation and increase flow through the pentose-phosphate shunt, freeing pyruvate and acetyl CoA for lipogenesis and increasing NADPH and precursors for biosynthesis [43]. This is consistent with patterns in the Fed adult rover catabolic groups (Figure 4C, Table S3). Instead of accumulating energy as fat, Fed adult sitters accumulate carbohydrates. Because of the lower density of fat and its higher caloric content, rovers store more energy per unit mass than sitters, a difference which should have implications for life history characteristics such as starvation resistance (see below).
Several studies note changes in fat stores in flies with mutations in insulin signaling genes. These include, loss of fat in melted mutants [44], gain of fat in InR, chico and Pi3K (also called Dp110) mutants [45],[46] and Pi3K-overexpression in larvae increases accumulation of nutrients in fat [47]. Nuclear foxo reduces fat, phenocopying starvation [44],[48], and it reduces head fat body insulin signaling [49]. Thus, there may be multiple different effects of insulin-related genes on fat. Could foxo mediate lower sitter fat levels? Rapamycin treatment (which acts downstream of foxo specifically on the Tor signaling pathway) also produces similar patterns of effects (Figure 7C). Hence, indirect effects of insulin signaling on the Tor pathway could also be involved. In support of this, PDK/Pk61C is the gene in the insulin pathway showing strongest transcriptional regulation in rovers versus both natural and mutant sitters (Figure S2B). PDK phosphorylates ribosomal S6 kinase (S6k), part of Tor regulation of translation [20],[21],[50]. Indeed, overexpression of Tor has been shown to increase triglycerides in adult male flies [44].
Genes repressed by foxo, rapamycin, or dilp3 ablation are rover-biased in Fed flies, while genes increased by insulin/Tor knockdown are rover-biased in FD flies (Figure 7A–7C); that is, if a gene's expression is increased by insulin/Tor signaling, it tends to be higher in Fed rovers, while if it's expression is increased by inhibition of insulin/Tor signaling, it tends to be higher in FD rovers. This is an example of the more general trend illustrated in Figure 5, where the change between Fed and FD flies is larger in rovers than in sitters for behaviours (89%), metabolites (84%), genes (77%), and gene groups (77%). In mammals, a reduced physiological response to food is a sign of insulin resistance; whether this is true of sitters waits further testing.
Our data provides direct evidence through genetic crosses and considerable correlational information through patterns of insulin gene expression and meta-analysis that the GEI effects of foraging in Fed and FD flies are mediated at least in part through interaction with the insulin/Tor signaling pathways.
Consequences of lipid–carbohydrate allocation differences
Whole-body energy stores in fed rovers and sitters differ: how might fatty rovers and starchy sitters differ in life-history? A number of life-history and ecological parameters have been shown to be related to lipid or carbohydrate reserves in flies, including flight capacity, starvation and desiccation resistance. Diptera in general and fruit flies in particular are dependent on glycogen reserves and hemolymph sugars to fuel flight muscles [51],[52],[53]. Glycogen phosphorylase, which has strong GEI in rovers and sitters, is a rate limiting enzyme for glycogen mobilization to support flight [54] (Figure 4A). Flies selected for postponed ageing show increased flight duration, glycogen reserves and resistance to desiccation, [55], while desiccation-selected flies show higher glycogen levels [56]. Glycogen, desiccation resistance, longevity and stress resistance may form a cluster of correlated traits in flies [57]. Lipid content of adult flies correlates with starvation resistance [58]; among lifespan-selected and other lines, starvation resistance was correlated with lipid content and not glycogen [55]. This correlation extends to sibling species D. simulans [59]. In a cricket species where lipids can be used to support flight, a trade-off between lipid reserves for flight and for egg production has been reported [60],[61].
The rover-sitter system, with its dichotomous Y-allocation [62] of energy stores to lipids and carbohydrates, may therefore be useful for studying single-gene influences on traits with costs and benefits associated with energy use and storage including flight capacity, desiccation and starvation resistance.
GEI due to foraging and neuromuscular function
The patterns of GEI and I between foraging and food (Fed vs. FD) are very consistent for genes whose primary function is in anabolic or catabolic pathways, with I positive in anabolic groups and negative in catabolic groups. However, there are many more genes with significant for GEI. An important set of such genes is involved in nerve and/or muscle function (Table S3). PKG affects synaptic plasticity in mammals [63],[64],[65],[66] and learning and memory in flies [33],[34].
The possibility that PKG may cause GEI through its role in regulating ion homeostasis in nerves and muscles deserves further examination. PRKG1, the mammalian homolog of for, regulates calcium and potassium fluxes in smooth muscle relaxation where it is associated with the myosin phosphatase complex, Ca++ATPases, and potassium ion channels [67]. We find a cluster of gene groups with I<0 associated with muscle and actin cytoskeleton, including genes such as wupA (troponin-I) and Prm (paramyosin). These are some of the genes whose expression is most correlated with for in a coexpression analysis across humans, flies, worms, and yeast [68].
Calcium/potassium levels are important in synaptic function and plasticity, and mutations in potassium channel genes affect habituation in the giant-fiber axon escape reflex in flies [69],[70]. Habituation of the giant-fiber escape reflex differs in adult rovers and sitters [31]. Rover-sitter differences in PKG are also associated with different voltage-dependent K+ currents in larval neuromuscular junctions, along with differences in neuronal excitability, neurotransmitter release, and synaptic transmission [71]. Rover-sitter differences in neural thermotolerance arise from differences in the regulation of K+ channel activity via a circuit involving PKG, PP2A, and ion channels [72]. Thus our demonstration of rover-sitter differences in gene expression of genes involved in neurotransmitter release, postsynaptic membranes, and calcium- and potassium-channels supports previous studies. It will be important to determine whether foraging interacts epistatically with other genes influencing K+ currents in neurons and muscles. It is also of interest to investigate whether the metabolic effects of allelic variation in for are independent of, or are tied to PKG's effects on ion homeostasis and neural function.
Magnitude of foraging GEI compared to effects of other genes
Our study used only a few strains of flies and thus does not speak to the importance of for-mediated effects on the genome in natural populations. However, we are able to consider allelic effects at the for locus relative to the genetic background effects in a principal components analysis which identifies genetic background (BG) and food (Fed vs. FD) as the most important factors, followed by the interaction of BG and food, for genotype main effects, and for interaction effects. Using the Storey-Tibshirani method to estimate the true proportion of differentially expressed genes πalt shows that for GEI affects 57% of the highest expression genes. We also found that the effect of the natural sitter background was to intensify gene expression contrasts with rovers but to reduce behavioural contrasts. Thus, an important future step is to quantify the relative importance and roles of for and other genes in a wider variety of natural genetic backgrounds.
Our results also speak to evolutionary questions about pleiotropy, epistasis, and plasticity. Pleiotropic genes may affect few traits when redundancy, degeneracy [73], or compensations in gene networks buffer the effects of mutations [74], while mutations in other genes produce large changes [75]. The number of traits influenced by a gene follows a power law, with a few genes having widespread affects [74],[76]. It has been proposed that the use of naturally occurring alleles or mild mutations is more relevant to studies of epistasis and network stability than the more common use of knockouts or severe loss of function mutations [73],[77],[78]. The question of whether some genes can increase phenotypic plasticity and thus whether selection can act to increase or decrease plasticity has been the subject of much debate [2]. In for we have an example of a gene with naturally occurring alleles maintained in a stable polymorphism in the wild [79]. We demonstrate that in adult flies for interacts pervasively with food, producing pleiotropic GEI in behaviours, lipids and carbohydrates, and gene expression. Our quantitative plasticity measure reveals that for×food GEI is often due to rovers having significantly higher food-related plasticity than sitters. We show that for interacts with genes of the insulin signaling pathway to produce some of these effects. The foraging gene may thus provide a suitable context for resolving some of these questions relating to phenotypic plasticity and selection.
Methods
Strains and rearing
The following allelic variants of the chromosome-2 foraging (for) gene were used in this study: the forR (natural rover), fors (natural sitter) variants and fors2 (a sitter mutant strain made on a forR genetic background) [9],[13]. All strains share common isogenic third chromosomes from forR and common X chromosomes. Genetic variation on the small fourth and Y chromosomes was not controlled. For tests of epistasis we used three null mutants of genes in the insulin signalling pathway (see below).
Flies were reared on medium b for behavioural assays, gene array and metabolite experiments (Table S1A) [80]. For additional behavioural assays reported in Table S1B, S1C flies were raised in media described in Table S1A, S1C. Flies were raised in 40 mL plastic vials containing 10 mL food medium in 12/12 h light/dark cycle (lights on 08:00), 25±1°C, 70±5% relative humidity (standard conditions).
Fly rearing and behaviour testing
Flies were collected 0–2 days post eclosion, separated under light CO2 anaesthesia, then reared in groups of 25–30 for 4 days. 12–13 males and 12–13 females were used in each rearing group. Adult rearing was done under standard conditions as described above. Flies were transferred to test media the night before behaviour tests. Test media consisted of 10 mL of food medium (Fed), or 10 mL 1% agar for food deprivation (FD) tests in vials. Flies were tested in the morning (9–12 a.m.) after 16–18 hours under Fed or FD conditions.
A plexiglass maze was used for the food-leaving assay; the maze is as described [80] and is shown in Figure S1. Each morning mazes were conditioned by passing through one sample of 25 natural sitter flies before testing commenced. Mazes were placed horizontally on a light table with a uniform light intensity of 1000 lumens. Flies were placed in a10×75 mm borosilicate glass tube (the “sugar entry tube”) containing 0.5 mL 0.25 M sucrose in 1% agar for 15 min prior to test. At start of testing, the sugar tube with flies is placed in the entry port of the maze. Empty glass collection tubes are placed in the 9 exit ports of the maze. After 3 min, flies in collection tubes at exit ports are counted, as are flies remaining in the sugar tube. Food-leaving score is (flies in collection tubes)/(total flies). All treatment conditions were tested on at least 3 different test days.
Quantitative complementation crosses
To test whether the rover and sitter for alleles interact epistatically with null alleles of the three genes involved in the insulin signaling pathway we used a form of quantitative complementation, a method of complementation developed for testing quantitative effects, in this case, gene interactions [27].We asked if one copy of a mutant allele of a gene involved in insulin signaling, in the presence of each of the for alleles, changes food-leaving behaviour. Crosses were made between rover (forR), natural sitter (fors), and mutant sitter (fors2) strains and balanced loss of function mutants in three insulin signaling pathway genes: (a) InR, the insulin receptor (mutant allele: InR93dj -4 [81]); (b) Pi3K92E/Dp110, the phosphatidylinositol 3-kinase catalytic subunit (Dp110B [25]); and (c) foxo, (foxo21 [22]). Strains carrying mutations in the insulin signaling pathway were: (a) In(3R)GC25, InR93Dj-4/TM3, Sb1 (Bloomington stock 9554) [82]; (b) yw;P[ry +,gH], Dp110B/TM3,Ser,y +(C) [83]; (c) foxo21/TM6C [22]. All mutants of the insulin signaling pathway were maintained heterozygous with balancer chromosomes which did not carry mutations in these insulin signaling genes and were on a sitter background. Epistasis is identified as a two-way statistical interaction between the variant (rover or sitter) and test genotype (mutant or control) [26] (see below). The balancer heterozygotes control for effects of natural variation in the genetic background that can result in increases or decreases in food leaving behaviour.
Metabolite analysis
Fourier Transform Ion Cyclotron Resonance Mass Spectrometry (FTICR MS) was used to analyze homogenized fly heads (equal numbers of males and females) from 5–7 day post eclosion forR and fors2 strains harvested in the morning. Food Deprived (FD) flies had been restricted to water in agar 12 hours before collection. Samples were taken in triplicate. Values shown are Signal to Noise (S/N) ratios. Each sample was analysed as described [84] using the DiscovaMetrics [85] package producing parent ion molecular weights accurate to within 0.0005 daltons; compound identifications were cross-checked against Kegg Ligand [86] and Metlin [87] databases. FTICR MS has maximum sensitivity to metabolites in the 100–1000 Dalton range, accuracy of 0.0001 Dalton, and uses six different buffer/ionization modes (Table S2) each detecting different classes of metabolites. For instance, compounds such as sugars and phosphates are detected in mode 1102 using a polar buffer and negative ion electrospray, while mode 1203 uses a non-polar buffer and positive atmospheric pressure chemical ionization mode to detect compounds such as triacylglycerols.
Whole body lipid and carbohydrate analysis was performed separately on males and females of 5–7 day post-eclosion forR, fors, and fors2 strains. For both lipid and carbohydrate analyses, results were standardized against dry weight. For lipids, the ether extraction method was used as described [88]. In brief, flies were frozen in liquid nitrogen, then weighed to the nearest 0.01 mg in groups of 5–10 flies on a Mettler Toledo XS205 balance. Flies were then dried at 60°C for 24 hours and reweighed. Lipids were then extracted in 1 mL of ether for 24 hours, after which ether was decanted and flies were dried at 60°C for 24 hours and weighed. Total carbohydrate levels were determined using amyloglucosidase digestion followed by spectrophotometric determination of total glucose using NAD to NADH reduction [89]. Sigma kit GAHK20. Briefly, hexokinase catalyzes phosphorylation of glucose in the presence of ATP to Glucose-6-phosphate (G6P), which is then oxidized to 6-phospho-gluconate in the presence of oxidized nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide (NAD) in a reaction catalyzed by glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase (G6PDH). During this oxidation, an equimolar amount of NAD is reduced to NADH. The consequent increase in absorbance at 340 nm is directly proportional to glucose concentration. Protein was measured using the bicinchonic acid (BCA) method. Total energy content was calculated based on ratios of 9∶4∶4 Kcal/gm for fats∶carbs∶protein.
Microarray analysis
Affymetrix Drosophila Genome 1.0 cDNA microarrays were used to evaluate effect of foraging genotype and feeding state on transcript levels in adult heads. Flies homozygous for each of the 3 alleles, forR, fors, and fors2, were raised to 5–7 days post-eclosion, and given Food or FD treatments as described above. Samples of flies (equal numbers of males and females) were frozen in liquid nitrogen, and heads were separated by sieving. RNA was extracted as described [90]. Triplicate RNA samples for each treatment were hybridized to Drosophila Genome 1.0 microarrays, for a total of 18 arrays. N = 3 within each treatment. Expression levels produced by MAS 5.0 were normalized by quantile normalization [91] of log2 transformed data. Full MIAME information and expression set data is filed as GEO accession GSE14371. Pathway analysis and gene ANOVA were performed as described in Statistical methods. Analysis of variance was used to detect significant GEI for individual genes after False Discovery Rate correction for multiple testing [17].
qRTPCR analysis
Levels of expression of 2 genes (Treh, CG10924) were confirmed by quantitative reverse transcriptase polymerase chain reaction analysis (Figure S3, Text S1). Heads of male and female forR and fors2 flies were raised as in the microarray analysis and then were frozen in liquid nitrogen. RNA was extracted using the Trizol method (15596-018, Life Technologies) and further purified using the Qiagen RNeasy kit (74106, Qiagen). The amount of RNA in each sample was determined using a Nanodrop spectrophotometer (ND-1000) and sample quality verified using 260/280 micron absorbance ratios.
Statistical methods
Scores of each trait are analysed with two-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) to determine whether significant GEI exists (see detailed procedures below). The Storey-Tibshirani False Discovery Rate (FDR) [17] is used for multiple testing correction and estimation of π0 using the qvalue package as implemented by Storey [17], with default parameters. Thus ANOVA p values have been replaced by FDR q values, and q<0.05 is deemed significant.
RNS measures which strain has higher plasticity and is defined as RNS = (|rover change|−|sitter change|)/C (for log2 transformed data C = 1, else C = mean of all treatments). That is, RNS compares the absolute magnitude of changes in response to food and is positive when rovers change more than sitters.
For ANOVA of gene expression data with two sitter strains and one rover, a modified general linear model design matrix was used (Text S1, Supplementary Methods) to ensure unbiased estimation. Briefly, factors RS (rover or sitter), food (Fed or FD), and BG (genetic background, 2 levels, 1 for rovers and mutant sitters, another for natural sitters) were analysed including main effects and the interactions RS×food and BG×food. A reduced model omitting the interaction BG×food was also fitted. For each gene, the first and second models were compared using Schwartz's Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) [92] to determine whether to report full or reduced model results. Thus if interaction of BG and food was significant (as determined by BIC) we reported statistics from the full model, else from the reduced model. FDR correction was then applied to p-values from the selected model.
For group-wise ANOVA analysis of groups of metabolites or genes, a linear model as above, with the addition of a factor G with one level for each gene or compound was used; this is similar to adjusting each gene or compound to have a mean of zero, but accounts more conservatively for lost degrees of freedom due to the adjustment. Only transformed variables with approximately equal variances are used in group-wise ANOVAs. Microarray data is log2 transformed, then subjected to quantile normalization and a variance-equalizing monotonic transform. After these steps variances for the top-expressing 60% of genes were approximately equal and data was normally distributed. For group-wise ANOVA of metabolites, log2 data was used. Data was tested with a covariate of molecular weight (MW). If MW or its interactions with food and genotype were significant, the ANCOVA with MW is reported; otherwise group-wise ANOVA results are reported.
For ANOVA of behavioural experiments where behaviours may vary from day to day (Day effect), Day was added as a random factor to the ANOVA described above for single genes, and significance of this mixed-model was determined by F-tests of fixed factor terms to their interactions with Day. See Table S1 and Table S5 for examples.
In the quantitative complementation crosses in Table S5, the interaction of for with a factor representing the presence or absence of the mutant insulin gene is tested. That is, we test for epistasis rather than GEI. The ANOVA analysis is identical in format to that just described, with the factor representing presence/absence of insulin mutant replacing the food factor.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We thank Brian Staveley and Sally Leevers for fly stocks; Hiwote Belay for RNA extractions; Don Robinson, Brandon Sheffield, and Bianco Marco for technical assistance; and Mo Kai Guo for qRTPCR at the Drosophila Canadian Microarray Facility. Karen Williams, Joel Levine, Amsale Belay, and three anonymous reviewers provided extremely useful comments on the manuscript.
Footnotes
The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
This research was supported by Grant DK70141-2 from NIDDK (www.niddk.nih.gov) to MBS and by the Neurosciences Research Foundation (www.nsi.edu). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Via S, Gomulkiewicz R, Dejong G, Scheiner SM, Schlichting CD, et al. Adaptive phenotypic plasticity - consensus and controversy. Trends Ecol Evol. 1995;10:212–217. doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(00)89061-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sarkar S. From the reaktionsnorm to the evolution of adaptive plasticity: A historical sketch, 1909–1999. In: DeWitt TJ, Scheiner SM, editors. Phenotypic plasticity - functional and conceptual approaches. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 10–30. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Windig JJ, de Kovel CGF, de Jong G. Genetics and mechanics of plasticity. In: DeWitt TJ, Scheiner SM, editors. Phenotypic plasticity: Functional and conceptual approaches. New York, N.Y.: Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 31–49. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Via S, Lande R. Genotype-environment interaction and the evolution of phenotypic plasticity. Evolution. 1985;39:505–522. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1985.tb00391.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gutteling EW, Riksen JA, Bakker J, Kammenga JE. Mapping phenotypic plasticity and genotype-environment interactions affecting life-history traits in Caenorhabditis elegans. Heredity. 2007;98:28. doi: 10.1038/sj.hdy.6800894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Sambandan D, Carbone MA, Anholt RRH, Mackay TFC. Phenotypic plasticity and genotype by environment interaction for olfactory behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics. 2008;179:1079–1088. doi: 10.1534/genetics.108.086769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kalderon D, Rubin GM. cGMP-dependent Protein Kinase genes in Drosophila. J Biol Chem. 1989;264:10738–10748. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Osborne KA. Natural behavior polymorphism due to a cGMP-dependent Protein Kinase of Drosophila. Science. 1997;277:834–836. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5327.834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.de Belle JS, Hilliker AJ, Sokolowski MB. Genetic localization of foraging (for): A major gene for larval behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics. 1989;123:157–164. doi: 10.1093/genetics/123.1.157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.de Belle JS, Sokolowski MB, Hilliker AJ. Genetic analysis of the foraging microregion of Drosophila melanogaster. Genome. 1993;36:94–101. doi: 10.1139/g93-013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kaun KR, Riedl CAL, Chakaborty-Chatterjee M, Belay AT, Douglas SJ, et al. Natural variation in food acquisition mediated via a Drosophila cGMP-dependent Protein Kinase. J Exp Biol. 2007;210:3547–3558. doi: 10.1242/jeb.006924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Graf SA, Sokolowski MB. The rover/sitter Drosophila foraging polymorphism as a function of larval development, food patch quality and starvation. J Insect Behav. 1989;2:301–313. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Pereira HS, Sokolowski MB. Mutations in the larval foraging gene affect adult locomotory behavior after feeding in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1993;90:5044–5046. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.11.5044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Scheiner R, Sokolowski MB, Erber J. Activity of cGMP-dependent Protein Kinase (PKG) affects sucrose responsiveness and habituation in Drosophila melanogaster. Learn Mem. 2004;11:303–311. doi: 10.1101/lm.71604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Belay AT, Scheiner R, So AKC, Douglas SJ, Chakaborty-Chatterjee M, et al. The foraging gene of Drosophila melanogaster: Spatial-expression analysis and sucrose responsiveness. J Comp Neurol. 2007;504:570–582. doi: 10.1002/cne.21466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Dennis G, Jr, Sherman BT, Hosack DA, Yang J, Gao W, et al. DAVID: Database for annotation, visualization, and integrated discovery. Genome Biol. 2003;4:P3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Storey JD, Tibshirani R. Statistical significance for genomewide studies. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:9440–9445. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1530509100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Grewal SS. Insulin/Tor signaling in growth and homeostasis: A view from the fly world. Int J Biochem Cell Biol. 2009;41:1006–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.biocel.2008.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wu Q, Brown MR. Signaling and function of insulin-like peptides in insects. Annu Rev Entomol. 2006;51:1–24. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.51.110104.151011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Radimerski T, Montagne J, Rintelen F, Stocker H, van der Kaay J, et al. dS6K-regulated cell growth is dPKB/dPI(3)K-independent, but requires dPDK1. Nat Cell Biol. 2002;4:251–255. doi: 10.1038/ncb763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Rintelen F, Stocker H, Thomas G, Hafen E. PDK1 regulates growth through Akt1 and S6k in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:15020–15025. doi: 10.1073/pnas.011318098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Junger MA, Rintelen F, Stocker H, Wasserman JD, Vegh M, et al. The Drosophila forkhead transcription factor foxo mediates the reduction in cell number associated with reduced insulin signaling. J Biol. 2003;2:2–17. doi: 10.1186/1475-4924-2-20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Puig O, Marr MT, Ruhf ML, Tjian R. Control of cell number by Drosophila foxo: Downstream and feedback regulation of the insulin receptor pathway. Genes Dev. 2003;17:2006–2020. doi: 10.1101/gad.1098703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Puig O, Tjian R. Transcriptional feedback control of insulin receptor by dfoxo/foxo1. Genes Dev. 2005;19:2435–2446. doi: 10.1101/gad.1340505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Williams KD, Busto M, Suster ML, So AKC, Ben-Shahar Y, et al. Natural variation in Drosophila melanogaster diapause due to the insulin-regulated PI3-kinase. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:15911–15915. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604592103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gibson G, Dworkin I. Uncovering cryptic genetic variation. Nat Rev Genet. 2004;5:681–690. doi: 10.1038/nrg1426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mackay TF. Quantitative trait loci in Drosophila. Nat Rev Genet. 2001;2:11–20. doi: 10.1038/35047544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Buch S, Melcher C, Bauer M, Katzenberger J, Pankratz MJ. Opposing effects of dietary protein and sugar regulate a transcriptional target of Drosophila insulin-like peptide signaling. Cell Metab. 2008;7:321–332. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2008.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Gershman B, Puig O, Hang L, Peitzsch RM, Tatar M, et al. High-resolution dynamics of the transcriptional response to nutrition in Drosophila: A key role for dfoxo. Physiol Genomics. 2007;29:24–34. doi: 10.1152/physiolgenomics.00061.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Guertin DA, Guntur KVP, Bell GW, Thoreen CC, Sabatini DM. Functional genomics identifies Tor-regulated genes that control growth and division. Curr Biol. 2006;16:958–970. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Engel JE, Xie XJ, Sokolowski MB, Wu CF. A cGMP-dependent protein kinase gene, foraging, modifies habituation-like response decrement of the giant fiber escape circuit in Drosophila. Learn Mem. 2000;7:341–352. doi: 10.1101/lm.31600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Mery F, Belay AT, Sokolowski MB, Kawecki TJ. Antagonistic effects of natural polymorphism at the foraging locus on short-term learning and long-term memory. J Neurogenet. 2006;20:177–177. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kaun KR, Hendel T, Gerber B, Sokolowski MB. Natural variation in Drosophila larval reward learning and memory due to a cGMP-dependent protein kinase. Learn Mem. 2007;14:342–349. doi: 10.1101/lm.505807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Mery F, Belay AT, So AK, Sokolowski MB, Kawecki TJ. Natural polymorphism affecting learning and memory in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:13051–13055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702923104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kaun KR, Riedl CAL, Chakaborty-Chatterjee M, Belay AT, Douglas SJ, et al. Natural variation in plasticity of glucose homeostasis and food intake. J Exp Biol. 2008;211:3160–3166. doi: 10.1242/jeb.010124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Fitzpatrick MJ, Sokolowski MB. In search of food: Exploring the evolutionary link between cGMP-dependent protein kinase (PKG) and behaviour. Integr Comp Biol. 2004;44:28–36. doi: 10.1093/icb/44.1.28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.You YJ, Kim J, Raizen DM, Avery L. Insulin, cGMP, and TGF-beta signals regulate food intake and quiescence in C. elegans: A model for satiety. Cell Metab. 2008;7:249–257. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2008.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Gray JM. Oxygen sensation and social feeding mediated by a C. elegans guanylate cyclase homologue. Nature. 2004;430:317–322. doi: 10.1038/nature02714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ben-Shahar Y, Leung HT, Pak WL, Sokolowski MB, Robinson GE. cGMP-dependent changes in phototaxis: A possible role for the foraging gene in honey bee division of labor. J Exp Biol. 2003;206:2507–2515. doi: 10.1242/jeb.00442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ingram KK, Oefner P, Gordon DM. Task-specific expression of the foraging gene in harvester ants. Mol Ecol. 2005;14:813–818. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2005.02450.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Lucas C, Sokolowski MB. Molecular basis for changes in behavioral state in ant social behaviors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:6351–6356. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809463106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lasko P, Sonenberg N. Coordinated transcriptional and translational control in metabolic homeostasis in flies. Genes Dev. 2007;21:235–237. doi: 10.1101/gad.1524707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ceddia RB, Bikopoulos GJ, Hilliker AJ, Sweeney G. Insulin stimulates glucose metabolism via the pentose phosphate pathway in Drosophila Kc cells. FEBS Letters. 2003;555:307–310. doi: 10.1016/s0014-5793(03)01261-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Teleman AA, Chen YW, Cohen SM. Drosophila melted modulates foxo and Tor activity. Dev Cell. 2005;9:271–281. doi: 10.1016/j.devcel.2005.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Brogiolo W, Stocker H, Ikeya T, Rintelen F, Fernandez R, et al. An evolutionarily conserved function of the Drosophila insulin receptor and insulin-like peptides in growth control. Curr Biol. 2001;11:213–221. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(01)00068-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Bohni R, Riesgo-Escovar J, Oldham S, Brogiolo W, Stocker H, et al. Autonomous control of cell and organ size by chico, a Drosophila homolog of vertebrate IRS1-4. Cell. 1999;97:865–875. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80799-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Britton JS, Lockwood WK, Li L, Cohen SM, Edgar BA. Drosophila's insulin/PI3-Kinase pathway coordinates cellular metabolism with nutritional conditions. Dev Cell. 2002;2:239–249. doi: 10.1016/s1534-5807(02)00117-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kramer JM, Davidge JT, Lockyer JM, Staveley BE. Expression of Drosophila foxo regulates growth and can phenocopy starvation. BMC Dev Biol. 2003;3:1–14. doi: 10.1186/1471-213X-3-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Hwangbo DS, Gershman B, Tu MP, Palmer M, Tatar M. Drosophila dfoxo controls lifespan and regulates insulin signalling in brain and fat body. Nature. 2004;429:562–566. doi: 10.1038/nature02549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Cohen P, Alessi DR, Cross DA. PDK1, one of the missing links in insulin signal transduction? FEBS Letters. 1997;410:3–10. doi: 10.1016/s0014-5793(97)00490-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Nayar JK, Vanhande E. Fuel for sustained mosquito flight. J Insect Physiol. 1971;17:471–5. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Rowley WA. Interval flights and glycogen utilization by mosquito Culex tarsalis. J Insect Physiol. 1970;16:1839–&. doi: 10.1016/0022-1910(70)90230-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Wigglesworth VB. The utilization of reserve substances in Drosophila during flight. J Exp Biol. 1949;26:150–163. doi: 10.1242/jeb.26.2.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Gade G, Auerswald L. Mode of action of neuropeptides from the adipokinetic hormone family. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2003;132:10. doi: 10.1016/s0016-6480(03)00159-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Graves JL, Toolson EC, Jeong C, Vu LN, Rose MR. Desiccation, flight, glycogen, and postponed senescence in Drosophila melanogaster. Physiol Zool. 1992;65:268–286. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Gibbs AG, Chippindale AK, Rose MR. Physiological mechanisms of evolved desiccation resistance in Drosophila melanogaster. J Exp Biol. 1997;200:1821–1832. doi: 10.1242/jeb.200.12.1821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hoffmann AA, Parsons PA. Selection for increased desiccation resistance in Drosophila melanogaster: Additive genetic control and correlated responses for other stresses. Genetics. 1989;122:837. doi: 10.1093/genetics/122.4.837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Hoffmann AA, Harshman LG. Desiccation and starvation resistance in Drosophila: Patterns of variation at the species, population and intrapopulation levels. Heredity. 1999;83(Pt 6):637. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2540.1999.00649.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Ballard JWO, Melvin RG, Simpson SJ. Starvation resistance is positively correlated with body lipid proportion in five wild caught drosophila simulans populations. J Insect Physiol. 2008;54:1371–1376. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2008.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Zhao Z, Zera AJ. Differential lipid biosynthesis underlies a tradeoff between reproduction and flight capability in a wing-polymorphic cricket. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:16829–16834. doi: 10.1073/pnas.262533999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Zera AJ, Zhao Z. Morph-dependent fatty acid oxidation in a wing-polymorphic cricket: implications for the trade-off between dispersal and reproduction. J Insect Physiol. 2003;49:933–943. doi: 10.1016/s0022-1910(03)00152-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Houle D. Genetic covariance of fitness correlates: What genetic correlations are made of and why it matters. Evolution. 1991;45:630–648. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1991.tb04334.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Ota KT, Pierre VJ, Ploski JE, Queen K, Schafe GE. The NO-cGMP-PKG signaling pathway regulates synaptic plasticity and fear memory consolidation in the lateral amygdala via activation of ERK/MAP Kinase. Learn Mem. 2008;15:792–805. doi: 10.1101/lm.1114808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Feil R, Hofmann F, Kleppisch T. Function of cGMP-dependent protein kinases in the nervous system. Rev Neurosci. 2005;16:23–41. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2005.16.1.23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Liu S, Rao Y, Daw N. Roles of Protein Kinase A and Protein Kinase G in synaptic plasticity in the visual cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:864–869. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.8.864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Wang X, Robinson P. Cyclic GMP-dependent protein kinase and cellular signaling in the nervous system. J Neurochem. 1997;68:443–456. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1997.68020443.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Dostmann WR, Tegge W, Frank R, Nickl CK, Taylor MS, et al. Exploring the mechanisms of vascular smooth muscle tone with highly specific, membrane-permeable inhibitors of cyclic GMP-dependent Protein Kinase I-alpha. Pharmacol Ther. 2002;93:203–215. doi: 10.1016/s0163-7258(02)00189-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Stuart JM, Segal E, Koller D, Kim SK. A gene-coexpression network for global discovery of conserved genetic modules. Science. 2003;302:249–255. doi: 10.1126/science.1087447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Engel JE, Wu CF. Altered habituation of an identified escape circuit in Drosophila memory mutants. J Neurosci. 1996;16:3486–3499. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-10-03486.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Engel JE, Wu CF. Genetic dissection of functional contributions of specific potassium channel subunits in habituation of an escape circuit in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 1998;18:2254–2267. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-06-02254.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Renger JJ, Yao WD, Sokolowski MB, Wu CF. Neuronal polymorphism among natural alleles of a cGMP-dependent kinase gene, foraging, in drosophila. J Neurosci. 1999;19:RC28. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-19-j0002.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Dawson-Scully K, Armstrong GA, Kent C, Robertson RM, Sokolowski MB. Natural variation in the thermotolerance of neural function and behavior due to a cGMP-dependent protein kinase. PLoS ONE. 2007;2:e773. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000773. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Greenspan RJ. The flexible genome. Nature Rev Genet. 2001;2:383–387. doi: 10.1038/35072018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Featherstone DE, Broadie K. Wrestling with pleiotropy: genomic and topological analysis of the yeast gene expression network. BioEssays. 2002;24:267–274. doi: 10.1002/bies.10054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.van Swinderen B, Greenspan RJ. Flexibility in a gene network affecting a simple behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics. 2005;169:2151–2163. doi: 10.1534/genetics.104.032631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Promislow DE. Protein networks, pleiotropy and the evolution of senescence. Proc Biol Sci. 2004;271:1225–1234. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Benfey PN, Mitchell-Olds T. From genotype to phenotype: systems biology meets natural variation. Science. 2008;320:495–497. doi: 10.1126/science.1153716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Greenspan RJ. A kinder, gentler genetic analysis of behavior: Dissection gives way to modulation. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1997;7:805–811. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(97)80139-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Fitzpatrick MJ, Feder E, Rowe L, Sokolowski MB. Maintaining a behaviour polymorphism by frequency-dependent selection on a single gene. Nature. 2007;447:210–212. doi: 10.1038/nature05764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Toma DP, White KP, Hirsch J, Greenspan RJ. Identification of genes involved in Drosophila melanogaster geotaxis, a complex behavioral trait. Nat Genet. 2002;31:349–353. doi: 10.1038/ng893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Oldham S, Stocker H, Laffargue M, Wittwer F, Wymann M, et al. The Drosophila insulin/IGF receptor controls growth and size by modulating PTDinsP(3) levels. Development. 2002;129:4103–4109. doi: 10.1242/dev.129.17.4103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Clancy DJ, Gems D, Harshman LG, Oldham S, Stocker H, et al. Extension of life-span by loss of chico, a Drosophila insulin receptor substrate protein. Science. 2001;292:104–106. doi: 10.1126/science.1057991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Weinkove D, Neufeld TP, Twardzik T, Waterfield MD, Leevers SJ. Regulation of imaginal disc cell size, cell number and organ size by Drosophila class I(a) Phosphoinositide 3-Kinase and its adaptor. Curr Biol. 1999;9:1019–1029. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(99)80450-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Zulak KG, Cornish A, Daskalchuk TE, Deyholos MK, Goodenowe DB, et al. Gene transcript and metabolite profiling of Elicitor-induced opium poppy cell cultures reveals the coordinate regulation of primary and secondary metabolism. Planta. 2007;225:1085–1106. doi: 10.1007/s00425-006-0419-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Gray GR, Heath D. A global reorganization of the metabolome in Arabidopsis during cold acclimation is revealed by metabolic fingerprinting. Physiol Plantarum. 2005;124:236–248. [Google Scholar]
- 86.Kanehisa M, Goto S, Hattori M, Aoki-Kinoshita KF, Itoh M, et al. From genomics to chemical genomics: new developments in KEGG. Nucl Acids Res. 2006;34:D354–D357. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkj102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Smith CA, O'Maille G, Want EJ, Qin C, Trauger SA, et al. Metlin: A metabolite mass spectral database. Ther Drug Monit. 2005;27:747–751. doi: 10.1097/01.ftd.0000179845.53213.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Clark AG. Causes and consequences of variation in energy storage in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics. 1989;123:131–144. doi: 10.1093/genetics/123.1.131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Kunst A, et al. Metabolites 1: Carbohydrates. In: Bergmeyer HU, editor. Weinheim: Verlag Chemie; 1984. 163p. 3rd – ed. [Google Scholar]
- 90.Dierick HA, Greenspan RJ. Molecular analysis of flies selected for aggressive behavior. Nat Genet. 2006;38:1023–1031. doi: 10.1038/ng1864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Bolstad BM, Irizarry RA, Astrand M, Speed TP. A comparison of normalization methods for high density oligonucleotide array data based on variance and bias. Bioinformatics. 2003;19:185–193. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/19.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Schwarz G. Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann Statist. 1978;6:461–464. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.