Skip to main content
Journal of Pediatric Psychology logoLink to Journal of Pediatric Psychology
. 2008 Jul 22;34(2):164–175. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn068

The Role of Cognitive Functioning in Medication Adherence of Children and Adolescents with HIV Infection

Kathleen Malee 1,, Paige L Williams 2, Grace Montepiedra 2, Sharon Nichols 3, Patricia A Sirois 4, Deborah Storm 5, John Farley 6, Betsy Kammerer 7; PACTG 219C Team*
PMCID: PMC2722123  PMID: 18647794

Abstract

Objective To evaluate the relationship between cognitive functioning and medication adherence in children and adolescents with perinatally acquired HIV infection. Methods Children and adolescents, ages 3–18 (N = 1,429), received a cognitive evaluation and adherence assessment. Multiple logistic regression models were used to identify associations between adherence and cognitive status, adjusting for potential confounding factors. Results Children's average cognitive performance was within the low-average range; 16% of children were cognitively impaired (MDI/FSIQ <70). Cognitive status was not associated with adherence to full medication regimens; however, children with borderline/low average cognitive functioning (IQ 70–84) had increased odds of nonadherence to the protease inhibitor class of antiretroviral therapy. Recent stressful life events and child health characteristics, such as HIV RNA detectability, were significantly associated with nonadherence. Conclusion Cognitive status plays a limited role in medication adherence. Child and caregiver psychosocial and health characteristics should inform interventions to support adherence.

Keywords: adolescents, children, cognitive functioning, HIV, medication adherence


Advances in medical treatment, through combination antiretroviral therapy (ART) and highly active ART (HAART) with protease inhibitors (PIs), have resulted in improved health outcomes among children with human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) infection (DeMartino et al., 2000; Gortmaker et al., 2001). Adherence to HAART regimens is difficult, however, due to the demanding nature of antiretroviral treatment and other unique challenges faced by children and adolescents with HIV and their caregivers. Antiretroviral medications, particularly regimens containing PIs, share characteristics that amplify the inherent difficulties of medication adherence in children and adolescents: poor palatability, heavy pill burden, dietary restrictions, acute and long-term side effects, and restrictions on daily schedules (Reddington et al., 2000; Van Dyke et al., 2002). Children and adolescents with HIV often face other life stressors that affect their ability to achieve adherence, including parental HIV disease, poverty, and limited or inconsistent social support (Steele, Nelson, & Cole, 2007). Adherence failure is dangerous in HIV disease as it may result in diminished treatment efficacy, development of genotypically resistant mutations, viral rebound, and subsequent reduction in HIV treatment options.

Despite the importance of adherence in HIV disease, our understanding of factors predictive of adherence in children and adolescents remains incomplete. Prior studies of adherence in children with HIV infection, while often atheoretical, have identified multiple contextual factors associated with adherence, including disease characteristics, features of the medication regimen, and characteristics of the caregiver and family, such as caregiver education and problem-solving skills, caregiver relationship to the child, and child and caregiver stress (Hammani et al., 2004; Martin et al., 2007; Mellins, Brackis-Cott, Dolezal, & Abrams, 2004; Naar-King et al., 2006; Pontali, 2005). Child and adolescent developmental issues, including older age, knowledge of HIV diagnosis, and adjustment to chronic illness have also been implicated, although findings remain inconsistent (Simoni et al., 2007; Williams et al., 2006).

Neurocognitive impairment, due to the neuropathological effects of HIV infection, is a significant complication of perinatally acquired HIV infection (Van Rie, Harrington, Dow, & Robertson, 2007). It has been associated with nonadherence in adults with HIV (Hinkin et al., 2004) and may provoke similar risk in children and adolescents. Global or mild cognitive deficits, if present, may impede or delay development of those functional skills essential for adherence, such as understanding and following parental and provider directions, remembering schedules and dosage, and communicating needs effectively.

The purpose of this study was to examine cognitive functioning in children and adolescents with HIV, and to evaluate its relationship with medication adherence. We hypothesized that adherence to ART and HAART with PIs will be compromised in the presence of neurocognitive deficits in children and adolescents with HIV infection. We also evaluated potential confounding factors, including demographic characteristics, biological markers of health, medication and adherence factors, and child and family psychosocial characteristics that have been associated with medication adherence in previous investigations.

Methods

Participants

Our analyses used cross-sectional data from Pediatric AIDS Clinical Trials Group (PACTG) Late Outcomes Protocol 219C (P219C), a prospective cohort study designed to assess long-term effects of HIV infection and ART treatment (Gortmaker et al., 2001). Children and adolescents were enrolled at over 80 participating P219C sites (see Appendix) in urban and rural areas of the US and Puerto Rico. Eligibility criteria for the present analysis were: perinatally acquired HIV, age 3–18 years, on ART during participation in P219C, at least one cognitive evaluation during the study, and an adherence assessment closest to and within 6 months of the cognitive evaluation. The study was approved by human subject research review boards at all participating sites. Written informed consent was obtained from children's parents or legal guardians or from older adolescents who could self-consent. Written assent was obtained from children ages 12 through 17 in accordance with local IRB guidelines.

In April 2004, there were 2,384 perinatally infected children enrolled in P219C; 2,191 were 3- to 18-years old at some point during study follow-up. Of these 2,191 participants, 1,682 (77%) had at least one cognitive evaluation, and 1,537 of these 1,682 (91%) had an ART adherence assessment within 6 months of the most recent cognitive evaluation. Within this subgroup of 1,537, 1,429 (93%) completed the age-appropriate cognitive measure and obtained valid scores. These 1,429 children and adolescents comprise the cohort for this study.

Approximately one-half (52%) of the participants were female, 52% were between 9 and 15 years of age, and 84% self-identified as either African-American (58%) or Latino (26%) (Table I). Forty-three percent had undetectable viral loads (HIV RNA <400 copies/ml), and 26% had a CDC class C diagnosis (severely immunocompromised). Seventy-two percent of participants received HAART with PIs. Children and adolescents lived with biological parents (42%), adult relatives (28%), such as grandparents, aunts, and cousins, and nonrelative adults (28%), including foster and adoptive parents. Fifty-six percent of caregivers had a high school education or less and 52% reported at least one recent life stressor. Forty-seven percent of participants knew their HIV diagnosis and 22% reported on their own medication adherence. Overall adherence rates were high, with 85% reporting complete adherence to all ART drugs in their regimen, and 87–90% reporting complete adherence to individual drug classes.

Table I.

Demographic, Health, and Psychosocial Characteristics of Participants (N = 1,429)

Characteristic N (%)
Female 742 (52)
Race/ethnicity
    White 204 (14)
    African-American 825 (58)
    Latino 373 (26)
    Other 27 (2)
Age at adherence visit (years)
    3–6 195 (14)
    >6–9 349 (24)
    >9–12 439 (31)
    >12–15 296 (21)
    >15–18 150 (10)
Primary caregiver
    Biological parent 605 (42)
    Nonrelative adult 407 (28)
    Adult relative 404 (28)
    Other 9 (<1)
    Self a 4 (<1)
Education of caregiver
    Grade 1–11 354 (25)
    High school graduate 444 (31)
    Some college/technical school 351 (24)
    College graduate or higher 171 (12)
    Other/not reported 109 (8)
Knows HIV status 677 (47)
Person assessing adherence
    Biological parent 512 (36)
    Other nonrelative adult 319 (22)
    Adult relative 282 (20)
    Self a 316 (22)
CD4 + (%)
    ≤15 137 (10)
    15–25 285 (20)
    ≥25 1,007 (70)
HIV-1 viral load (copies/ml)
    ≤400 617 (43)
    ≥400–10,000 455 (32)
    ≥10,000 357 (25)
CDC class Cb
    No 1,059 (74)
    Yes 367 (26)
Complete adherence over prior 3 days
    To all ART drugs 1,216 (85)
    To NRTIs 1,229/1,420 (87)
    To NNRTIs 446/494 (90)
    To PIs 918/1,055 (87)
On PI at adherence visit 1,055 (74)
Antiretroviral regimen
    HAART with PI 1,024 (72)
    HAART with NNRTI 147 (10)
    Single NRTI 15 (1)
    Dual NRTI 150 (10)
    ≥3 NRTIs 54 (4)
    Other combination therapy 39 (3)
Duration of HAART (years)
    <2 443 (31)
    2–4 600 (42)
    >4–6 326 (23)
    >6 60 (4)
Memory prompt required 415 (27)
Recent life stressors
    None 644 (45)
    At least one 738 (52)
    Missing 47 (3)
Type of life stressors
    Financial stressful event 329 (23)
    Family structure stressful event 299 (21)
    Illness/death in family/friend 416 (29)
    Any of above 738 (52)
Type of ART remindersc
    Activity of daily living 389 (27)
    Pillboxes 226 (16)
    Buddy system 110 (8)
    Labels 186 (13)
Physical/social role functioningd
    Limited activities due to illness 160 (11)
    Limited school attendance 169 (12)
    Repeated a grade 341 (24)
    Participated in sports 773 (54)
    Attended special class/help 438 (31)

aIncludes three children who had the assistance of another in reporting adherence.

bThree missing observations.

cParticipant may report use of more than one type of ART reminder.

dEach category had between 173–176 missing observations.

Measures

Cognitive Functioning

Cognitive testing was conducted at 3-year intervals, beginning at age 3 years. The Bayley Scales of Infant Development–Second Edition (Bayley-II; Bayley, 1993) were used for children between 36 and 42 months of age. Age-appropriate Wechsler tests were used for older children: Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised (WPPSI-R; Wechsler, 1989) for ages 3.5 to <6 years; Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Third Edition (WISC-III; Wechsler, 1991) for ages 6–16; and Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition (WAIS-III; Wechsler, 1997) for ages 17 and 18 years. We used the most recent cognitive assessment conducted between the ages of 3 and 18 years. Tests were administered according to standardized procedures. For those children and adolescents whose primary language was Spanish (N = 236; 16.5%) or other non-English language (N = 40; 2.8%), cognitive testing was provided by an examiner fluent in the child's primary language, or a professional translator was used.

The Mental Development Index (MDI) of the Bayley-II and the Full Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) of the Wechsler tests were used to measure general cognitive ability. The MDI and FSIQ are standardized scores based on the general US population (M = 100, SD = 15). We also included the Wechsler Verbal IQ (VIQ), Performance IQ (PIQ), and Verbal Comprehension (VC), Perceptual Organization (PO), Freedom from Distractibility/Working Memory (FD/WM), and Processing Speed (PS) indices. Impairment was defined as scores that were two or more SDs below the population mean (MDI and Wechsler scores <70). We excluded data from children who were unable to complete the age-appropriate cognitive tests due to cognitive or behavioral limitations, whose raw scores were below test norms, or whose data were considered invalid due to non-English primary language or sensory or motor handicaps. Comparison of children and adolescents who were excluded (N = 62) with those included in the analysis revealed significant differences in the proportion of females (39% female vs. 52%, Fisher's exact test, p =.05) and attendance in special class (64% vs. 35%, Fisher's exact test, p =.01). The mean MDI/FSIQ of the non-English speaking participants (M = 80, SD = 15.1) was lower than that of the English-speaking participants (M = 86, SD = 16.3) (two-sample t-test for means: t = 5.2, p <.0001). However, there were no significant differences between these groups in adherence to the whole medication regimen or to PIs.

Medication Adherence

Medication adherence was assessed with a validated self-report measure (Van Dyke et al., 2002), using a scripted interview with the person who self-identified as responsible for medication administration, whether parent/caregiver or child/adolescent. Respondents identified each medication in the ART regimen and reported the number of doses of each medication missed during each of the preceding 3 days, which may have included weekends and holidays. The adherence report reflected the respondent's ability to administer the medication regimen and/or the willingness and ability of the child/adolescent to take the medicine. Medication burden (the total number of expected doses over the past 3 days) was recorded. The need to prompt respondents for names of ART medications was identified, as were the types of aids utilized as reminders, including pillboxes, buddy system, and activities of daily living. We defined separate indicators of adherence for each drug class: nucleoside reverse transcriptase inhibitors (NRTIs), nonnucleoside reverse transcriptase inhibitors (NNRTIs), and PIs, since PIs are sometimes considered more burdensome than other medication classes (Reddington et al., 2000). Complete adherence was defined as taking all prescribed ART medications during the 3 days preceding the study visit. Adherence was self-assessed by 12% of participants with cognitive impairment, 22% of those in the 70 to <85 range, 25% of those in the 85–100 range, and 27% of those in the >100 range.

Procedures

Data collection for PACTG 219C has been previously described (Williams et al., 2006) and is briefly reviewed. Socio-demographic information, including age, gender, race/ethnicity, primary language, and caregiver characteristics, were collected at study entry, as were detailed medical histories, including clinical diagnoses and medication history. We examined measures of virologic and immunologic status that are assessed every 3 months in PACTG 219C, including HIV-1 RNA viral load and CD4+ T-lymphocyte count and percentage, to account for the complex role played by child and adolescent HIV disease status, which may influence adherence behaviors (Martin et al., 2007; Reddington et al., 2000; Williams et al., 2006). HIV-1 RNA viral load provides a quantitative measurement of HIV viremia in peripheral blood and CD4+ T-lymphocyte percentage provides an indication of immunologic status (higher viral levels and lower CD4 percentage are correlated with increased risk of clinical progression of HIV disease). CDC classification of HIV disease (Center for Disease Control and Prevention, 1994) was also recorded every 3 months at follow-up visits, as was information regarding the current use of ART medications and adherence. Adherence information closest to and within 6 months of the cognitive evaluation was used in the analysis.

Psychosocial information, such as occurrence of stressful life events and presence or absence of physical/social role limitations, was obtained from the General Health Assessment for Children (GHAC), a validated health-related quality-of-life measure, completed annually (Gortmaker et al., 1998). Caregivers reported the occurrence of negative life events during the prior 12 months, including parental job loss, hospitalization of family member, death in the family, and others. Eighteen different stressful life events were categorized into one of three groups involving changes in financial stability, family structure, and family health (Lee, Gortmaker, McIntosh, Hughes, & Oleske, 2006). We identified whether the participant experienced at least one stressor in any of these three categories. Physical/social role functioning was evaluated for participants over 5 years of age. Five different physical/social role functioning events were classified as having occurred or not and included repeating a grade in school, receiving special help in school, participating in sports, or having limitations in physical activity or school attendance.

Data Analytical Strategy

Fisher's Exact tests were used to compare the proportions of children with complete adherence between those with and without cognitive impairment (MDI or FSIQ <70), and Pearson chi-squared tests were used to compare adherence levels across four levels of cognitive impairment (MDI or FSIQ <70, 70–84, 85–100, >100). The Wilk–Shapiro, Kolmogorov–Smirnov, and Anderson–Darling tests were used to assess whether the IQ scores followed a normal distribution. One-sample tests based on Student's t distribution were used to evaluate the deviation of mean IQ scores from the population mean, and one-sample tests based on the standard normal distribution were used to evaluate whether impairment rates (i.e., percent with cognitive impairment) deviated significantly from general population norms. The two-sample t-test was used to compare cognitive scores of children with English as primary language versus those with Spanish or other primary languages.

Demographic characteristics (gender, age, race/ethnicity, and non-English primary language), biological markers of HIV disease (CD4 + count and percent, viral load >400 copies per milliliter, and CDC class C diagnosis), medication and adherence factors (ART and other medication use, duration on HAART, person assessing adherence, medication burden, need for prompting, types of reminders used), child and family psychosocial characteristics (child knowledge of HIV status, primary caregiver, caregiver education level, occurrence of recent stressful life events, and physical/social role functioning limitations), and child psychiatric/neurological profiles (medication and diagnosis history) that have been associated with medication adherence in previous investigations were included in the analyses as covariates (Martin et al., 2007; Mellins et al., 2004; Naar-King et al., 2006; Pontali, 2005; Simoni et al., 2007; Williams et al., 2006). Multiple logistic regression models were used to evaluate associations of adherence with cognitive functioning, adjusting for the identified covariates. Model selection using backward elimination and a 10% level of significance was used to identify significant predictors of nonadherence.

Results

Cognitive Functioning

The children's average performance was within the low-average range on the Bayley-II and Wechsler measures of cognitive ability (Table II), and the mean MDI/FSIQ (M = 85, SD = 16) for the cohort was lower than the general population mean (one-sample t-test for means: t = 35.44, p <.0001). Of the 1,429 children, 232 (16%) were in the <70 IQ range, 502 (35%) were in the 70 to <85 range, 457 (32%) were in the 85–100 range, and 238 (17%) were in the >100 range. The percentage of children with cognitive impairment (16% with MDI or FSIQ <70) was significantly higher than expected (one-sample z-test for proportions: z = 33.17, p <.01), with the highest impairment rates in VIQ and VCI and the lowest rates in PSI.

Table II.

Bayley-II and Wechsler Scores and Prevalence of Cognitive Impairment (N = 1,429)

Test Na FSIQ/MDI VIQ PIQ VC PO FD/WM PS
BSID-II
    Mean (SD) 30 83 (15)
    Percentage with score <70 20
WPPSI-R
    Mean (SD) 188 84 (14) 84 (14) 87 (15)
    Percentage with score <70 13 15 10
WISC-III
    Mean (SD) 1188 85 (17) 84 (16) 88 (17) 85 (16) 88 (17) 86(16) 93(17)
    Percentage with score <70 17 18 13 18 13 16 8
WAIS-III
    Mean (SD) 36 87 (16) 86 (16) 91 (16) 89 (17) 92 (18) 83 (15) 86(13)
    Percentage with score <70 11 14 6 3 6 26 12
Overall
    Mean (SD) 1429 85 (16) 84 (16) 88 (17) 85 (16) 88 (17) 86 (16) 93(17)
    Percentage with score <70 16 17 13 17 13 16 8
Z-statisticb 33.17++ 35.52++ 24.12++ 32.92++ 22.34++ 29.89++ 8.98++
T-statisticc 36.07++ 37.22++ 27.70++ 31.67++ 23.77++ 29.75++ 11.02++

Cognitive impairment defined as standard score <70.

aMaximum number with any single test score.

bThis corresponds to the upper-tailed one-sample z-test for the true proportion of impairment in this population (p-value <.05 implies impairment rate is significantly higher than the general population rate of 2.5%).

cThis corresponds to the lower-tailed t-test for the true mean score (p-value <.05 implies mean score is significantly smaller than the general population mean of 100).

++p < 0.01, one-tailed.

Cognitive Functioning and Adherence

We found no significant association between levels of cognitive functioning (MDI or FSIQ <70, 70–84, 85–100, or >100) and adherence to the entire medication regimen (Pearson's chi-squared test: χ2 = 5.85, df = 3, p =.12). Further, there were no significant associations between cognitive status and adherence to the NRTI drug class (Pearson's χ2 = 4.37, df = 3, p =.22) and the NNRTI drug class (Pearson's χ2 = 4.31, df = 3, p =.23) (Table III).

Table III.

Percent of Participants with Complete (100%) Adherence by Drug Class and Levels of Cognitive Functioning

Mental development index/Full scale IQ level
Drug class <70 70–<85 85–100 >100 Overall
Entire regimen 205 (88) 413 (82) 391 (86) 207 (87) 1,216 (85)
(n = 232) (n = 502) (n = 457) (n = 238) (n = 1429)
NRTIs 207(89) 419 (84) 396 (87) 207 (88) 1,229 (87)
(n = 232) (n = 498) (n = 454) (n = 236) (n = 1,420)
NNRTIs 70 (95) 163 (88) 145(89) 68 (94) 446 (90)
(n = 74) (n = 185) (n = 163) (n = 72) (n = 494)
PIsa 166 (91) 305 (83) 297 (89) 150 (88) 918 (87)
(n = 183) (n = 368) (n = 333) (n = 171) (n = 1,055)

aPearson's chi-squared test (χ2 = 9.25, df = 3, p =.03).

There was a significant association between cognitive status and adherence to the drug class of PIs (Pearson's chi-squared test: χ2 = 9.25, df = 3, p =.03). Children who function within the borderline/low-average range of cognitive ability (MDI/FSIQ = 70–84) were less adherent to PI therapy compared to those below and above that range (83% vs. 91% adherent, Fisher's exact test, p =.01; and 83% vs. 89%, p =.02, respectively).

Final Models of Predictors of Nonadherence

Entire Regimen

In the final multiple logistic regression model, cognitive functioning, as measured by MDI and FSIQ, was not predictive of nonadherence to the entire medication regimen (Table IV). Factors significantly associated with higher odds of nonadherence included occurrence of a recent life stressor [odds ratio (OR): 1.74, p =.001) and HIV RNA detectability (>400 copies/ml) (OR: 1.58, p =.01). Factors associated with higher odds of complete adherence included: more years on HAART (OR: 0.86, p =.001), higher caregiver education (OR: 0.83, p =.02), having a nonrelative (OR: 0.60, p =.04) or relative (OR: 0.56, p =.02) adult responsible for medication, and repeating a grade in school (OR: 0.63, p =.02).

Table IV.

Final Multiple Logistic Regression Model for Predictors of Nonadherence to Entire ART Regimen (n = 1,429)a

Predictor Estimated OR 95% confidence interval p-value
Cognitive functioning .18
    Impaired: (MDI/FSIQ: <70) 1.00 (ref)
    MDI/FSIQ: 70–<85 1.42 0.84–2.40 .19
    MDI/FSIQ: 85–100 1.10 0.63–1.90 .74
    MDI/FSIQ: >100 0.86 0.45–1.62 .63
Age group (years) .85
    3–<6 1.00 (ref)
    6–12 1.26 0.57–2.79 .57
    >12–18 1.29 0.52–3.17 .58
Adherence assessor .005
    Biological parent 1.00 (ref)
    Self or self plus other 1.28 0.79–2.07 .32
    Relative 0.56 0.34–0.91 .02
    Other adult 0.60 0.37–0.99 .04
Recent life stress 1.74 1.25–2.44 .001
HIV–1 RNA >400 copies/ml 1.58 1.11–2.24 .01
Reminder: Pillbox 1.47 0.97–2.24 .07
Child knows HIV status 1.40 0.92–2.14 .12
Memory prompt required 1.36 0.96–1.93 .09
Participation in sports 1.35 0.95–1.90 .09
Duration of HAARTb 0.86 0.78–0.94 .001
Caregiver education level 0.83 0.72–0.97 .02
Reminder: daily activities 0.70 0.47–1.04 .08
Repeated a grade 0.63 0.43–0.94 .02
Reminder: buddy system 0.52 0.25–1.06 .07

Higher values for estimated OR indicate higher risk of nonadherence.

aA total of 178 observations were deleted due to missing values for the response or the explanatory variables.

bCorresponds to every additional year on HAART.

PIs Alone

After adjusting for hypothesized covariates, we found a marginal association between cognitive functioning and nonadherence to the individual drug class of PIs (p =.06) (Table V). Children in the borderline to low-average range of cognitive functioning had marginally higher odds of nonadherence to PIs than those with cognitive impairment (OR: 1.94, p =.05). Additional factors associated with higher odds of nonadherence to PIs included: HIV RNA detectability (OR: 2.07, p =.001), limitations in daily activities (OR: 1.98, p =.02), recent life stress (OR: 1.72, p =.01), and higher medication burden (OR: 1.25, p =.01). Factors associated with higher odds of complete adherence to PIs included: more years on HAART (OR: 0.82, p =.003), using activities of daily living as an adherence support (OR: 0.60, p =.04), and having an adult relative responsible for medication (OR: 0.52, p =.04).

Table V.

Final Multiple Logistic Regression Model for Predictors of Non-adherence to Protease Inhibitors (N = 1,055a Participants on PIs)

Predictor Estimated odds ratio 95% confidence interval p-value
Cognitive functioning .06
    Impaired (MDI/FSIQ: <70) 1.00 (ref)
    MDI/FSIQ: 70–<85 1.94 0.99–3.79 .05
    MDI/FSIQ: 85–100 1.07 0.53–2.19 .85
    MDI/FSIQ: >100 1.23 0.56–2.69 .61
Age group (years) .14
    3–<6 1.00 (ref)
    6–12 2.69 0.79–9.23 .12
    >12–18 3.61 0.99–13.19 .05
Adherence assessor .04
    Biological parent 1.00 (ref)
    Self or self + other 1.26 0.71–2.23 .43
    Relative 0.52 0.28–0.98 .04
    Other adult 0.62 0.34–1.15 .13
HIV-1 RNA >400 copies/ml 2.07 1.34–3.22 .001
Activities limited 1.98 1.11–3.55 .02
Recent life stress 1.72 1.12–2.64 .01
Participated in sports 1.51 0.97–2.35 .07
Medication burdenb 1.25 1.05–1.48 .01
Duration of HAARTc 0.82 0.71–0.93 .003
Reminder: activities of daily living 0.60 0.37–0.98 .04
CD4 count <200 0.40 0.16–1.02 .06

Higher values for estimated odds ratio indicate higher risk of nonadherence.

a157 observations were deleted due to missing values for the response or the explanatory variables.

bCorresponds to every additional drug dose per day.

cCorresponds to every additional year on HAART.

Effect of Age

We included age, grouped into three categories (3 to <6 years, 6–12 years, >12–18 years), in the final models; however, results were not significant. Moreover, the parameter estimates corresponding to the three cognitive levels in the models remained essentially unchanged (<10% change) when age group was included in the models. Age group was not significantly associated with nonadherence to the entire medication regimen or with nonadherence to PIs. In a supportive analysis, with age excluded in the final models, results regarding the association of cognitive functioning with nonadherence to PIs were slightly stronger (p =.05).

Discussion

The cognitive functioning of children in this investigation is consistent with recent studies of children with HIV infection (Jeremy et al. 2005; Mialky, Vagnoni, & Rutstein, 2001; Nozyce et al., 2006). The mean score of the children in this cohort was within the low-average range of cognitive functioning, and their performance in most areas, including verbal, memory, and perceptual organization, was below expectations for the general population. Sixteen percent of children in this cohort were cognitively impaired (MDI or FSIQ <70). This finding is consistent with recent studies that describe a decrement in rates of progressive HIV encephalopathy but not elimination of significant neurocognitive impairment among children with HIV during the HAART era (Chiriboga, Fleishman, Champion, Gaye-Robinson, & Abrams, 2005).

Contrary to our hypothesis, the cognitive status of children and adolescents in this study, whether impaired or not, was not significantly associated with adherence to their full medication regimen. In fact, high rates of complete adherence to medication regimens were consistently observed among children with cognitive impairment, in contrast to the adult experience (Hinkin et al., 2004). This finding suggests the presence of adequate caregiver vigilance and active participation in the process of adherence with these children and is supported by the limited number of children with cognitive impairment who assume primary responsibility for their medication. Caregivers of children with cognitive impairment may recognize that their children are less knowledgeable about their medication regimen and have limited ability to understand the immediate and long-term consequences of nonadherence. As these children progress through adolescence and transition into adult health care programs, sensitive support and surveillance of adherence will be critical for the well-being of the individual as well as for the protection of public health.

Adherence to PI therapy was of special interest in this investigation due to its unique challenges among ART drug classes. Earlier longitudinal studies indicated that PI therapy is initiated sooner among children with more advanced disease, is difficult to administer or take due to large pill size or poor palatability of liquid formulations, and may have complex acute and long-term side-effects (Gortmaker et al., 2001; Storm et al., 2005). In addition, some PIs have inadequate penetration to the central nervous system and may be limited in their impact on the presence and severity of neurocognitive involvement, despite effective suppression of systemic HIV infection (Ferrando, Rabkin, van Gorp, Lin, & McElhiney, 2003). Among those children with PI-containing medication regimens in this investigation, child cognitive status conferred modest additional risk for nonadherence to PIs. Children with cognitive scores in the borderline to low-average range were marginally less successful adhering to PIs than those with cognitive impairment and those whose scores were at or above age level. Their cognitive and learning difficulties may be less appreciated by caregivers than the problems of children with greater neurocognitive impairment, and as a result, the supports necessary for adherence to difficult medications may be inadequate. It is also possible that these children were entrusted to manage their own adherence before they achieved readiness for this level of independence. The observed trend of lower rates of adherence among children with borderline/low average cognitive status across all drug classes attests to the clinical importance of ongoing adherence supervision for these children. Further research is necessary to better articulate the specific deficits that contribute to higher risk for nonadherence among these children and adolescents, particularly with respect to PIs.

Our investigation highlights the important role of the caregiving environment for medication adherence, given children's dependence on their caregivers for health care management, and is consistent with earlier investigations. Recent life stressors have been associated with poorer psychological outcomes (Moss, Bose, Wolters, & Brouwers, 1998) and with nonadherence in earlier studies (Mellins et al., 2004; Williams et al., 2006) and have been related to cognitive impairment among HIV positive adults (Pukay-Martin, Cristiani, Saveanu, & Bornstein, 2003). In this study, recent life stressors, which were experienced by more than half (52%) of the participants, were associated with higher odds of nonadherence, suggesting that adherent behavior is sensitive to variations in child and family well-being. Families affected by HIV are unfortunately often vulnerable to both sudden and chronic stress due to the impact of HIV as a multigenerational, stigmatized illness, and due to shared characteristics of families with HIV, such as minority group membership, poverty, discrimination, inadequate housing, and exposure to crime and violence (Brown, Lourie, & Pao, 2000).

Several caregiving factors, including higher caregiver education and the nature of the child–caregiver relationship, appeared to be protective and reduced the risk for medication nonadherence in children with HIV infection. Children whose medications were administered by caregivers such as relatives (e.g., aunts, grandparents) or nonrelative adults (e.g., foster/adoptive parents) demonstrated lower risk for nonadherence. These caregivers may have more personal resources and be less burdened by the psychological and physical health complications often experienced by biological parents with HIV disease. Parents with HIV disease may require increased social support to ensure medication success for their children, particularly if their current or chronic life stress is high (Bauman, Silver, Draimin, & Hudis, 2007). Of interest, the regular use of specific aids for adherence, such as linking medication-taking to daily activities and using a buddy system were marginally associated with complete adherence, in keeping with studies showing the benefits of daily routines and social support for medication adherence in chronic illness (Greening, Stoppelbein, Konishi, Jordan, & Moll, 2007; Lyon et al., 2003). In contrast, the need for a memory prompt to recall medication names and the use of a pillbox, which organizes medications and provides a visual memory cue, were marginally associated with nonadherence. It is possible that pillboxes are initiated in the presence of adherence failure or in the context of declining caregiver supervision and increased child participation in the task of taking medicine on a daily basis.

Several child health and developmental factors, as well as specific medication factors, were associated with medication adherence. In our investigation, children with longer time on complex treatment regimens and those with advanced HIV disease were more likely to be adherent. These findings are consistent with studies of adults and children with HIV infection who were more adherent as HIV disease progressed or symptoms developed (Eldred, Wu, Chaisson, & Moor, 1998; Giacomet et al., 2003; Gibb et al., 2003). Caregivers, children, and adolescents may become or remain more vigilant about medication-taking behaviors when children's health becomes more precarious. In contrast, recent inadequate virologic control, as indicated by detectable viral load, as well as high medication burden and health-related limitations in children's daily activities were associated with higher risk for nonadherence, and reflect the current impact of HIV disease and its treatment upon health behaviors. It is important to note, however, that detectable viral load may be observed in the context of perfect adherence, especially among heavily treated children and adolescents who sometimes experience suboptimal suppression of viral replication and antiretroviral drug resistance.

Earlier studies and clinical observation suggest a relationship between older age and nonadherence to medication among children and adolescents with HIV (Mellins et al., 2004; Williams et al., 2006). However, our results did not support such a relationship, perhaps because the variance associated with age was accounted for by other, more explanatory covariates, such as occurrence of stressful life events that may increase in frequency as children age, and adherence assessor. When modeling for nonadherence to PIs, for example, age of child was significant when adherence assessor was removed from the model but was nonsignificant when adherence assessor was retained in the model. Adherence assessor, on the other hand, remained significant regardless of whether age of child was in the model or not, suggesting that adherence assessor was the more important covariate in the model.

This study was limited by the use of a cross-sectional design to explore dynamic developmental processes. Participants in P219C who completed valid cognitive evaluations may represent an inherently more adherent subgroup of children and adolescents with HIV; thus, our results may not adequately reflect the medication adherence of all children and adolescents with HIV due to selection bias in P219C. We were unable to compare the cognitive functioning of our cohort with a demographically matched sample of children. Additional measures of child adaptive functioning, memory, and executive functioning as well as information regarding the physical and psychological well-being of caregivers were not available in P219C but would contribute information relevant to families’ health maintenance behaviors. Overall, medication adherence rates of children and adolescents in this study (85%) were high relative to earlier investigations, which measured adherence over longer time periods of 1 week to 1 month (Mellins et al., 2004; Reddington et al., 2000). Our use of a single, 3-day self-report measure of adherence may underestimate the true prevalence of nonadherence, possibly due to social desirability, improved adherence immediately prior to a medical visit, limitations in the integrity of children's and parents’ memories, and the restricted time frame for reporting. However, Williams et al. (2006) provided support for the validity of our adherence measure, identifying a clear trend for improved adherence with declining levels of viral load.

This investigation has important strengths. To date, this is the largest sample of children with HIV infection whose adherence and cognitive status were examined concurrently at different ages. Our large sample size allowed us to identify risk and protective factors for medication adherence among children with HIV. It is clear that multiple factors influence the adherence process, including individual child characteristics, health and medication factors, and the social context within which adherence occurs.

Our findings can assist families and health care providers to target preventive, multilevel interventions to support children and families at highest risk for nonadherence, particularly when medication regimens are initiated. Developmentally appropriate education, deliberate practice of emerging and complex self-care routines, and ongoing monitoring of adherence are essential for all children, regardless of cognitive status, especially during early adolescence when caregivers and teens anticipate greater independence. Children and families who experience stressful life events, even if short-lived, may require additional psychosocial support and monitoring to enhance adherence. Stress management and problem-focused coping skills may be particularly useful for children and caregivers and could be facilitated in a primary care setting in which culturally sensitive mental health care and case management are available. Prospective longitudinal studies are needed to further delineate children's adherence needs as they develop and to evaluate the efficacy of targeted strategies for adherence support.

Acknowledgments

We thank the children and their families, the Study Team and the individuals and institutions involved in the conduct of PACTG 219C for contributing to this research. This study was funded by the United States National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases. This work was supported by the Statistical and Data Analysis Center (SDAC) of the Pediatric AIDS Clinical Trials group at the Harvard School of Public Health under the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases cooperative agreement No. 5U01 A141110. The following institutions were involved in the design, data collection, and conduct of PACTG 219C, but were not involved in the present analysis, the interpretation of the data, the writing of the article, or the decision to submit for publication.

Conflicts of interest: The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases or the National Institute of Health.

Appendix

The members of the PACTG 219C Team are as follows. Baylor Texas Children's Hospital: M. Turcich, L. Harris, I. Iovino; University of Florida, Jacksonville: Ann Usitalo, PhD, Mobeen H. Rathore, MD, Ayesha Mirza, MD, Saniyyah Mahmoudi, LSN, ARNP; Chicago Children's Memorial Hospital: K. Malee; University of Puerto Rico, University Children's Hospital AIDS Program: C. Cruz, Nydia Scalley; Bronx Lebanon Hospital Center: A. Khadivi; San Juan Hospital: D. Miranda; University of Miami: GB Scott, CD Mitchell, L Taybo, S Willumsen; University of Medicine & Dentistry of New Jersey: Susan Adubato, PhD, Jennis Hanna, PhD; Charity Hospital of New Orleans & Earl K. Long Early Intervention Clinic: P. Sirois; UCSD Mother, Child & Adolescent HIV Program: S. Nichols; Howard University: S. Wilson; Jacobi Medical Center: R. Shaw; St. Christopher's Hospital for Children, Philadelphia: Mitzie L. Grant, PhD, Janet S. Chen, MD, Jill A. Foster, MD; Baystate Medical Center Children's Hospital: Susan McQuiston, PhD; Los Angeles County Medical Center/USC: J Homans, M Neely, LS Spencer, A Kovacs; Children's Hospital Boston: B. Kammerer; Children's Hospital of Michigan: J. Meade, R. Rothermel, L. Schuster, C. Yumoto; St. Jude Children's Research Hospital, Memphis: M. Miles, T. Cooper, Patricia Garvie, PhD; New York University School of Medicine/Bellevue Hospital: W Borkowsky, S Chandwani, N Deygoo, S Akleh; The Children's Hospital at Downstate: S. Bewley; The Columbia Presbyterian Medical Center & Cornell University New York Presbyterian Hospital:B. Rohwedder, C. Mellins; The Children's Hospital of Philadelphia: N. Silverman; Children's Hospital of Oakland: Rita Jeremy, PhD; UCSF, Moffitt Hospital: Rita Jeremy, PhD; Children's Hospital, University of Colorado, Denver: R. McEvoy; Johns Hopkins University Pediatrics: N Hutton, B Griffith, M Joyner, C Kiefner; Children's Hospital and Regional Medical Center, Washington: M Acker, R Croteau, C McLellan, K Mohan, David Breiger, PhD; Metropolitan Hospital Center: M. Bamji, I. Pathak, S. Manwani, E. Patel, Evelyn Abreu; Children's National Medical Center: P. Glass, M. New, D. Marshall, M. Lyon, S. Lee; University of Massachusetts Medical School: Margaret Bowden; University of Alabama at Birmingham: J. Ackerson, D. Marullo; University of Maryland Medical Center: V. Teppler; Schneider Children's Hospital: VR Bonagura, SJ Schuval, C Colter, L Campbell, Kunsang Gyato, PhD; Boston Medical Center: Cynthia Chase, PhD; University of Illinois: Renee Smith, PhD; SUNY Stony Brook: J. Schneider, M. Milazzo; North Broward Hospital District: A. Puga, Barbara Kalish, PhD; Duke University: Karen O’Donnell, Felicia Wiley, Kareema Whitfield, Maragaret Donnelly; Harlem Hospital: S Champion, M Frere, M DiGrado, EJ Abrams; Cook County Hospital: J. Martinez; University of South Alabama: M Mancao; Connecticut Children's Medical Center: Gary Isenberg, PhD; University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill: J. McKeeman; Ruiz Arnau University Hospital: W. Figueroa, E. Reyes; SUNY Upstate Medical University: LB Weiner, KA Contello, WA Holz, MJ Famiglietti; Children's Medical Center of Dallas; University of Florida at Gainesville:L. Schiavoni; Children's Hospital at Albany Medical Center: AD Fernandez, PA Hughes, N Wade, ME Adams; Lincoln Medical & Mental Health Center: R. Shaw; Phoenix Children's Hospital: JP Piatt, J Foti, L Clarke-Steffen; Public Health Unit of Palm Beach County: J. Sleasman, C. Delaney; Medical College of Georgia: M. Cohen, B. Kiernan; Yale University School of Medicine: WA Andiman, S Romano, L Hurst, J de Jesus, Michael Westerveld, PhD; Vanderbilt University Medical Center: G Wilson, Evon B. Lee, PhD; University of Rochester Medical Center: Heather Adams, PhD, Geoffrey A. Weinberg, MD, Barbra Murante, MS, RN, PNP, Susan Laverty, RN; St Josephs Hospital and Medical Center, New Jersey: N. Hutchcon, A. Townley; Emory University Hospital: S. Nesheim, R. Dennis; University of South Florida: D. Lindsey-Blue, T. Chenneville; Children's Hospital of the King's Daughters: V. Vandewater; Medical University of South Carolina: GM Johnson; University of Mississippi Medical Center: H. Gay, S. Sadler; Harbor-UCLA Medical Center:T. Mastandrea, Karen Hickel-Rathburn, PhD; Mount Sinai Medical Center: D. Johnson; Children's Hospital of Los Angeles: J. Church, T. Dunaway, C. Salata; Long Beach Memorial: A. Deveikis, L. Melton; Robert Wood Johnson Medical School: S Gaur, P Whitley-Williams, A Malhotra, L Cerracchio; Sinai Children's Hospital: M Dolan, J D’Agostino, R Posada; The Medical Center, Pediatric Columbus, Georgia: C. Mani, S. Cobb; Medical College of Virginia: SR Lavoie, TY Smith; Cooper Hospital, University Medical Center: A. Feingold, S. Burrows-Clark; University of Cincinnati: J. Mrus, R. Beiting; Columbus Children's Hospital: J. Cass, K. Murdock, Keith Yates, PhD; Sacred Heart Children's CMS of Florida: W. Albritton; St. Luke's/Roosevelt Hospital Center: R Warford, S Arpadi; Incarnation Children's Center, New York: A. Gershon, P. Miller; Montefiore Medical – AECOM: A. Rubinstein, G. Krienik; Children's Hospital of Los Angeles: M. Nguyen, M. Chau, T. Waxman, C. Forbes; San Francisco General Hospital: D. Wara, A. Kamrin, S. Farrales; Cornell University New York Presbyterian: R. Johan-Liang, K. O'Keefe; St. Louis Children's Hospital:K. Sirl, M. Cradock; North Shore University Hospital: S. Pahwa, L. Rodriquez; Oregon Health and Science University: S. Cagle, K. Pettit-Kekel; Tulane/LSU: M. Silio.

References

  1. Bauman LJ, Silver EJ, Draimin BH, Hudis J. Children of mothers with HIV/AIDS: Unmet needs for mental health services. Pediatrics. 2007;120:e1141–e1147. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-2680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  3. Brown LK, Lourie KJ, Pao M. Children and adolescents living with HIV and AIDS: A review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2000;41:81–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Center for Disease Control and Prevention. 1994 revised classification system for human immunodeficiency virus infection in children less than 13 years of age. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 1994;43(RR-12):1–10. [Google Scholar]
  5. Chiriboga CA, Fleishman S, Champion S, Gaye-Robinson L, Abrams EJ. Incidence and prevalence of HIV encephalopathy in children with HIV infection receiving highly active anti-retroviral therapy (HAART) Journal of Pediatrics. 2005;146:402–407. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2004.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. DeMartino M, Tovo PA, Balduicci M, Galli L, Gaviano C, Rezza G, et al. for the Italian Register for HIV Infection in Children and the Italian National AIDS Registry. Reduction in mortality with availability of antiretroviral therapy for children with perinatal HIV-1 infection. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2000;284:190–197. doi: 10.1001/jama.284.2.190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Eldred L, Wu A, Chaisson R, Moore R. Adherence to antiretroviral and pneumocystis prophylaxis in HIV disease. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome Human Retrovirology. 1998;18:117–125. doi: 10.1097/00042560-199806010-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Ferrando SJ, Rabkin JG, van Gorp W, Lin S, McElhiney M. Longitudinal improvement in psychomotor processing speed is associated with potent combination antiretroviral therapy in HIV-1 infection. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2003;15:208–214. doi: 10.1176/jnp.15.2.208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Giacomet V, Albano F, Starace F, de Franciscis A, Giaquinto C, Castelli Gattinara G, et al. Adherence to antiretroviral therapy and its determinants in children with human immunodeficiency virus infection: A multicentre, national study. Acta Paediatrica. 2003;92:1398–1402. doi: 10.1080/08035250310006737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gibb DM, Goodall RL, Giacomet V, McGhee L, Compagnucci A, Lyall H. Adherence to prescribed antiretroviral therapy in human immunodeficiency virus-infected children in the PENTA 5 trial. The Pediatric Infectious Diseases Journal. 2003;22:56–62. doi: 10.1097/00006454-200301000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gortmaker SL, Hughes M, Cervia J, Brady M, Johnson G, Seage G, et al. for the Pediatric AIDS Clinical Trials Group Protocol 219 Team. Effect of combination therapy including protease inhibitors on mortality among children and adolescents infected with HIV-1. New England Journal of Medicine. 2001;345:1522–1528. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa011157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gortmaker SL, Lenderking WR, Clark C, Lee S, Fowler MG, Oleske J. Development and use of a pediatric quality of life questionnaire in AIDS clinical trials: Reliability and validity of the General Health Assessment for Children (GHAC) In: Drotar D, editor. Assessing pediatric health-related quality of life and functional status: Implications for research, practice and policy. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Feldbaum, Associates; 1998. pp. 219–235. [Google Scholar]
  13. Greening L, Stoppelbein L, Konishi C, Jordan SS, Moll G. Child routines and youths’ adherence to treatment for type 1 diabetes. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2007;32:437–447. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsl029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hammani N, Nostlinger C, Hoeree T, Lefevre P, Jonckheer T, Kolsteren P. Integrating adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy into children's daily lives: A qualitative study. Pediatrics. 2004;114:591–597. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hinkin CH, Hardy DJ, Mason KI, Castellon SA, Durvasula RS, Lam MN, et al. Medication adherence in HIV-infected adults: Effect of patient age, cognitive status, and substance abuse. AIDS. 2004;18(Suppl 1):S19–S25. doi: 10.1097/00002030-200418001-00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Jeremy R, Kim S, Nozyce M, Nachman S, McIntosh K, Pelton S, et al. and the PACTG 338 and 377 study groups. Neuropsychological functioning and viral load in stable antiretroviral therapy-experienced HIV-infected children. Pediatrics. 2005;115:380–387. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Lee GM, Gortmaker SL, McIntosh K, Hughes MD, Oleske JM. Quality of life for children and adolescents: Impact of HIV infection and antiretroviral treatment. Pediatrics. 2006;117:273–283. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Lyon M, Trexler C, Akpan-Townsend C, Pao M, Selden K, Fletcher J, et al. A family group approach to increasing adherence to therapy in HIV infected youth: Results of a pilot project. AIDS Patient Care STDs. 2003;17:299–308. doi: 10.1089/108729103322108175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Martin S, Elliott-DeSorbo DK, Wolters PL, Toledo-Tamula MA, Roby G, Zeichner S, et al. Patient, caregiver and regimen characteristics associated with adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy among HIV-infected children and adolescents. The Pediatric Infectious Diseases Journal. 2007;26:61–67. doi: 10.1097/01.inf.0000250625.80340.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Mellins C, Brackis-Cott E, Dolezal C, Abrams E. The role of psychosocial and family factors in adherence to antiretroviral treatment in human immunodeficiency virus-infected children. The Pediatric Infectious Disease Journal. 2004;23:1035–1041. doi: 10.1097/01.inf.0000143646.15240.ac. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Mialky E, Vagnoni J, Rutstein R. School-age children with perinatally acquired HIV infection: Medical and psychosocial issues in a Philadelphia cohort. AIDS Patient Care and STDs. 2001;15:575–579. doi: 10.1089/108729101753287667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Moss H, Bose S, Wolters P, Brouwers P. A preliminary study of factors associated with psychological adjustment and disease course in school-age children infected with the human immunodeficiency virus. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 1998;19:18–25. doi: 10.1097/00004703-199802000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Naar-King S, Arfken C, Frey M, Harris M, Secord E, Ellis D. Psychosocial factors and treatment adherence in paediatric HIV/AIDS. AIDS Care. 2006;18:621–628. doi: 10.1080/09540120500471895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Nozyce M, Lee S, Wiznia A, Nachman S, Mofenson L, Smith M, et al. A behavioral and cognitive profile of clinically stable HIV infected children. Pediatrics. 2006;117:763–770. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Pontali E. Facilitating adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy in children with HIV infection: What are the issues and what can be done? Paediatric Drugs. 2005;7:137–149. doi: 10.2165/00148581-200507030-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Pukay-Martin N, Cristiani S, Saveanu R, Bornstein R. The relationship between stressful life events and cognitive functioning in HIV-infected men. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2003;15:436–441. doi: 10.1176/jnp.15.4.436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Reddington C, Cohen JM, Baldillo A, Toye M, Smith D, Kneut C, et al. Adherence to medication regimens among children with human immunodeficiency virus infection. The Pediatric Infectious Disease Journal. 2000;19:1148–1153. doi: 10.1097/00006454-200012000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Simoni JM, Montgomery A, Martin E, New M, Demas PA, Rana S. Adherence to antiretroviral therapy for pediatric HIV infection: A qualitative systematic review with recommendations for research and clinical management. Pediatrics. 2007;119:e1371–e1384. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Steele RG, Nelson TD, Cole BP. Psychosocial functioning in children with AIDS and HIV infection: Review of the literature from a socioecological framework. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2007;28:58–69. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31803084c6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Storm DS, Boland MG, Gortmaker SL, He Y, Skurnick J, Howland L, et al. Protease inhibitor combination therapy, severity of illness, and quality of life among children with perinatally acquired HIV-1 infection. Pediatrics. 2005;115:e173–e182. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Van Dyke RB, Lee S, Johnson GM, Wiznia W, Mohan K, Stanley K, et al. Reported adherence as a determinant of response to highly active antiretroviral therapy in children who have human immunodeficiency virus infection. Pediatrics. 2002;109:e61. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.4.e61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Van Rie A, Harrington PR, Dow A, Robertson K. Neurologic and neurodevelopmental manifestations of pediatric HIV/AIDS: A global perspective. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology. 2007;11:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2006.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Wechsler D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence-Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  34. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children. 3rd. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  35. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3rd. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  36. Williams P, Storm D, Montepiedra G, Nichols S, Kammerer B, Sirois P, et al. Predictors of adherence to antiretroviral medications in children and adolescents with HIV infection. Pediatrics. 2006;118:e1745–e1757. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Pediatric Psychology are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES