Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Aug 5.
Published in final edited form as: J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008 Jan;49(1):88–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01819.x

Neural connectivity in children with bipolar disorder: impairment in the face emotion processing circuit

Brendan A Rich 1, Stephen J Fromm 1, Lisa H Berghorst 1, Daniel P Dickstein 1, Melissa A Brotman 1, Daniel S Pine 1, Ellen Leibenluft 1
PMCID: PMC2721276  NIHMSID: NIHMS124840  PMID: 18181882

Abstract

Background

Pediatric bipolar disorder (BD), a highly debilitating illness, is characterized by amygdala abnormalities, i.e., volume reduction and hyperactivation during face processing. Evidence of perturbed amygdala functional connectivity with other brain regions would implicate a distributed neural circuit in the pathophysiology of BD, and would further elucidate the neural mechanisms associated with BD face emotion misinterpretation.

Methods

Thirty-three BD and 24 healthy age, gender, and IQ-matched subjects completed a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task of face emotion identification in which attention was directed to emotional (hostility, fearfulness) and nonemotional (nose width) aspects of faces. Voxel-wise analyses examined whole brain functional connectivity with the left amygdala.

Results

Compared to healthy subjects, BD subjects had significantly reduced connectivity between the left amygdala and two regions: right posterior cingulate/precuneus and right fusiform gyrus/parahippocampal gyrus. Deficits were evident regardless of mood state and comorbid diagnoses.

Conclusions

BD youth exhibit deficient connectivity between the amygdala and temporal association cortical regions previously implicated in processing facial expressions and social stimuli. In conjunction with previously documented volumetric and functional perturbations in these brain regions, dysfunction in this distributed neural circuit may begin to clarify the pathophysiology of the face emotion misperceptions and social deficits seen in BD youth.

Keywords: Bipolar disorder, children, face perception, neural connectivity, amygdala


Pediatric bipolar disorder (BD) is the focus of increasing research interest. Approximately 1% of children and adolescents are diagnosed with BD (Johnson et al., 2000), a highly debilitating psychiatric illness. One study found that nearly 65% of BD youth had attempted suicide and 75% had been psychiatrically hospitalized (Dickstein et al., 2005b). Typically, BD youth have deficient social skills and few friendships (Geller et al., 2000; Lewinsohn et al., 2003).

Recent studies document impaired face-emotion labeling in BD youth, a deficit which has been previously associated with social impairment (De Sonneville et al., 2002). Specifically, BD youth misclassify emotional facial expressions (Guyer et al., 2007; McClure et al., 2005), and misinterpret neutral faces as being significantly more threatening than do controls (Rich et al., 2006). In the latter study, negative misinterpretations by BD youth were associated with hyperactivation of the left amygdala: increasingly negative misperceptions of neutral expressions were correlated with increased amygdala activation. The amygdala mediates emotional processing (LeDoux, 2000), and studies document amygdala volumetric deficits in BD youth (Blumberg et al., 2005; Chang et al., 2005; Chen et al., 2004; Delbello et al., 2004; Dickstein et al., 2005a). Thus, affective neuroscience research using structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) implicates the amygdala in pediatric BD.

However, brain regions are not isolated entities but instead form highly interconnected neural circuits. Therefore, it is important to not only identify brain regions that are dysfunctional in pediatric BD, but also to ascertain whether the connections among nodes of neural circuits differ between BD patients and controls. We examined this with functional connectivity, which provides a measure of the temporal correlation between neurophysiological measurements from different brain regions (Friston et al., 1993; Friston et al., 1997). While this technique cannot describe signal direction nor account for intermediate or multiple connections, it can elucidate the extent to which the activity in one brain area is associated with the activity in another brain area.

The current study compares BD youth to healthy controls on neural functional connectivity during a face processing task. The left amygdala served as our reference area based on our prior study which identified amygdala hyperactivation when BD youth misinterpreted face expressions (Rich et al., 2006). Because this was the first study of functional connectivity in BD youth, we viewed this study as exploratory. However, while we mapped amygdala connectivity across the entire brain, we were particularly interested in the temporal association cortex, given its role in face-processing (Critchley et al., 2000; Haxby et al., 2000).

Methods

Inclusion/exclusion criteria

BD (N = 33) and control (N = 24) subjects were recruited as described previously (Rich et al., 2006). The National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) IRB approved this study. Parents and children gave written informed consent/assent. BD inclusion criteria required subjects ages 7–18 to meet DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for BD, with the strict requirements of a history of at least one full duration hypomanic or manic episode – i.e., lasting ≥4 days for hypomania or ≥7 days for mania – with abnormally elevated or expansive mood and/or grandiosity, and at least three DSM-IV criterion ‘B’ mania symptoms (Leibenluft et al., 2003). Diagnoses were made by master’s-level clinicians using the Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders (K-SADS-PL; Kaufman et al., 1997). Control subjects and a parent also completed the K-SADS-PL to ensure that the subject had no psychiatric history. The control sample included three pairs of siblings. As a result, in secondary analyses we randomly removed a sibling from each pair to determine if this altered our results (see Results below). To evaluate current mood, clinicians administered the Children’s Depression Rating Scale (CDRS; Poznanski et al., 1984) and Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS; Young et al., 1978) to patients and their parents within 24 hours of scanning. General functioning was measured using the Children’s Global Assessment Scale (CGAS; Shaffer et al., 1983).

Exclusion criteria included IQ <70, pervasive developmental disorder, psychosis that interfered with study compliance, unstable medical illness, and substance abuse within three months.

fMRI task

As described elsewhere (Rich et al., 2006), the faces fMRI task consisted of participants viewing four face types (happy, angry, fearful, and neutral) selected from standardized sets (Ekman & Friesen, 1976; Gur et al., 2001). Subjects used a button box (MRI Devices, Waukesha, WI) to conduct one of three ratings of the face on a 5-point scale: 1) the threat-level of the face (‘How hostile is the face?’); 2) their fearful response to the face (‘How afraid are you?’); or 3) a non-emotional facial feature (‘How wide is the nose?’). A fourth passive face viewing condition was also included.

The 160-trial run was divided into blocks comprised of 10 trials: eight faces (two of each face type) and 2 fixations. At the beginning of each block, subjects were presented with a screen instructing them to conduct one of the three rating types or perform passive viewing throughout that block. Face stimuli consisted of 32 different actors, with each actor presenting just one face emotion. Order of face type/fixation were randomized within each block, and order of block type (e.g., ratings type/passive viewing) and actor-emotion pairings were randomized across participants. For each block, instructions were presented for 3000 ms and each face or fixation was displayed for 4000 ms, followed by a 750–1250 ms inter-trial interval. We used the rapid event related paradigm of Friston et al. (1998) and Zarahn and Slifstein (2001), which allowed us to analyze trials by face and ratings type. Behavioral data consisted of face ratings and the reaction time (RT) required to make these ratings.

MRI data acquisition

Whole-brain fMRI data were acquired on a General Electric Signa 3T scanner. Following localization and shimming, T2* weighted images were acquired using echo-planar single-shot gradient echo imaging (64 × 64 matrix, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 40 ms, FOV = 240 mm, 3.75 × 3.75 × 5 mm voxels, 23 contiguous 5 mm axial slices parallel to the AC-PC line). High-resolution T1 weighted anatomical images were acquired to aid with spatial normalization (180 1 mm sagittal slices, FOV = 256, NEX = 1, TR = 11.4 ms, TE = 4.4 ms, matrix = 256 × 256, TI = 300 ms, bandwidth = 130 Hz/pixel, 22 kHz/256 pixels).

fMRI pre-processing

Analyses were conducted with Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) software (SPM99, Wellcome Department of Neurology, London, UK). Data were corrected for slice timing and motion, co-registered to the anatomical data, spatially normalized, and re-sliced into isotropic 2 mm voxels. The data were spatially smoothed with a Gaussian kernel (FWHM = 8). See Rich et al. (2006) for a more detailed description of the initial data analysis procedures.

Functional connectivity analyses

For each subject, functional connectivity was measured by examining the temporal correlation of the signal between the left amygdala and the signal from each of the other voxels in the brain (Friston et al., 1997; Greicius et al., 2003). This signal was collected for the entire duration of the experiment, i.e., across all face stimuli and ratings. At each voxel in the brain, the time series was normalized by the grand mean (the mean over all voxels in the brain and all time points), detrended and then bandpass filtered (frequency cutoffs at .0083 and .15 Hz). A general linear model was applied to this data using SPM. The user-specified single regressor of interest was the amygdala time-series; specifically, we extracted the time-series from the left amygdala by taking the mean over all amygdalar voxels at each time point in the temporally filtered time series. The global signal and six motion parameters were treated as regressors of no interest. The regression coefficient corresponding to the regressor of interest was entered into a second, group-level analysis. The threshold for significance was p < .0001 uncorrected. We transformed Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates in SPM to Talairach coordinates and then labeled the regions using Analyses of Functional Neuroimages (AFNI).

Statistical tests

For functional connectivity analyses of fMRI data, we used t-tests generated from the group-level random effects model. These tests examined amygdala connectivity within each sample and group differences between BD subjects and controls. To minimize Type I errors, the Greenhouse–Geisser procedure was applied when appropriate. Data were analyzed using SPSS version 14.0 (SPSS, 2005).

Results

Participant demographics and clinical characteristics (Table 1)

Table 1.

Clinical characteristics of the sample

Pediatric bipolar disorder Control p value
N 33 24
Age (years) 14.4 ± 3.0 14.4 ± 2.2 .98
Sex: male 39.4 (13) 37.5 (9) .55
Ethnicity: Caucasian 90.9 (30) 58.3 (14) .01
FSIQ 108.6 ± 10.9 110.6 + 12.7 .53
Number of diagnoses 2.7 ± 1.4
Comorbid diagnoses 75.8 (25)
Bipolar Disorder I 90.9 (30)
  ADHD 48.5 (16)
  ODD 24.2 (8)
  Any anxiety 39.4 (13)
  Separation anxiety 24.2 (8)
  GAD 15.2 (5)
Medication use 81.8 (27)
  Antipsychotics 51.5 (17)
  Mood stabilizers 39.4 (13)
  Antidepressants 30.3 (10)
  Lithium 30.3 (10)
  Anticonvulsants 27.3 (9)
  Stimulants 27.3 (9)
  Sedatives 12.1 (4)
Clinical ratings
  CGAS 54.0 ± 14.0
  CDRS 28.7 ± 8.8
  YMRS 9.2 ± 6.3

Note. Data are presented as mean ± standard deviation or percent of sample (number of subjects). FSIQ = Full Scale IQ as measured by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). All diagnoses are current and based on DSM-IV criteria. ADHD = Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; ODD = Oppositional defiant disorder; GAD = Generalized anxiety disorder; CGAS = Children’s Global Assessment Scale; CDRS = Children’s Depression Rating Scale; YMRS = Young Mania Rating Scale.

BD patients (N = 33) and controls (N = 24) did not differ on age (BD = 14.4 ± 3.0 yrs; control = 14.4 ± 2.2 yrs), sex (male: BD = 39.4%; control = 37.5%), or IQ (BD = 108.6 ± 10.9; control = 110.6 ± 12.7) as measured with the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, 1999). There was a significantly greater proportion of Caucasians in the BD sample (X2 = 11.01, p = .01). Demographic similarity was maintained when we randomly removed one sibling from each of the three sibling pairs. Criteria for Bipolar I were met by 90.9% (N = 30) of BD patients; 75.8% (N = 25) had at least one comorbid diagnosis (mean number of diagnoses = 2.7 ± 1.4); 81.8% (N = 27) of patients were medicated when scanned (mean = 2.5 ± 1.7 medications per subject).

CDRS and YMRS scores showed that 54.5% (N = 18) of BD patients were euthymic at the time of scanning, 33.3% (N = 11) were hypomanic (YMRS > 12 but <26, CDRS < 40), 9.1% (N = 3) were depressed (CDRS > 40, YMRS <12), and 3.0% (N = 1) were mixed (CDRS > 40 and YMRS > 12). Patients’ CGAS score (54.0 ± 14.0) indicated moderate impairment.

Functional connectivity results

Relative to healthy subjects, BD youth exhibited significantly lower functional connectivity between the left amygdala and two regions: 1) a region bordering the right posterior cingulate and precuneus (24, −66, 20) [t(56) = 4.18, p < .0001 uncorrected] (Figure 1 and Table 2), and 2) a region bordering the right fusiform gyrus (FG) and parahippocampal gyrus (PHG) (24, −38, −14) [t(56) = 4.01, p < .0001 uncorrected] (Figure 2 and Table 2).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Neural connectivity between left amygdala and right posterior cingulate/precuneus. BD subjects (N = 33) have significantly lower connectivity than controls (N = 24) (t = 4.18, p < .0001 uncorrected) between the left amygdala and the posterior cingulate/precuneus (24, −66, 20)

Table 2.

Regions where pediatric bipolar subjects, compared to controls, had significantly lower connectivity with the left amygdala

MNI coordinates
Region of interest x y z t p Number of voxels (ke)
Posterior cingulate/precuneus 24 −66 20 4.18 <.0001 7
Fusiform gyrus/parahippocampal gyrus 24 −38 −14 4.01 <.0001 2

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Neural connectivity between left amygdala and right fusiform gyrus/parahippocampal gyrus. BD subjects (N = 33) have significantly less connectivity than controls (N = 24) (t = 4.01, p < .0001 uncorrected) between the left amygdala and the right fusiform gyrus/parahippocampal gyrus (24, −38, −14)

When we randomly removed one sibling from each of the three sibling pairs and used peak voxel data to compare the groups, controls continued to have significantly greater connectivity than BD youth in the posterior cingulate/precuneus [t(53) = 6.86, p < .0001] and the FG/PHG [t(53) = 7.03, p < .01].

Role of demographic variables, comorbid diagnoses, and medication use

Connectivity was not correlated with BD patients’ mood during testing, as measured by CDRS or YMRS scores. In addition, an ANOVA examined functional connectivity between euthymic BD subjects (N = 18), noneuthymic BD subjects (N = 15), and controls. There were group differences at both the posterior cingulate/precuneus [F(2,54) = 13.75, p < .001] and FG/PHG [F(2,54) = 3.95, p = .03]. Specifically, both euthymic and noneuthymic BD youth had significantly lower connectivity than controls between the amygdala and both neural regions [posterior cingulate/precuneus: euthymic BD subjects vs. controls (p < .001), noneuthymic BD subjects vs. controls (p = .04); FG/PHG: euthymic BD subjects vs. controls (p = .01), noneuthymic BD subjects vs. controls (p = .05)]. Thus, weaker neural connectivity in BD youth was not related to mood state.

Regarding comorbid diagnoses, separate ANOVAs comparing controls to BD subjects with vs. without comorbid attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (with: N = 16; without: N = 17), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) (with: N = 8; without: N = 25), and anxiety disorders (with: N = 13; without: N = 20) found that BD youth without these comorbid disorders had significantly lower connectivity than controls at both the posterior cingulate/precuneus [ADHD: F(2,54) = 9.23, p < .001; BD without ADHD vs. controls p = .003; ODD: F(2,54) = 11.04, p < .001, BD without ODD vs. controls p = .001; anxiety: F(2,54) = 10.20, p < .001; BD without anxiety vs. controls p < .001)] and FG/PHG regions [ADHD: F(2,54) = 5.62, p = .006; BD without ADHD vs. controls p = .001; ODD: F(2,54) = 4.20, p = .02, BD without ODD vs. controls p = .005; anxiety: F(2,54) = 4.60, p = .02; BD without anxiety vs. controls p = .01)] Thus, connectivity deficits were evident in BD youth without ADHD, ODD, or anxiety.

Examining the impact of medication, there were too few unmedicated BD subjects (N = 6) to compare this sample to controls. Bivariate correlations did not find a significant relationship between number of medications and connectivity in BD youth [posterior cingulate/precuneus (r = .24, p = .17); FG/PHG (r = .15, p = .42)]. We then conducted ANOVA comparisons of controls to those BD subjects on vs. off specific classes of medications (i.e., antipsychotics, mood stabilizers, antidepressants, anticonvulsants, stimulants, sedatives, lithium; none mutually exclusive). When each medication class was examined separately (e.g., BD youth taking lithium and possibly other medications vs. BD youth not taking lithium but possibly taking other medications), there was significantly diminished connectivity between the amygdala and posterior cingulate/precuneus and FG/PHG in BD youth not receiving that medication class vs. controls (all p’s < .02). Thus, lower connectivity in BD youth does not appear to be driven by the presence of a particular medication.

Discussion

Prior behavioral and fMRI studies demonstrate face processing deficits in pediatric bipolar disorder (BD; McClure et al., 2005; Rich et al., 2006). Specifically, compared to healthy subjects, BD youth: 1) view neutral faces as more threatening; and 2) have increased left amygdala activity when rating their fear of, or the hostility on, neutral faces (Rich et al., 2006). To expand on these findings, we compared functional connectivity (Friston et al., 1997) between the left amygdala and other brain regions in BD youth versus healthy controls when processing facial expressions. Functional connectivity provides a measure of the temporal correlation of activity between different brain regions. We found that BD youth have significantly less functional connectivity between the left amygdala and a network involving a region bordering the right posterior cingulate/precuneus, and another region bordering the right fusiform gyrus (FG)/parahippocampal gyrus (PHG).

Our results indicate that BD youth display impaired signal communication in neural networks critical to processing faces and emotional stimuli. Both the FG and posterior cingulate are associated with the visual processing of facial expressions. Specifically, the FG is specialized in processing face identity and, to a lesser extent, face expression (Haxby et al., 2000; Kanwisher et al., 1997), while the posterior cingulate responds to threatening stimuli that are of immediate social importance (Critchley et al., 2000). Further, the PHG has been implicated in emotional and cognitive learning, specifically associating stimuli with affective meaning (Fanselow & LeDoux, 1999; Yaniv et al., 2000). Face emotion classification is thought to reflect, in part, the amygdala-FG-posterior cingulate network (Haxby et al., 2002), while the amygdala receives input from the PHG (Shi & Cassell, 1999). Thus, disruption of these circuits may explain partly the face emotion misperception seen in BD youth.

The previously documented amygdala hyperactivity in BD youth (Rich et al., 2006) may reflect the end result of deficient signal transmission from a network involving the FG, posterior cingulate, or PHG to the amygdala. The amygdala regulates the attribution of emotional significance to threatening faces (Calder et al., 2001). Thus, if the amygdala fails to receive from the FG, posterior cingulate, or PHG information regarding face expression or stimulus affective meaning, BD youth may aberrantly perceive neutral faces as threatening. Conversely, information is transmitted from the amygdala to regions of the temporal association cortex (LeDoux, 2000). Impaired connectivity in BD youth may mean that such information is transmitted incompletely to regions such as the FG or posterior cingulate, possibly resulting in face emotion misperceptions.

In addition to elucidating possible neural correlates of face misperception in pediatric BD, our results may help to explain the social deficits seen in these youth (Geller et al., 2000; Lewinsohn et al., 2003). The coordinated function of the amygdala-FG-posterior cingulate circuit mediates not only face emotion processing, as already noted, but also a diverse set of social functions, including interpretation of social signals and regulation of emotional and behavioral responses to these signals (Amaral et al., 1992; Critchley et al., 2000; Haxby et al., 2002). Further, the PHG is thought to be the ‘gateway’ for cortical sensory input to the amygdala before this information is sent to neural regions responsible for regulating behavior (Kolb, 1990). Thus, disruption of these circuits, as tentatively documented here, is likely to impair social signal processing and emotional and behavioral regulation, which in turn is likely to result in the social deficits that characterize BD youth.

The involvement of the posterior cingulate, precuneus, and PHG also indicate that memory may be a factor in the face misperception and social deficits seen in BD youth. Memory impairments are thought to be a trait marker of adult and pediatric BD (Martinez-Aran et al., 2000; Pavuluri et al., 2006b), and prior research finds diminished recall of face expressions by BD youth (Dickstein et al., in press). The posterior cingulate (Maddock et al., 2003) and precuneus (Cavanna & Trimble, 2006) play primary roles in retrieving prior experiences and regulating the impact of emotional salience on memory, and the amygdale–PHG circuit has been implicated in the formation of aversive emotional memories (Fanselow & LeDoux, 1999; Yaniv et al., 2000). Impaired connectivity between the amygdala and the posterior cingulate, FG, and PHG may lead to inadequate recall of prior social encounters which would otherwise serve to shape BD youth’s face processing and social function.

This study broadens our understanding of the pathophysiology of pediatric BD. Our results expand upon prior research which has documented in BD youth volumetric deficits and functional aberrations in the FG (Frazier et al., 2005a; Pavuluri et al., 2006a), posterior cingulate (Chang et al., 2004; Kaur et al., 2005), and amygdala (Blumberg et al., 2003; Blumberg et al., 2005; Chang et al., 2005; Chen et al., 2004; Delbello et al., 2004; Dickstein et al., 2005a; Rich et al., 2006). Thus, neuroimaging research is consistent in implicating volumetric, functional, and now connectivity impairments of the amygdala, FG, and posterior cingulate in the pathophysiology of pediatric BD.

Although memory deficits are thought to be trait markers of BD, as noted above, there is heterogeneity in studies investigating potential neural correlates. We are not aware of functional nor structural studies reporting perturbations in the precuneus or PHG in pediatric BD. In BD adults, memory tasks have evoked both heightened (Deckersbach et al., 2006) and diminished (Malhi et al., 2007) PHG activation, as well as hypoactivation of the precuneus (Malhi et al., 2007). If discussion of the neural correlates of memory is expanded to include the hippocampal formation, results of structural studies are inconsistent in both BD youth (Blumberg et al., 2003; Chang et al., 2005; Chen et al., 2004; Dickstein et al., 2005a; Frazier et al., 2005b) and adults (Altshuler et al., 2000; Hauser et al., 2000; Strakowski et al., 1999; Strasser et al., 2005; Yucel et al., 2007). Hippocampal hyperactivation has been reported in BD adults during a memory task involving word lists (Deckersbach et al., 2006), but it is unclear how this relates to a visual task such as face perception. Further, there are no reports of hippocampal functional perturbations in pediatric BD. Clearly, additional research is required to better understand the role of the precuneus and PHG, or even more generally the hippocampal formation, in BD.

A primary limitation of our study is the clinical diversity of our BD sample, though this is typical of that seen in other samples of BD youth (Birmaher et al., 2006). While there were high rates of comorbidity in our BD patients, the fact that BD youth without comorbid ADHD, ODD, or anxiety had significantly lower connectivity than controls indicates that the deficit may reflect BD itself rather than the presence of a co-occurring disorder.

Another possible confounding variable is mood state, given that in BD adults face emotional identification and its neural correlates may differ between euthymic (Bozikas et al., 2006; Getz et al., 2003; Harmer et al., 2002; Yurgelun-Todd et al., 2000) and manic (Lembke & Ketter, 2002; Lennox et al., 2004) subjects. However, we found that both euthymic and noneuthymic BD youth had significantly lower functional connectivity than controls, indicating that these neural deficits may be trait-based and not mood-state dependent.

Medication status is an additional potential confound. Since most BD patients were medicated, we were unable to fully determine the extent to which our results were associated with BD or, instead, with medication effects. However, there was no relationship between number of medications and connectivity, and lower connectivity in BD youth did not seem to be due to the presence of a particular class of medication. The results of all post hoc analyses should be interpreted with caution because they are subject to Type I error due to multiple comparisons and Type II error due to small sample sizes.

Finally, it is important to note that an alternate explanation for our results is differences in measurement error between the groups, which would impact the estimated regression coefficient magnitude via attenuation bias (van Belle & Fisher, 1996). Further, it is possible that a hypothesis- driven study may fail to confirm the preliminary results presented here.

Conclusion

In sum, our results in BD youth suggest deficient functional connectivity among neural regions mediating the evaluation of facial emotional expressions. When we examined connectivity between the left amygdala and the entire brain, exploratory data implicate regions primarily associated with processing face expressions (fusiform gyrus), social information (the posterior cingulate/precuneus), and emotional learning and memory (parahippocampal gyrus, precuneus). Given that this is the first study of functional connectivity in BD youth, our results should be viewed as preliminary. The use of samples with different clinical characteristics, or alternate methodology parameters and cognitive-emotional tasks, may identify other networks with impaired connectivity in BD youth. Since our technique measures connectivity strength but not direction, future studies using effective connectivity methods will enable exploration of the directionality of neural circuitry and provide a better understanding of dysfunction in the limbic-temporal association cortex circuit in BD youth.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH, NIMH. We gratefully acknowledge the children and families of patients and controls. We also acknowledge the staff of the Bipolar Spectrum Disorders Unit at the NIMH.

Abbreviations

ADHD

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

BD

bipolar disorder

CDRS

Children’s Depression Rating Scale

fMRI

functional magnetic resonance imaging

FG

fusiform gyrus

K-SADS-PL

Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders

ODD

oppositional-defiant disorder

PHG

parahippocampal gyrus

YMRS

Young Mania Rating Scale

Footnotes

Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared.

References

  1. Altshuler LL, Bartzokis G, Grieder T, Curran J, Jimenez T, Leight K, Wilkins J, Gerner R, Mintz J. An MRI study of temporal lobe structures in men with bipolar disorder or schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;48:147–162. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00836-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amaral D, Price JL, Pitaken A, Carmichael S. In: The amygdala: Neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory and mental dysfunction. Aggleton JP, editor. New York: Wiley-Liss; 1992. pp. 1–66. [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  4. Birmaher B, Axelson D, Strober M, Gill MK, Valeri S, Chiappetta L, Ryan N, Leonard H, Hunt J, Iyengar S, Keller M. Clinical course of children and adolescents with bipolar spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:175–183. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.2.175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blumberg HP, Fredericks C, Wang F, Kalmar JH, Spencer L, Papademetris X, Pittman B, Martin A, Peterson BS, Fulbright RK, Krystal JH. Preliminary evidence for persistent abnormalities in amygdala volumes in adolescents and young adults with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorder. 2005;7:570–576. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00264.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Blumberg HP, Kaufman J, Martin A, Whiteman R, Zhang JH, Gore JC, Charney DS, Krystal JH, Peterson BS. Amygdala and hippocampal volumes in adolescents and adults with bipolar disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:1201–1208. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bozikas VP, Tonia T, Fokas K, Karavatos A, Kosmidis MH. Impaired emotion processing in remitted patients with bipolar disorder. Journal of Affect Disorders. 2006;91:53–56. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Calder AJ, Lawrence AD, Young AW. Neuropsychology of fear and loathing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2001;2:352–363. doi: 10.1038/35072584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. The precuneus: A review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. 2006;129:564–583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chang K, Adleman NE, Dienes K, Simeonova DI, Menon V, Reiss A. Anomalous prefrontal-subcortical activation in familial pediatric bipolar disorder: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61:781–792. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chang K, Karchemskiy A, Barnea-Goraly N, Garrett A, Simeonova DI, Reiss A. Reduced amygdalar gray matter volume in familial pediatric bipolar disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2005;44:565–573. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000159948.75136.0d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chen BK, Sassi R, Axelson D, Hatch JP, Sanches M, Nicoletti M, Brambilla P, Keshavan MS, Ryan ND, Birmaher B, Soares JC. Cross-sectional study of abnormal amygdala development in adolescents and young adults with bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;56:399–405. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Critchley H, Daly E, Phillips M, Brammer M, Bullmore E, Williams S, Van Amelsvoort T, Robertson D, David A, Murphy D. Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Human Brain Mapping. 2000;9:93–105. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(200002)9:2&#x0003c;93::AID-HBM4&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. De Sonneville LM, Verschoor CA, Njiokiktjien C, Op het Veld V, Toorenaar N, Vranken M. Facial identity and facial emotions: Speed, accuracy, and processing strategies in children and adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2002;24:200–213. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.2.200.989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Deckersbach T, Dougherty DD, Savage C, McMurrich S, Fischman AJ, Nierenberg A, Sachs G, Rauch SL. Impaired recruitment of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and hippocampus during encoding in bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;59:138–146. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Delbello MP, Zimmerman ME, Mills NP, Getz GE, Strakowski SM. Magnetic resonance imaging analysis of amygdala and other subcortical brain regions in adolescents with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorder. 2004;6:43–52. doi: 10.1046/j.1399-5618.2003.00087.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dickstein DP, Milham MP, Nugent AC, Drevets WC, Charney DS, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Frontotemporal alterations in pediatric bipolar disorder: Results of a voxel-based morphometry study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005a;62:734–741. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.7.734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dickstein DP, Rich BA, Binstock AB, Pradella AG, Towbin KE, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Comorbid anxiety in phenotypes of pediatric bipolar disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 2005b;15:534–548. doi: 10.1089/cap.2005.15.534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dickstein DP, Rich BA, Roberson-Nay R, Berghorst L, Vinton D, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Neural activation during encoding of emotional faces in pediatric bipolar disorder. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00418.x. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  21. Fanselow MS, LeDoux JE. Why we think plasticity underlying Pavlovian fear conditioning occurs in the basolateral amygdala. Neuron. 1999;23:229–232. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80775-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Frazier JA, Breeze JL, Makris N, Giuliano AS, Herbert MR, Seidman L, Biederman J, Hodge SM, Dieterich ME, Gerstein ED, Kennedy DN, Rauch SL, Cohen BM, Caviness VS. Cortical gray matter differences identified by structural magnetic resonance imaging in pediatric bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorder. 2005a;7:555–569. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00258.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Frazier JA, Chiu S, Breeze JL, Makris N, Lange N, Kennedy DN, Herbert MR, Bent EK, Koneru VK, Dieterich ME, Hodge SM, Rauch SL, Grant PE, Cohen BM, Seidman LJ, Caviness VS, Biederman J. Structural brain magnetic resonance imaging of limbic and thalamic volumes in pediatric bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005b;162:1256–1265. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.7.1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, Dolan RJ. Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage. 1997;6:218–229. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Friston KJ, Fletcher P, Josephs O, Holmes A, Rugg MD, Turner R. Event-related fMRI: Characterizing differential responses. Neuroimage. 1998;7:30–40. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Friston KJ, Frith CD, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RS. Functional connectivity: The principal-component analysis of large (PET) data sets. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism. 1993;13:5–14. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.1993.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Geller B, Bolhofner K, Craney JL, Williams M, Delbello MP, Gundersen K. Psychosocial functioning in a prepubertal and early adolescent bipolar disorder phenotype. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2000;39:1543–1548. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200012000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Getz GE, Shear PK, Strakowski SM. Facial affect recognition deficits in bipolar disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2003;9:623–632. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703940021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U S A. 2003;100:253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gur RC, Ragland JD, Moberg PJ, Turner TH, Bilker WB, Kohler C, Siegel SJ, Gur RE. Computerized neurocognitive scanning: I. Methodology and validation in healthy people. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;25:766–776. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00278-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Guyer AE, McClure EB, Adler A, Brotman MA, Rich BA, Pine DS, Ernst M, Leibenluft E. Specificity of facial expression labeling deficits in childhood psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:863–871. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01758.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Harmer CJ, Grayson L, Goodwin GM. Enhanced recognition of disgust in bipolar illness. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;51:298–304. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01249-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hauser P, Matochik J, Altshuler LL, Denicoff KD, Conrad A, Li X, Post RM. MRI-based measurements of temporal lobe and ventricular structures in patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2000;60:25–32. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(99)00154-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI. The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2000;4:223–233. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01482-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI. Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;51:59–67. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01330-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Johnson JG, Cohen P, Brook JS. Associations between bipolar disorder and other psychiatric disorders during adolescence and early adulthood: A community-based longitudinal investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;157:1679–1681. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. The fusiform face area: A module in human extra-striate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:4302–4311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, Ryan N. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36:980–988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kaur S, Sassi RB, Axelson D, Nicoletti M, Brambilla P, Monkul ES, Hatch JP, Keshavan MS, Ryan N, Birmaher B, Soares JC. Cingulate cortex anatomical abnormalities in children and adolescents with bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:1637–1643. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.9.1637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kolb B. Association cortex. In: Kolb B, Tees RC, editors. The cerebral cortex of the rat. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1990. pp. 431–471. [Google Scholar]
  41. LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2000;23:155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Leibenluft E, Charney DS, Towbin KE, Bhangoo RK, Pine DS. Defining clinical phenotypes of juvenile mania. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:430–437. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.3.430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lembke A, Ketter TA. Impaired recognition of facial emotion in mania. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;159:302–304. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.2.302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lennox BR, Jacob R, Calder AJ, Lupson V, Bullmore ET. Behavioural and neurocognitive responses to sad facial affect are attenuated in patients with mania. Psychological Medicine. 2004;34:795–802. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704002557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR, Klein DN. Bipolar disorders during adolescence. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2003 Suppl.:47–50. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.108.s418.10.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Maddock RJ, Garrett AS, Buonocore MH. Posterior cingulate cortex activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence decision task. Human Brain Mapping. 2003;18:30–41. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Malhi GS, Lagopoulos J, Owen AM, Ivanovski B, Shnier R, Sachdev P. Reduced activation to implicit affect induction in euthymic bipolar patients: An fMRI study. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2007;97:109–122. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Martinez-Aran A, Vieta E, Colom F, Reinares M, Benabarre A, Gasto C, Salamero M. Cognitive dysfunctions in bipolar disorder: Evidence of neuropsychological disturbances. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 2000;69:2–18. doi: 10.1159/000012361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. McClure EB, Treland JE, Snow J, Schmajuk M, Dickstein DP, Towbin KE, Charney DS, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Deficits in social cognition and response flexibility in pediatric bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:1644–1651. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.9.1644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Pavuluri MN, O’Connor MM, Harral E, Sweeney JA. Affective neural circuitry during facial emotion processing in pediatric bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2006a doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Pavuluri MN, Schenkel LS, Aryal S, Harral EM, Hill SK, Herbener ES, Sweeney JA. Neurocognitive function in unmedicated manic and medicated euthymic pediatric bipolar patients. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006b;163:286–293. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.2.286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Poznanski EO, Grossman JA, Buchsbaum Y, Banegas M, Freeman L, Gibbons R. Preliminary studies of the reliability and validity of the children’s depression rating scale. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1984;23:191–197. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198403000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Rich BA, Vinton D, Roberson-Nay R, Hommer R, Berghorst L, McClure E, Fromm S, Pine D, Leibenluft E. Limbic hyperactivation during processing of neutral facial expressions in children with bipolar disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U S A. 2006;103:8900–8905. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0603246103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Shaffer D, Gould MS, Brasic J, Ambrosini P, Fisher P, Bird H, Aluwahlia S. A Children’s Global Assessment Scale (CGAS) Archives of General Psychiatry. 1983;40:1228–1231. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790100074010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Shi CJ, Cassell MD. Perirhinal cortex projections to the amygdaloid complex and hippocampal formation in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1999;406:299–328. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19990412)406:3<299::aid-cne2>3.0.co;2-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Strakowski SM, Delbello MP, Sax KW, Zimmerman ME, Shear PK, Hawkins JM, Larson ER. Brain magnetic resonance imaging of structural abnormalities in bipolar disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56:254–260. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.3.254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Strasser HC, Lilyestrom J, Ashby ER, Honeycutt NA, Schretlen DJ, Pulver AE, Hopkins RO, DePaulo JR, Potash JB, Schweizer B, Yates KO, Kurian E, Barta PE, Pearlson GD. Hippocampal and ventricular volumes in psychotic and nonpsychotic bipolar patients compared with schizophrenia patients and community control subjects: A pilot study. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:633–639. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. van Belle G, Fisher LD. Biostatistics: A methodology for the health sciences. New York: Wiley-Interscience; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  59. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  60. Yaniv D, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE, Richter-Levin G. Perirhinal cortex and thalamic stimulation induces LTP in different areas of the amygdala. Annals of the New York Academy of Science. 2000;911:474–476. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06749.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, Meyer DA. A rating scale for mania: Reliability, validity and sensitivity. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1978;133:429–435. doi: 10.1192/bjp.133.5.429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Yucel K, Taylor VH, McKinnon MC, Macdonald K, Alda M, Young LT, Macqueen GM. Bilateral hippocampal volume increase in patients with bipolar disorder and short-term lithium treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007 doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Yurgelun-Todd DA, Gruber SA, Kanayama G, Killgore WD, Baird AA, Young AD. fMRI during affect discrimination in bipolar affective disorder. Bipolar Disorder. 2000;2:237–248. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-5618.2000.20304.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Zarahn E, Slifstein M. A reference effect approach for power analysis in fMRI. Neuroimage. 2001;14:768–779. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES